Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 21
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Child Dev ; 90(5): e565-e583, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30737991

RESUMO

The causal role of speed of processing (SOP) in developmental language disorder (DLD) is unclear given that SOP has been implicated in other neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. This study investigated associations between SOP, language, and inattention/hyperactivity in a U.K. epidemiological cohort (N = 528). Monolingual children from a range of socioeconomic backgrounds were assessed longitudinally; at ages 5-6 (2012/2013) and 7-8 years (2014/2015). Persistent weaknesses in SOP characterized children with DLD but did not predict language longitudinally. Ratings of inattention/hyperactivity moderated the association between SOP and language, indicating that SOP deficits are particularly detrimental for language when coupled with poor attention/hyperactivity. SOP may be a shared risk factor for DLD and inattention/hyperactivity or a general marker of neurodevelopmental disorder.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/psicologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Processos Mentais/fisiologia , Agitação Psicomotora/psicologia , Fatores Socioeconômicos
2.
Child Dev ; 90(5): e548-e564, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30676649

RESUMO

We followed children at family risk of dyslexia and children with preschool language difficulties from age 3½, comparing them with controls (N = 234). At age 8, children were classified as having dyslexia or Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) and compared at earlier time points with controls. Children with dyslexia have specific difficulties with phonology and emergent reading skills in the preschool period, whereas children with DLD, with or without dyslexia, show a wider range of impairments including significant problems with executive and motor tasks. For children with both dyslexia and DLD, difficulties with phonology are generally more severe than those observed in children with dyslexia or DLD alone. Findings confirm that poor phonology is the major cognitive risk factor for dyslexia.


Assuntos
Dislexia/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Dislexia/psicologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Masculino , Fonética , Transtornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico , Transtornos Psicomotores/psicologia , Leitura , Fatores de Risco
3.
Psychol Sci ; 29(8): 1270-1282, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29791271

RESUMO

This study evaluated the claim that auditory processing deficits are a cause of reading and language difficulties. We report a longitudinal study of 245 children at family risk of dyslexia, children with preschool language impairments, and control children. Children with language impairments had poorer frequency-discrimination thresholds than controls at 5.5 years, but children at family risk of dyslexia did not. A model assessing longitudinal relationships among frequency discrimination, reading, language, and executive function skills showed that frequency discrimination was predicted by executive skills but was not a longitudinal predictor of reading or language skills. Our findings contradict the hypothesis that frequency discrimination is causally related to dyslexia or language impairment and suggest that individuals at risk for dyslexia or who have language impairments may perform poorly on auditory processing tasks because of comorbid attentional difficulties.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Discriminação Psicológica , Dislexia/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Leitura
4.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 58(10): 1092-1105, 2017 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28921543

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Language development has been characterised by significant individual stability from school entry. However, the extent to which trajectories of language growth vary in children with language disorder as a function of co-occurring developmental challenges is a question of theoretical import, with implications for service provision. METHODS: SCALES employed a population-based survey design with sample weighting procedures to estimate growth in core language skills over the first three years of school. A stratified sample (n = 529) received comprehensive assessment of language, nonverbal IQ, and social, emotional and behavioural difficulties at 5-6 years of age and 95% of the sample (n = 499) were assessed again at ages 7-8. Language growth was measured using both raw and standard scores in children with typical development, children with language disorder of unknown origin, and children with language disorders associated with a known clinical condition and/or intellectual disability. RESULTS: Overall, language was stable at the individual level (estimated ICC = 0.95) over the first three years of school. Linear mixed effects models highlighted steady growth in language raw scores across all three groups, including those with multiple developmental challenges. There was little evidence, however, that children with language disorders were narrowing the gap with peers (z-scores). Adjusted models indicated that while nonverbal ability, socioeconomic status and social, emotional and behavioural deficits predicted initial language score (intercept), none predicted language growth (slope). CONCLUSIONS: These findings corroborate previous studies suggesting stable language trajectories after ages 5-6 years, but add considerably to previous work by demonstrating similar developmental patterns in children with additional nonverbal cognitive deficits, social, emotional, and behavioural challenges, social disadvantage or clinical diagnoses.


Assuntos
Inteligência/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Transtornos da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Testes de Linguagem/estatística & dados numéricos , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Prognóstico
5.
Dev Sci ; 20(4)2017 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27496263

RESUMO

The 'automatic letter-sound integration hypothesis' (Blomert, ) proposes that dyslexia results from a failure to fully integrate letters and speech sounds into automated audio-visual objects. We tested this hypothesis in a sample of English-speaking children with dyslexic difficulties (N = 13) and samples of chronological-age-matched (CA; N = 17) and reading-age-matched controls (RA; N = 17) aged 7-13 years. Each child took part in two priming experiments in which speech sounds were preceded by congruent visual letters (congruent condition) or Greek letters (baseline). In a behavioural experiment, responses to speech sounds in the two conditions were compared using reaction times. These data revealed faster reaction times in the congruent condition in all three groups. In a second electrophysiological experiment, responses to speech sounds in the two conditions were compared using event-related potentials (ERPs). These data revealed a significant effect of congruency on (1) the P1 ERP over left frontal electrodes in the CA group and over fronto-central electrodes in the dyslexic group and (2) the P2 ERP in the dyslexic and RA control groups. These findings suggest that our sample of English-speaking children with dyslexic difficulties demonstrate a degree of letter-sound integration that is appropriate for their reading level, which challenges the letter-sound integration hypothesis.


Assuntos
Estimulação Acústica , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Alfabetização , Percepção da Fala , Adolescente , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Humanos , Tempo de Reação
6.
BMC Psychiatry ; 17(1): 129, 2017 04 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28381293

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Rating scales are often used to identify children with potential Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), yet there are frequently discrepancies between informants which may be moderated by child characteristics. The current study asked whether correspondence between parent and teacher ratings on the Strengths and Weakness of ADHD symptoms and Normal behaviour scale (SWAN) varied systematically with child language ability. METHOD: Parent and teacher SWAN questionnaires were returned for 200 children (aged 61-81 months); 106 had low language ability (LL) and 94 had typically developing language (TL). After exploring informant correspondence (using Pearson correlation) and the discrepancy between raters, we report inter-class correlation coefficients, to assess inter-rater reliability, and Cohen's kappa, to assess agreement regarding possible ADHD caseness. RESULTS: Correlations between informant ratings on the SWAN were moderate. Children with LL were rated as having increased inattention and hyperactivity relative to children with TL; teachers, however, rated children with LL as having more inattention than parents. Inter-rater reliability of the SWAN was good and there were no systematic differences between the LL and TL groups. Case agreement between parent and teachers was fair; this varied by language group with poorer case agreement for children with LL. CONCLUSION: Children's language abilities affect the discrepancy between informant ratings of ADHD symptomatology and the agreement between parents and teachers regarding potential ADHD caseness. The assessment of children's core language ability would be a beneficial addition to the ADHD diagnostic process.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/diagnóstico , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Pais/psicologia , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/psicologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Inquéritos e Questionários
7.
Child Dev ; 88(3): 812-827, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27647334

RESUMO

Children learning English as an additional language (EAL) often experience lower academic attainment than monolingual peers. In this study, teachers provided ratings of English language proficiency and social, emotional, and behavioral functioning for 782 children with EAL and 6,485 monolingual children in reception year (ages 4-5). Academic attainment was assessed in reception and Year 2 (ages 6-7). Relative to monolingual peers with comparable English language proficiency, children with EAL displayed fewer social, emotional, and behavioral difficulties in reception, were equally likely to meet curriculum targets in reception, and were more likely to meet targets in Year 2. Academic attainment and social, emotional, and behavioral functioning in children with EAL are associated with English language proficiency at school entry.


Assuntos
Sucesso Acadêmico , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Emoções , Aprendizagem , Multilinguismo , Comportamento Social , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Emoções/fisiologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Masculino
8.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 57(2): 180-7, 2016 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26307609

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The developmental relationships between executive functions (EF) and early language skills are unclear. This study explores the longitudinal relationships between children's early EF and language skills in a sample of children with a wide range of language abilities including children at risk of dyslexia. In addition, we investigated whether these skills independently predict children's attention/behaviour skills. METHOD: Data are presented from 243 children at four time points. Children were selected for being at risk of reading difficulties either because of a family history of dyslexia (FR; N = 90) or because of concerns regarding their language development (LI; N = 79) or as typically developing controls (TD; N = 74). The children completed tasks to assess their executive function and language skills at ages 4, 5 and 6 years. At 6 (T4) and 7 years (T5) parents and teachers rated the children's attention/behaviour skills. RESULTS: There was a strong concurrent relationship between language and EF at each assessment. Longitudinal analyses indicated a considerable degree of stability in children's language and EF skills: the influence of language on later EF skills (and vice versa) was weak and not significant in the current sample. Children's EF, but not language, skills at T3 predicted attention/behaviour ratings at T4/T5. CONCLUSIONS: There is a strong concurrent association between language and EF skills during the preschool and early school years, when children with language impairment show persistent EF deficits. Latent variables measuring language and EF show high longitudinal stability with little evidence of significant or strong reciprocal influences between these constructs. EF, but not language, skills predict later ratings of children's attention and behaviour.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Comportamento Infantil/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Instituições Acadêmicas
9.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 57(1): 65-73, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26041601

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The youngest children in an academic year are reported to be educationally disadvantaged and overrepresented in referrals to clinical services. In this study we investigate for the first time whether these disadvantages are indicative of a mismatch between language competence at school entry and the academic demands of the classroom. METHODS: We recruited a population sample of 7,267 children aged 4 years 9 months to 5 years 10 months attending state-maintained reception classrooms in Surrey, England. Teacher ratings on the Children's Communication Checklist-Short (CCC-S), a measure of language competence, the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire-Total Difficulties Score (SDQ), a measure of behavioural problems, and the Early Years Foundation Stage Profile (EYFSP), a measure of academic attainment, were obtained at the end of the reception year. RESULTS: The youngest children were rated by teachers as having more language deficits, behaviour problems, and poorer academic progress at the end of the school year. Language deficits were highly associated with behaviour problems; adjusted odds ratio 8.70, 95% CI [7.25-10.45]. Only 4.8% of children with teacher-rated language deficits and 1.3% of those with co-occurring language and behaviour difficulties obtained a 'Good Level of Development' on the EYFSP. While age predicted unique variance in academic attainment (1%), language competence was the largest associate of academic achievement (19%). CONCLUSION: The youngest children starting school have relatively immature language and behaviour skills and many are not yet ready to meet the academic and social demands of the classroom. At a population level, developing oral language skills and/or ensuring academic targets reflect developmental capacity could substantially reduce the numbers of children requiring specialist clinical services in later years.

10.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 57(11): 1247-1257, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27184709

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Diagnosis of 'specific' language impairment traditionally required nonverbal IQ to be within normal limits, often resulting in restricted access to clinical services for children with lower NVIQ. Changes to DSM-5 criteria for language disorder removed this NVIQ requirement. This study sought to delineate the impact of varying NVIQ criteria on prevalence, clinical presentation and functional impact of language disorder in the first UK population study of language impairment at school entry. METHODS: A population-based survey design with sample weighting procedures was used to estimate population prevalence. We surveyed state-maintained reception classrooms (n = 161 or 61% of eligible schools) in Surrey, England. From a total population of 12,398 children (ages 4-5 years), 7,267 (59%) were screened. A stratified subsample (n = 529) received comprehensive assessment of language, NVIQ, social, emotional and behavioural problems, and academic attainment. RESULTS: The total population prevalence estimate of language disorder was 9.92% (95% CI 7.38, 13.20). The prevalence of language disorder of unknown origin was estimated to be 7.58% (95% CI 5.33, 10.66), while the prevalence of language impairment associated with intellectual disability and/or existing medical diagnosis was 2.34% (95% CI 1.40, 3.91). Children with language disorder displayed elevated symptoms of social, emotional and behavioural problems relative to peers, F(1, 466) = 7.88, p = .05, and 88% did not make expected academic progress. There were no differences between those with average and low-average NVIQ scores in severity of language deficit, social, emotional and behavioural problems, or educational attainment. In contrast, children with language impairments associated with known medical diagnosis and/or intellectual disability displayed more severe deficits on multiple measures. CONCLUSIONS: At school entry, approximately two children in every class of 30 pupils will experience language disorder severe enough to hinder academic progress. Access to specialist clinical services should not depend on NVIQ.


Assuntos
Inteligência/fisiologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Pré-Escolar , Inglaterra/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Prevalência
11.
Psychol Sci ; 26(12): 1877-86, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26525072

RESUMO

The development of reading skills is underpinned by oral language abilities: Phonological skills appear to have a causal influence on the development of early word-level literacy skills, and reading-comprehension ability depends, in addition to word-level literacy skills, on broader (semantic and syntactic) language skills. Here, we report a longitudinal study of children at familial risk of dyslexia, children with preschool language difficulties, and typically developing control children. Preschool measures of oral language predicted phoneme awareness and grapheme-phoneme knowledge just before school entry, which in turn predicted word-level literacy skills shortly after school entry. Reading comprehension at 8½ years was predicted by word-level literacy skills at 5½ years and by language skills at 3½ years. These patterns of predictive relationships were similar in both typically developing children and those at risk of literacy difficulties. Our findings underline the importance of oral language skills for the development of both word-level literacy and reading comprehension.


Assuntos
Compreensão , Dislexia/psicologia , Alfabetização/normas , Fonética , Leitura , Semântica , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Estudos Prospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Reino Unido
12.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 56(9): 976-87, 2015 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25832320

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Causal theories of dyslexia suggest that it is a heritable disorder, which is the outcome of multiple risk factors. However, whether early screening for dyslexia is viable is not yet known. METHODS: The study followed children at high risk of dyslexia from preschool through the early primary years assessing them from age 3 years and 6 months (T1) at approximately annual intervals on tasks tapping cognitive, language, and executive-motor skills. The children were recruited to three groups: children at family risk of dyslexia, children with concerns regarding speech, and language development at 3;06 years and controls considered to be typically developing. At 8 years, children were classified as 'dyslexic' or not. Logistic regression models were used to predict the individual risk of dyslexia and to investigate how risk factors accumulate to predict poor literacy outcomes. RESULTS: Family-risk status was a stronger predictor of dyslexia at 8 years than low language in preschool. Additional predictors in the preschool years include letter knowledge, phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, and executive skills. At the time of school entry, language skills become significant predictors, and motor skills add a small but significant increase to the prediction probability. We present classification accuracy using different probability cutoffs for logistic regression models and ROC curves to highlight the accumulation of risk factors at the individual level. CONCLUSIONS: Dyslexia is the outcome of multiple risk factors and children with language difficulties at school entry are at high risk. Family history of dyslexia is a predictor of literacy outcome from the preschool years. However, screening does not reach an acceptable clinical level until close to school entry when letter knowledge, phonological awareness, and RAN, rather than family risk, together provide good sensitivity and specificity as a screening battery.


Assuntos
Dislexia/diagnóstico , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Predisposição Genética para Doença , Idioma , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Prognóstico , Fatores de Risco
13.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 55(3): 237-46, 2014 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24117483

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Comorbidity among developmental disorders such as dyslexia, language impairment, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder is common. This study explores comorbid weaknesses in preschool children at family risk of dyslexia with and without language impairment and considers the role that comorbidity plays in determining children's outcomes. METHOD: The preschool attention, executive function and motor skills of 112 children at family risk for dyslexia, 29 of whom also met criteria for language impairment, were assessed at ages 3½ and 4½ years. The performance of these children was compared to the performance of children with language impairment and typically developing controls. RESULTS: Weaknesses in attention, executive function and motor skills were associated with language impairment rather than family risk status. Individual differences in language and executive function are strongly related during the preschool period, and preschool motor skills predicted unique variance (4%) in early reading skills over and above children's language ability. CONCLUSION: Comorbidity between developmental disorders can be observed in the preschool years: children with language impairment have significant and persistent weaknesses in motor skills and executive function compared to those without language impairment. Children's early language and motor skills are predictors of children's later reading skills.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Destreza Motora/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Comorbidade , Dislexia/epidemiologia , Feminino , Predisposição Genética para Doença , Humanos , Individualidade , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Masculino , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Estudos Prospectivos , Leitura , Risco
14.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 54(9): 958-68, 2013 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23772651

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Children at family risk of dyslexia have been reported to show phonological deficits as well as broader language delays in the preschool years. METHOD: The preschool language skills of 112 children at family risk of dyslexia (FR) at ages 3½ and 4½ were compared with those of children with SLI and typically developing (TD) controls. RESULTS: Children at FR showed two different profiles: one third of the group resembled the children with SLI and scored poorly across multiple domains of language including phonology. As a group, the remaining children had difficulties on tasks tapping phonological skills at T1 and T2. At the individual level, we confirmed that some FR children had both phonological and broader oral language difficulties (compared with TD controls), some had only phonological difficulties and some appeared to be developing typically. CONCLUSIONS: We have highlighted the early overlap between family risk of dyslexia and SLI. A family history of dyslexia carries an increased risk for SLI and the two disorders both show an increased incidence of phonological deficits which appear to a proximal risk factor for developing a reading impairment.


Assuntos
Dislexia/etiologia , Família/psicologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/psicologia , Pré-Escolar , Dislexia/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos da Linguagem/complicações , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Fonética , Fatores de Risco , Testes de Articulação da Fala
15.
BMC Complement Altern Med ; 12: 167, 2012 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23025450

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Difficulty in controlling attention can lead to mental fatigue in the healthy population. We identified one trial reporting a benefit in patients' attention using a homeopathic formula preparation. One component of the preparation was potassium phosphate, widely available off the shelf as Kali phos 6x for cognitive problems. The aim of this exploratory trial was to assess the effectiveness of Kali phos 6x for attention problems associated with mental fatigue. METHODS: We recruited student and staff volunteers (University of York) with self-reported mental fatigue, excluding any using homeopathy or prescribed stimulants, or with a diagnosis of chronic fatigue syndrome. In a triple blind, cross-over, placebo-controlled clinical trial, 86 volunteers were randomized to receive Kali phos 6x or identical placebo 10 minutes before taking a psychological test of attention (Stroop Colour-Word Test). One week later they were crossed over and took the other preparation before repeating the test. RESULTS: We found no evidence of a treatment effect in a comparison of Kali phos 6x with placebo (Kali phos minus placebo = -1.1 (95% CI -3.0 to 0.9, P = 0.3) Stroop score units, Cohen effect size = -0.17) even when allowing for a weak period effect with accuracy scores in the second period being higher than those in the first (P = 0.05). We observed a ceiling effect in the Stroop test which undermined our ability to interpret this result. CONCLUSIONS: Kali phos 6x was not found to be effective in reducing mental fatigue. A ceiling effect in our primary outcome measure meant that we could not rule out a type II error. Thorough piloting of an adequate outcome measure could have led to an unequivocal result. CURRENT CONTROLLED TRIALS: ISRCTN16521161.


Assuntos
Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Homeopatia , Materia Medica/farmacologia , Fadiga Mental/tratamento farmacológico , Estudos Cross-Over , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Materia Medica/uso terapêutico , Autorrelato , Resultado do Tratamento
16.
Dev Psychol ; 58(3): 522-534, 2022 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34941300

RESUMO

Emerging evidence suggests interventions can improve childhood self-regulation. One intervention approach that has shown promise is Taekwondo martial arts instruction, though little is known about its acceptability among stakeholders or its mechanisms of effect. We extend evidence on Taekwondo interventions in three ways: (a) testing the efficacy of a standard introductory course of Taekwondo, (b) assessing the acceptability of Taekwondo instruction among school children, and (c) investigating two self-regulatory mechanisms by which Taekwondo may operate (executive functions and motivation). This article reports findings from a randomized control trial implementing a standard 11-week beginners' course of Taekwondo. Participants were from a mixed-sex, nonselective U.K. primary school (N = 240, age range 7 to 11 years). Measures of self-regulation included teacher-rated effortful control, impulsivity, prosocial behavior, and conduct problems; computer-based assessments of executive functions; and child self-reported expectancies and values to use self-regulation. Postintervention, children in the Taekwondo condition were rated by teachers as having fewer symptoms of conduct problems and better effortful control (specifically attentional control), and they also had better executive attention assessed by a flanker task. Effects were not found for teacher-rated inhibitory control, activation control, impulsivity, and prosocial behavior or for assessments of response inhibition, verbal working memory, and switching. Taekwondo was rated very positively by children. Finally, there was evidence that children who completed Taekwondo classes reported higher expectancies and values to use self-regulation and that expectancies and values mediated intervention effects on self-regulation. We conclude that short standard Taekwondo courses are well received by pupils, improve attentional self-regulation, and reduce symptoms of conduct problems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Artes Marciais , Autocontrole , Criança , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Humanos
17.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry ; 52(2): 195-203, 2011 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20860755

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Deficits in time perception (the ability to judge the duration of time intervals) have been found in children with both attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and dyslexia. This paper investigates time perception, phonological skills and executive functions in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms (AS). METHOD: Children with dyslexia-only (n = 17), AS-only (n = 17), comorbid dyslexia+AS (n = 25), and typically developing controls (n = 42), matched for age and non-verbal ability, were assessed on measures of phonological skills, executive function and time perception (duration discrimination and time reproduction). RESULTS: Children with dyslexia were impaired on measures of phonological skill and duration discrimination compared to children without dyslexia (though problems on duration discrimination appeared to be attributable to mild symptoms of inattention in this group). In contrast, children with AS exhibited impairments on measures of both time perception and executive function compared to children without AS. Children with dyslexia+AS showed an additive combination of the deficits associated with dyslexia-only and AS-only. CONCLUSIONS: Dyslexia and AS appear to be associated with distinct patterns of cognitive deficit, which are present in combination in children with dyslexia+AS.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/psicologia , Dislexia/psicologia , Função Executiva , Leitura , Percepção do Tempo , Aprendizagem Verbal , Adolescente , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Comorbidade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fonética
18.
PLoS One ; 14(3): e0213492, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30845265

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Centile curves and standard scores are common in epidemiological research. However, standardised norms and centile growth curves for language disorder that reflect the entire UK local school population do not exist. METHODS: Scores on six language indices assessing receptive and expressive functioning of children were obtained from the SCALES population survey. Monolingual English speaking participants were aged between five and nine years. Children who attended special schools at study intake, or who were learning English as an additional language were excluded. We constructed language norms using the LMS method of standardisation which allows for skewed measurements. We made use of probability weights that were produced from a two-step logistic model. Distributions of estimated standard scores from an intensively assessed sub-population and from the full population were contrasted to demonstrate the role of weights. RESULTS: Non-overlapping centile curves and standardised scores at each age were obtained for the six language indices. The use of weights was essential at retrieving the target distribution of the scores. An online calculator that estimates standardised scores for the measures was constructed and made freely available. CONCLUSIONS: The findings highlight the usefulness and flexibility of the LMS method at dealing with the standardisation of linguistic and educational measures that are sufficiently continuous. The paper adds to the existing literature by providing population norms for a number of language tests that were calculated from the same group of individuals.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Transtornos da Linguagem , Idioma , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Inglaterra/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos da Linguagem/epidemiologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Linguagem/terapia , Masculino
19.
J Learn Disabil ; 49(3): 272-81, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25124507

RESUMO

High comorbidity rates between reading disorder (RD) and mathematics disorder (MD) indicate that, although the cognitive core deficits underlying these disorders are distinct, additional domain-general risk factors might be shared between the disorders. Three domain-general cognitive abilities were investigated in children with RD and MD: processing speed, temporal processing, and working memory. Since attention problems frequently co-occur with learning disorders, the study examined whether these three factors, which are known to be associated with attention problems, account for the comorbidity between these disorders. The sample comprised 99 primary school children in four groups: children with RD, children with MD, children with both disorders (RD+MD), and typically developing children (TD controls). Measures of processing speed, temporal processing, and memory were analyzed in a series of ANCOVAs including attention ratings as covariate. All three risk factors were associated with poor attention. After controlling for attention, associations with RD and MD differed: Although deficits in verbal memory were associated with both RD and MD, reduced processing speed was related to RD, but not MD; and the association with RD was restricted to processing speed for familiar nameable symbols. In contrast, impairments in temporal processing and visuospatial memory were associated with MD, but not RD.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Discalculia/fisiopatologia , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Transtorno de Aprendizagem Específico/fisiopatologia , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Criança , Comorbidade , Discalculia/epidemiologia , Dislexia/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Risco , Transtorno de Aprendizagem Específico/epidemiologia
20.
Dev Neuropsychol ; 37(5): 453-72, 2012.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22799763

RESUMO

Reaction time (RT) variability on a Stop Signal task was examined among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and/or dyslexia in comparison to typically developing (TD) controls. Children's go-trial RTs were analyzed using a novel ex-Gaussian method. Children with ADHD symptoms had increased variability in the fast but not the slow portions of their RT distributions compared to those without ADHD symptoms. The RT distributions of children with dyslexia were similar to those of TD-controls. It is argued that variability in responding may be underpinned by impairments in response preparation or timing during Stop Signal tasks.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/fisiopatologia , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Inibição Psicológica , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA