Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 83
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cell ; 183(4): 954-967.e21, 2020 11 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33058757

RESUMO

The curse of dimensionality plagues models of reinforcement learning and decision making. The process of abstraction solves this by constructing variables describing features shared by different instances, reducing dimensionality and enabling generalization in novel situations. Here, we characterized neural representations in monkeys performing a task described by different hidden and explicit variables. Abstraction was defined operationally using the generalization performance of neural decoders across task conditions not used for training, which requires a particular geometry of neural representations. Neural ensembles in prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and simulated neural networks simultaneously represented multiple variables in a geometry reflecting abstraction but that still allowed a linear classifier to decode a large number of other variables (high shattering dimensionality). Furthermore, this geometry changed in relation to task events and performance. These findings elucidate how the brain and artificial systems represent variables in an abstract format while preserving the advantages conferred by high shattering dimensionality.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/anatomia & histologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/anatomia & histologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Mapeamento Encefálico , Simulação por Computador , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Redes Neurais de Computação , Neurônios/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Reforço Psicológico , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas
2.
Nature ; 629(8013): 861-868, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38750353

RESUMO

A central assumption of neuroscience is that long-term memories are represented by the same brain areas that encode sensory stimuli1. Neurons in inferotemporal (IT) cortex represent the sensory percept of visual objects using a distributed axis code2-4. Whether and how the same IT neural population represents the long-term memory of visual objects remains unclear. Here we examined how familiar faces are encoded in the IT anterior medial face patch (AM), perirhinal face patch (PR) and temporal pole face patch (TP). In AM and PR we observed that the encoding axis for familiar faces is rotated relative to that for unfamiliar faces at long latency; in TP this memory-related rotation was much weaker. Contrary to previous claims, the relative response magnitude to familiar versus unfamiliar faces was not a stable indicator of familiarity in any patch5-11. The mechanism underlying the memory-related axis change is likely intrinsic to IT cortex, because inactivation of PR did not affect axis change dynamics in AM. Overall, our results suggest that memories of familiar faces are represented in AM and perirhinal cortex by a distinct long-latency code, explaining how the same cell population can encode both the percept and memory of faces.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Facial , Memória de Longo Prazo , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Lobo Temporal , Animais , Face , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Neurônios/fisiologia , Córtex Perirrinal/fisiologia , Córtex Perirrinal/citologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/anatomia & histologia , Lobo Temporal/citologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Rotação
3.
Nature ; 632(8026): 841-849, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39143207

RESUMO

Humans have the remarkable cognitive capacity to rapidly adapt to changing environments. Central to this capacity is the ability to form high-level, abstract representations that take advantage of regularities in the world to support generalization1. However, little is known about how these representations are encoded in populations of neurons, how they emerge through learning and how they relate to behaviour2,3. Here we characterized the representational geometry of populations of neurons (single units) recorded in the hippocampus, amygdala, medial frontal cortex and ventral temporal cortex of neurosurgical patients performing an inferential reasoning task. We found that only the neural representations formed in the hippocampus simultaneously encode several task variables in an abstract, or disentangled, format. This representational geometry is uniquely observed after patients learn to perform inference, and consists of disentangled directly observable and discovered latent task variables. Learning to perform inference by trial and error or through verbal instructions led to the formation of hippocampal representations with similar geometric properties. The observed relation between representational format and inference behaviour suggests that abstract and disentangled representational geometries are important for complex cognition.


Assuntos
Cognição , Hipocampo , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Tonsila do Cerebelo/citologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Lobo Frontal/citologia , Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Hipocampo/fisiologia , Hipocampo/citologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Neurocirurgia , Lobo Temporal/fisiologia , Lobo Temporal/citologia , Adulto Jovem
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(51)2021 12 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34916282

RESUMO

The observation of place cells has suggested that the hippocampus plays a special role in encoding spatial information. However, place cell responses are modulated by several nonspatial variables and reported to be rather unstable. Here, we propose a memory model of the hippocampus that provides an interpretation of place cells consistent with these observations. We hypothesize that the hippocampus is a memory device that takes advantage of the correlations between sensory experiences to generate compressed representations of the episodes that are stored in memory. A simple neural network model that can efficiently compress information naturally produces place cells that are similar to those observed in experiments. It predicts that the activity of these cells is variable and that the fluctuations of the place fields encode information about the recent history of sensory experiences. Place cells may simply be a consequence of a memory compression process implemented in the hippocampus.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Memória Espacial/fisiologia , Animais , Hipocampo/citologia , Rede Nervosa , Navegação Espacial/fisiologia
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(37): 23021-23032, 2020 09 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32859756

RESUMO

Our decisions often depend on multiple sensory experiences separated by time delays. The brain can remember these experiences and, simultaneously, estimate the timing between events. To understand the mechanisms underlying working memory and time encoding, we analyze neural activity recorded during delays in four experiments on nonhuman primates. To disambiguate potential mechanisms, we propose two analyses, namely, decoding the passage of time from neural data and computing the cumulative dimensionality of the neural trajectory over time. Time can be decoded with high precision in tasks where timing information is relevant and with lower precision when irrelevant for performing the task. Neural trajectories are always observed to be low-dimensional. In addition, our results further constrain the mechanisms underlying time encoding as we find that the linear "ramping" component of each neuron's firing rate strongly contributes to the slow timescale variations that make decoding time possible. These constraints rule out working memory models that rely on constant, sustained activity and neural networks with high-dimensional trajectories, like reservoir networks. Instead, recurrent networks trained with backpropagation capture the time-encoding properties and the dimensionality observed in the data.


Assuntos
Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Animais , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico/métodos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Redes Neurais de Computação , Neurônios/fisiologia , Primatas
6.
Ann Surg Oncol ; 28(4): 2169-2179, 2021 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32974699

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Timing of autologous reconstruction relative to postmastectomy radiation therapy (PMRT) is debated. Benefits of immediate reconstruction must be weighed against a possibly heightened risk of complications from flap irradiation. We reviewed flap outcomes after single operation plus PMRT in a large institutional cohort. METHODS: Medical records were reviewed for women who underwent simultaneous mastectomy-autologous reconstruction with PMRT from 2007 to 2016. Primary endpoints were rates and types of radiation-related flap complications and reoperations, whose predictors were assessed by multivariable analysis. A p value < 0.10 was deemed significant to avoid type II error. Non-parametric logistic regression generated a model of PMRT timing associated with probabilities of complications and reoperations. RESULTS: One-hundred and thirty women underwent 208 mastectomy reconstruction operations, with a median follow up of 35.1 months (interquartile range 23.6-56.5). Forty-seven (36.2%) women experienced radiation-related complications, commonly fat necrosis (44.1%) and chest wall asymmetry (28.8%). Complications were higher among women who received PMRT < 3 months after surgery (46.8% for < 3 months vs. 29.3% for ≥ 3 months; p = 0.06), most of whom received neoadjuvant chemotherapy, and among women treated with internal mammary nodal (IMN) radiation (65.2% vs. 26.4%; p < 0.01); IMN radiation remained strongly associated in multivariable analysis (odds ratio [OR] 5.24; p < 0.01). Thirty-two (24.6%) women underwent 70 reoperations, commonly fat grafting (51.9%) and fat necrosis excision (17.1%). Reoperations were higher among women who received PMRT < 3 months after surgery (48.9 for < 3 months vs. 36.6 for ≥ 3 months; p = 0.19), which was significantly associated in multivariable analysis (OR 0.42; p = 0.08 for ≥ 3 months). The probabilities of complications and reoperations were lowest when PMRT was administered ≥ 3 months after surgery. CONCLUSIONS: Among a large institutional cohort, immediate autologous reconstruction was associated with similar rates of adverse flap outcomes as historically reported alternatively sequenced protocols. IMN radiation increased risk, while PMRT ≥ 3 months after surgery decreased risk. Additional studies are needed to elaborate the impact of IMN radiation and early PMRT in immediate versus delayed autologous reconstruction.


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama , Mamoplastia , Neoplasias da Mama/radioterapia , Neoplasias da Mama/cirurgia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Mastectomia , Complicações Pós-Operatórias/etiologia , Radioterapia Adjuvante , Estudos Retrospectivos , Resultado do Tratamento
7.
Nature ; 522(7556): 309-14, 2015 Jun 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26053122

RESUMO

Spatial working memory, the caching of behaviourally relevant spatial cues on a timescale of seconds, is a fundamental constituent of cognition. Although the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus are known to contribute jointly to successful spatial working memory, the anatomical pathway and temporal window for the interaction of these structures critical to spatial working memory has not yet been established. Here we find that direct hippocampal-prefrontal afferents are critical for encoding, but not for maintenance or retrieval, of spatial cues in mice. These cues are represented by the activity of individual prefrontal units in a manner that is dependent on hippocampal input only during the cue-encoding phase of a spatial working memory task. Successful encoding of these cues appears to be mediated by gamma-frequency synchrony between the two structures. These findings indicate a critical role for the direct hippocampal-prefrontal afferent pathway in the continuous updating of task-related spatial information during spatial working memory.


Assuntos
Hipocampo/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Memória Espacial/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Vias Aferentes/fisiologia , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Hipocampo/citologia , Masculino , Camundongos , Modelos Neurológicos , Optogenética , Córtex Pré-Frontal/citologia
8.
J Neurosci ; 38(46): 9900-9924, 2018 11 14.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30249794

RESUMO

For many neural network models in which neurons are trained to classify inputs like perceptrons, the number of inputs that can be classified is limited by the connectivity of each neuron, even when the total number of neurons is very large. This poses the problem of how the biological brain can take advantage of its huge number of neurons given that the connectivity is sparse. One solution is to combine multiple perceptrons together, as in committee machines. The number of classifiable random patterns would then grow linearly with the number of perceptrons, even when each perceptron has limited connectivity. However, the problem is moved to the downstream readout neurons, which would need a number of connections as large as the number of perceptrons. Here we propose a different approach in which the readout is implemented by connecting multiple perceptrons in a recurrent attractor neural network. We prove analytically that the number of classifiable random patterns can grow unboundedly with the number of perceptrons, even when the connectivity of each perceptron remains finite. Most importantly, both the recurrent connectivity and the connectivity of downstream readouts also remain finite. Our study shows that feedforward neural classifiers with numerous long-range afferent connections can be replaced by recurrent networks with sparse long-range connectivity without sacrificing the classification performance. Our strategy could be used to design more general scalable network architectures with limited connectivity, which resemble more closely the brain neural circuits that are dominated by recurrent connectivity.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The mammalian brain has a huge number of neurons, but the connectivity is rather sparse. This observation seems to contrast with the theoretical studies showing that for many neural network models the performance scales with the number of connections per neuron and not with the total number of neurons. To solve this dilemma, we propose a model in which a recurrent network reads out multiple neural classifiers. Its performance scales with the total number of neurons even when each neuron of the network has limited connectivity. Our study reveals an important role of recurrent connections in neural systems like the hippocampus, in which the computational limitations due to sparse long-range feedforward connectivity might be compensated by local recurrent connections.


Assuntos
Modelos Neurológicos , Redes Neurais de Computação , Neurônios/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Encéfalo/fisiologia
9.
Nature ; 497(7451): 585-90, 2013 May 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23685452

RESUMO

Single-neuron activity in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is tuned to mixtures of multiple task-related aspects. Such mixed selectivity is highly heterogeneous, seemingly disordered and therefore difficult to interpret. We analysed the neural activity recorded in monkeys during an object sequence memory task to identify a role of mixed selectivity in subserving the cognitive functions ascribed to the PFC. We show that mixed selectivity neurons encode distributed information about all task-relevant aspects. Each aspect can be decoded from the population of neurons even when single-cell selectivity to that aspect is eliminated. Moreover, mixed selectivity offers a significant computational advantage over specialized responses in terms of the repertoire of input-output functions implementable by readout neurons. This advantage originates from the highly diverse nonlinear selectivity to mixtures of task-relevant variables, a signature of high-dimensional neural representations. Crucially, this dimensionality is predictive of animal behaviour as it collapses in error trials. Our findings recommend a shift of focus for future studies from neurons that have easily interpretable response tuning to the widely observed, but rarely analysed, mixed selectivity neurons.


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Haplorrinos/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/citologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Análise de Célula Única
10.
J Neurosci ; 37(45): 11021-11036, 2017 11 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28986463

RESUMO

Complex cognitive behaviors, such as context-switching and rule-following, are thought to be supported by the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Neural activity in the PFC must thus be specialized to specific tasks while retaining flexibility. Nonlinear "mixed" selectivity is an important neurophysiological trait for enabling complex and context-dependent behaviors. Here we investigate (1) the extent to which the PFC exhibits computationally relevant properties, such as mixed selectivity, and (2) how such properties could arise via circuit mechanisms. We show that PFC cells recorded from male and female rhesus macaques during a complex task show a moderate level of specialization and structure that is not replicated by a model wherein cells receive random feedforward inputs. While random connectivity can be effective at generating mixed selectivity, the data show significantly more mixed selectivity than predicted by a model with otherwise matched parameters. A simple Hebbian learning rule applied to the random connectivity, however, increases mixed selectivity and enables the model to match the data more accurately. To explain how learning achieves this, we provide analysis along with a clear geometric interpretation of the impact of learning on selectivity. After learning, the model also matches the data on measures of noise, response density, clustering, and the distribution of selectivities. Of two styles of Hebbian learning tested, the simpler and more biologically plausible option better matches the data. These modeling results provide clues about how neural properties important for cognition can arise in a circuit and make clear experimental predictions regarding how various measures of selectivity would evolve during animal training.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The prefrontal cortex is a brain region believed to support the ability of animals to engage in complex behavior. How neurons in this area respond to stimuli-and in particular, to combinations of stimuli ("mixed selectivity")-is a topic of interest. Even though models with random feedforward connectivity are capable of creating computationally relevant mixed selectivity, such a model does not match the levels of mixed selectivity seen in the data analyzed in this study. Adding simple Hebbian learning to the model increases mixed selectivity to the correct level and makes the model match the data on several other relevant measures. This study thus offers predictions on how mixed selectivity and other properties evolve with training.


Assuntos
Aprendizado de Máquina , Redes Neurais de Computação , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Animais , Análise por Conglomerados , Cognição/fisiologia , Simulação por Computador , Feminino , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Neurônios , Córtex Pré-Frontal/citologia
11.
Annu Rev Neurosci ; 33: 173-202, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20331363

RESUMO

Neuroscientists have often described cognition and emotion as separable processes implemented by different regions of the brain, such as the amygdala for emotion and the prefrontal cortex for cognition. In this framework, functional interactions between the amygdala and prefrontal cortex mediate emotional influences on cognitive processes such as decision-making, as well as the cognitive regulation of emotion. However, neurons in these structures often have entangled representations, whereby single neurons encode multiple cognitive and emotional variables. Here we review studies using anatomical, lesion, and neurophysiological approaches to investigate the representation and utilization of cognitive and emotional parameters. We propose that these mental state parameters are inextricably linked and represented in dynamic neural networks composed of interconnected prefrontal and limbic brain structures. Future theoretical and experimental work is required to understand how these mental state representations form and how shifts between mental states occur, a critical feature of adaptive cognitive and emotional behavior.


Assuntos
Tonsila do Cerebelo/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Processos Mentais/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Tonsila do Cerebelo/anatomia & histologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Córtex Pré-Frontal/anatomia & histologia
12.
Ann Plast Surg ; 81(2): 235-239, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29794501

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The deep inferior epigastric perforator (DIEP) flap has gained popularity for autologous free flap breast reconstruction. Historically, patients receiving post mastectomy radiation therapy (PMRT) were not candidates for immediate autologous reconstruction due to concerns for flap volume depletion, fat necrosis, and flap failure. However, this literature is anecdotal and lacks case controls. We objectively analyzed the effects radiation imparts on immediate DIEP flap reconstruction using 3-dimensional software and inherent controls. METHODS: We performed a cohort study on breast cancer patients who underwent immediate bilateral DIEP flap reconstructions followed by PMRT between 2005 and 2014. Exclusion criteria included patients less than 6 months from PMRT completion and bilateral PMRT. Three-dimensional photographs were analyzed using Geomagic (Rock Hill, SC) software to compare flap position, projection, and volume between the irradiated and nonirradiated reconstructed breasts. Breast Q survey evaluated patients' satisfaction. RESULTS: Eleven patients met inclusion criteria. Average time from PMRT completion to photo acquisition was 1.93 years. There was no statistical difference in average volume or projection in the irradiated versus nonirradiated side (P = 0.087 and P = 0.176, respectively). However, position of the irradiated flaps was significantly higher on the chest wall compared to controls (mean difference, 1.325 cm; P < 0.004). CONCLUSIONS: Three-dimensional analysis exhibited no statistical differences in projection or volume between irradiated DIEP flaps and nonirradiated controls. However, irradiated DIEP flaps were positioned higher on the chest wall, similar to observations in irradiated tissue expanders/implants. Patients were satisfied as measured by Breast Q. Immediate bilateral DIEP flap reconstructions can safely be performed with PMRT with satisfactory results.


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama/radioterapia , Artérias Epigástricas , Mamoplastia , Retalho Perfurante/patologia , Fotografação/métodos , Neoplasias da Mama/cirurgia , Feminino , Humanos , Imageamento Tridimensional , Mamoplastia/métodos , Mastectomia , Retalho Perfurante/irrigação sanguínea , Radioterapia Adjuvante , Estudos Retrospectivos
13.
Neural Comput ; 28(10): 2011-44, 2016 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27557100

RESUMO

Neuromorphic engineering combines the architectural and computational principles of systems neuroscience with semiconductor electronics, with the aim of building efficient and compact devices that mimic the synaptic and neural machinery of the brain. The energy consumptions promised by neuromorphic engineering are extremely low, comparable to those of the nervous system. Until now, however, the neuromorphic approach has been restricted to relatively simple circuits and specialized functions, thereby obfuscating a direct comparison of their energy consumption to that used by conventional von Neumann digital machines solving real-world tasks. Here we show that a recent technology developed by IBM can be leveraged to realize neuromorphic circuits that operate as classifiers of complex real-world stimuli. Specifically, we provide a set of general prescriptions to enable the practical implementation of neural architectures that compete with state-of-the-art classifiers. We also show that the energy consumption of these architectures, realized on the IBM chip, is typically two or more orders of magnitude lower than that of conventional digital machines implementing classifiers with comparable performance. Moreover, the spike-based dynamics display a trade-off between integration time and accuracy, which naturally translates into algorithms that can be flexibly deployed for either fast and approximate classifications, or more accurate classifications at the mere expense of longer running times and higher energy costs. This work finally proves that the neuromorphic approach can be efficiently used in real-world applications and has significant advantages over conventional digital devices when energy consumption is considered.


Assuntos
Metabolismo Energético , Redes Neurais de Computação , Semicondutores , Máquina de Vetores de Suporte , Metabolismo Energético/fisiologia , Humanos , Semicondutores/tendências , Máquina de Vetores de Suporte/tendências
14.
Microsurgery ; 35(1): 29-33, 2015 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24470389

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: The free fibular flap is the workhorse for mandibular reconstruction. Three-dimensional (3D) planning, with use of cutting guides and prebent plates, has been introduced. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the interfragmentary gap size and symmetry between conventional freehand preparation versus those using 3D planning. METHODS: A retrospective review was performed. Conventional free form and 3D planned fibular reconstructions performed by the senior authors at a single institution were included. Reconstructions were further subdivided into "body only" and "complex." Demographic and intraoperative data were collected. Postoperative CT scans were analyzed using Materialize software. Interfragmentary gap distances (mm) and symmetry (degrees) were assessed. RESULTS: Nineteen fibular reconstructions met inclusion criteria, ten conventional free form, and nine 3D planned reconstructions. Interfibular gaps measured 0.36 ± 0.50 mm in the 3D group versus 1.88 ± 1.09 mm in the non-3D group (P = 0.004). Overall symmetry (a ratio between right and left angles) measured versus 1.027 ± 0.08 in the 3D-planned versus 1.024 ± 0.09 in the non-3D group in (P = 0.944). Within only mandibular body reconstructions, symmetry was similar between the two techniques: 1.05 ± 0.12 in the 3D group versus 0.97 ± 0.05 in the non-3D group (P = 0.295). CONCLUSIONS: 3D planning lessens interfibular gap dimensions and may enhance axial symmetry. Space between native mandible and fibula is not appreciably altered using planning. Future efforts will focus on the accuracy and reproducibility of the 3D planned to actual results as well as clinical significance and efficiency benefits.


Assuntos
Transplante Ósseo/métodos , Simulação por Computador , Imageamento Tridimensional , Reconstrução Mandibular/métodos , Cirurgia Assistida por Computador/métodos , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Placas Ósseas , Desenho Assistido por Computador , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Estudos Retrospectivos , Coleta de Tecidos e Órgãos/métodos , Adulto Jovem
15.
J Neurosci ; 33(9): 3844-56, 2013 Feb 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23447596

RESUMO

Intelligent behavior requires integrating several sources of information in a meaningful fashion-be it context with stimulus or shape with color and size. This requires the underlying neural mechanism to respond in a different manner to similar inputs (discrimination), while maintaining a consistent response for noisy variations of the same input (generalization). We show that neurons that mix information sources via random connectivity can form an easy to read representation of input combinations. Using analytical and numerical tools, we show that the coding level or sparseness of these neurons' activity controls a trade-off between generalization and discrimination, with the optimal level depending on the task at hand. In all realistic situations that we analyzed, the optimal fraction of inputs to which a neuron responds is close to 0.1. Finally, we predict a relation between a measurable property of the neural representation and task performance.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Generalização Psicológica/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Neurônios/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Animais , Percepção de Cores , Humanos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Neurônios/classificação , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção de Tamanho
16.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 9(7): e1003146, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23935470

RESUMO

Long-term memories are likely stored in the synaptic weights of neuronal networks in the brain. The storage capacity of such networks depends on the degree of plasticity of their synapses. Highly plastic synapses allow for strong memories, but these are quickly overwritten. On the other hand, less labile synapses result in long-lasting but weak memories. Here we show that the trade-off between memory strength and memory lifetime can be overcome by partitioning the memory system into multiple regions characterized by different levels of synaptic plasticity and transferring memory information from the more to less plastic region. The improvement in memory lifetime is proportional to the number of memory regions, and the initial memory strength can be orders of magnitude larger than in a non-partitioned memory system. This model provides a fundamental computational reason for memory consolidation processes at the systems level.


Assuntos
Memória , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa , Sinapses
17.
Learn Mem ; 20(12): 710-29, 2013 Nov 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24255101

RESUMO

In the adult mammalian brain, newly generated neurons are continuously incorporated into two networks: interneurons born in the subventricular zone migrate to the olfactory bulb, whereas the dentate gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus integrates locally born principal neurons. That the rest of the mammalian brain loses significant neurogenic capacity after the perinatal period suggests that unique aspects of the structure and function of DG and olfactory bulb circuits allow them to benefit from the adult generation of neurons. In this review, we consider the distinctive features of the DG that may account for it being able to profit from this singular form of neural plasticity. Approaches to the problem of neurogenesis are grouped as "bottom-up," where the phenotype of adult-born granule cells is contrasted to that of mature developmentally born granule cells, and "top-down," where the impact of altering the amount of neurogenesis on behavior is examined. We end by considering the primary implications of these two approaches and future directions.


Assuntos
Giro Denteado/fisiologia , Neurogênese/fisiologia , Animais , Giro Denteado/citologia , Neurônios/fisiologia
18.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 28(7): 677-690, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38553340

RESUMO

One major challenge of neuroscience is identifying structure in seemingly disorganized neural activity. Different types of structure have different computational implications that can help neuroscientists understand the functional role of a particular brain area. Here, we outline a unified approach to characterize structure by inspecting the representational geometry and the modularity properties of the recorded activity and show that a similar approach can also reveal structure in connectivity. We start by setting up a general framework for determining geometry and modularity in activity and connectivity and relating these properties with computations performed by the network. We then use this framework to review the types of structure found in recent studies of model networks performing three classes of computations.


Assuntos
Encéfalo , Modelos Neurológicos , Rede Nervosa , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Animais , Conectoma
19.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Oct 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39415994

RESUMO

The brain has large-scale modular structure in the form of brain regions, which are thought to arise from constraints on connectivity and the physical geometry of the cortical sheet. In contrast, experimental and theoretical work has argued both for and against the existence of specialized sub-populations of neurons (modules) within single brain regions. By studying artificial neural networks, we show that this local modularity emerges to support context-dependent behavior, but only when the input is low-dimensional. No anatomical constraints are required. We also show when modular specialization emerges at the population level (different modules correspond to orthogonal subspaces). Modularity yields abstract representations, allows for rapid learning and generalization on novel tasks, and facilitates the rapid learning of related contexts. Non-modular representations facilitate the rapid learning of unrelated contexts. Our findings reconcile conflicting experimental results and make predictions for future experiments.

20.
Nat Commun ; 15(1): 6479, 2024 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39090091

RESUMO

Animals likely use a variety of strategies to solve laboratory tasks. Traditionally, combined analysis of behavioral and neural recording data across subjects employing different strategies may obscure important signals and give confusing results. Hence, it is essential to develop techniques that can infer strategy at the single-subject level. We analyzed an experiment in which two male monkeys performed a visually cued rule-based task. The analysis of their performance shows no indication that they used a different strategy. However, when we examined the geometry of stimulus representations in the state space of the neural activities recorded in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, we found striking differences between the two monkeys. Our purely neural results induced us to reanalyze the behavior. The new analysis showed that the differences in representational geometry are associated with differences in the reaction times, revealing behavioral differences we were unaware of. All these analyses suggest that the monkeys are using different strategies. Finally, using recurrent neural network models trained to perform the same task, we show that these strategies correlate with the amount of training, suggesting a possible explanation for the observed neural and behavioral differences.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Macaca mulatta , Córtex Pré-Frontal , Animais , Masculino , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Córtex Pré-Frontal/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Redes Neurais de Computação , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Neurônios/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA