Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 197
Filtrar
2.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 19432, 2024 08 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39191807

RESUMEN

Societies are becoming more polarised, driven in part by misconceptions about out-groups' beliefs. To understand these effects, one must examine the cognitive processes underlying how people think about others. Here, we investigate whether people are less prone to theorise about the minds of out-groups, or less able to do so. Participants (Study 1: n = 128; Study 2: n = 128) made inferences about social and political beliefs held by real in-group and out-group members, and could choose to receive further information to improve these inferences. Results show: (1) participants sought equivalent or greater information about out-groups relative to in-groups; but despite this, (2) made significantly less accurate inferences for out-groups; and (3) were significantly less aware of their reduced ability. This shows that poorer mental state inference is not underpinned by a reduced propensity to consider out-group minds, but instead by a worse representation of the minds of out-groups.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Cognición/fisiología , Teoría de la Mente , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adolescente , Política
3.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 379(1908): 20230256, 2024 Aug 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39005033

RESUMEN

Theories of emotion ascribe a fundamental role to the processing of bodily signals (interoception) in emotional experience. Despite evidence consistent with this, current knowledge is limited by a focus on interoceptive accuracy and laboratory-based interoception measures. This experience-sampling study examines how state interoceptive attention and state emotional experience are related in everyday life, providing the first data to our knowledge examining: (1) within-subject fluctuations in interoceptive attention across domains, and (2) the relationship between trait and state interoception. Compared with rates of exteroceptive attention (auditory attention: engaged 83% of the time), interoceptive signals captured attention approximately 20% of the time, with substantial within- and between-person variability across domains. There were relationships between interoceptive attention and emotion in daily life (greater attention being associated with more negative valence and fatigue) that were specific to interoceptive attention (different patterns were observed with exteroceptive attention). State measures of interoceptive (but not exteroceptive) attention were correlated with the trait interoceptive attention, but not accuracy. Results underscore the relationship between interoceptive attention and emotion, providing new insights into interoceptive attention and the structure of interoceptive ability. Future research should examine the source(s) of within- and between-person variability in interoceptive and exteroceptive attention and its relationship with emotional experience. This article is part of the theme issue 'Sensing and feeling: an integrative approach to sensory processing and emotional experience'.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Atención , Interocepción , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Emociones/fisiología , Adolescente , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea
4.
Emotion ; 2024 Jul 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39052419

RESUMEN

Autistic youths tend to react negatively to uncertain events. Little is known about the cognitive processes associated with this intolerance of uncertainty, most notably the tendency to actively gather information to minimize uncertainty. Past research has relied on self-report measures that may not allow investigation of the multifaceted processes associated with intolerance of uncertainty, including information gathering. Alexithymia (difficulties in identifying and describing one's own emotions) commonly co-occurs with autistic traits, but its role in information gathering has rarely been considered. Accordingly, 97 typically developing youths (aged 6-25 years) performed an information gathering task in which they were asked to gather information to infer socioemotional (emotional state) and nonsocial (clothing preference) information about another person when information gathering was costly versus not costly. Dimensional autistic traits were associated with more information gathering regardless of costs and information type. Computational modeling suggested this may be because of the delayed emergence of subjective costs of information gathering in high autistic trait individuals, resulting in later guesses. Alexithymia was uniquely associated with inconsistent emotional responses to rewards and losses and to reduced information gathering about emotional states when assessed using parent-report measures. Future validation in youths diagnosed with autism is warranted to test the generalizability of the findings. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

5.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 162: 105717, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38754718

RESUMEN

There is a reliable association between autism and Feeding and Eating Disorders. Concerningly, where these two conditions co-occur, clinical outcomes of Feeding and Eating Disorders are significantly worse, and treatment less effective, than when the Feeding and Eating Disorders occur in neurotypical individuals. Problematically, the reason for the association between autism and Feeding and Eating Disorders is poorly understood, which constrains advances in clinical care. This paper outlines several possible mechanisms that may underlie the observed association and suggests ways in which they may be empirically tested. Mechanisms are split into those producing an artefactual association, and those reflecting a genuine link between conditions. Artefactual associations may be due to conceptual overlap in both diagnostic criteria and measurement, Feeding and Eating Disorders causing transient autistic traits, or the association being non-specific in nature. A genuine association between autism and Feeding and Eating Disorders may be due to common causal factors, autism directly or indirectly causing Feeding and Eating Disorders, and Feeding and Eating Disorders being a female manifestation of autism.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos , Humanos , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos/fisiopatología , Trastornos de Alimentación y de la Ingestión de Alimentos/etiología , Trastorno Autístico/fisiopatología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología
6.
Ann Thorac Surg ; 117(6): 1178-1185, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38484909

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Junctional ectopic tachycardia (JET) complicates congenital heart surgery in 2% to 8.3% of cases. JET is associated with postoperative morbidity in single-center studies. We used the Pediatric Cardiac Critical Care Consortium data registry to provide a multicenter epidemiologic description of treated JET. METHODS: This is a retrospective study (February 2019-August 2022) of patients with treated JET. Inclusion criteria were (1) <12 months old at the index operation, and (2) treated for JET <72 hours after surgery. Diagnosis was defined by receiving treatment (pacing, cooling, and medications). A multilevel logistic regression analysis with hospital random effect identified JET risk factors. Impact of JET on outcomes was estimated by margins/attributable risk analysis using previous risk-adjustment models. RESULTS: Among 24,073 patients from 63 centers, 1436 (6.0%) were treated for JET with significant center variability (0% to 17.9%). Median time to onset was 3.4 hours, with 34% present on admission. Median duration was 2 days (interquartile range, 1-4 days). Tetralogy of Fallot, atrioventricular canal, and ventricular septal defect repair represented >50% of JET. Patient characteristics independently associated with JET included neonatal age, Asian race, cardiopulmonary bypass time, open sternum, and early postoperative inotropic agents. JET was associated with increased risk-adjusted durations of mechanical ventilation (incidence rate ratio, 1.6; 95% CI, 1.5-1.7) and intensive care unit length of stay (incidence rate ratio, 1.3; 95% CI, 1.2-1.3), but not mortality. CONCLUSIONS: JET is treated in 6% of patients with substantial center variability. JET contributes to increased use of postoperative resources. High center variability warrants further study to identify potential modifiable factors that could serve as targets for improvement efforts to ameliorate deleterious outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Cardíacos , Cardiopatías Congénitas , Complicaciones Posoperatorias , Taquicardia Ectópica de Unión , Humanos , Taquicardia Ectópica de Unión/epidemiología , Taquicardia Ectópica de Unión/etiología , Estudios Retrospectivos , Lactante , Femenino , Masculino , Procedimientos Quirúrgicos Cardíacos/efectos adversos , Complicaciones Posoperatorias/epidemiología , Cardiopatías Congénitas/cirugía , Recién Nacido , Incidencia , Factores de Riesgo , Estados Unidos/epidemiología
7.
Trials ; 25(1): 143, 2024 Feb 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38395922

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Adolescence is a period of heightened vulnerability to developing mental health problems, and rates of mental health disorder in this age group have increased in the last decade. Preventing mental health problems developing before they become entrenched, particularly in adolescents who are at high risk, is an important research and clinical target. Here, we report the protocol for the trial of the 'Building Resilience through Socioemotional Training' (ReSET) intervention. ReSET is a new, preventative intervention that incorporates individual-based emotional training techniques and group-based social and communication skills training. We take a transdiagnostic approach, focusing on emotion processing and social mechanisms implicated in the onset and maintenance of various forms of psychopathology. METHODS: A cluster randomised allocation design is adopted with randomisation at the school year level. Five-hundred and forty adolescents (aged 12-14) will be randomised to either receive the intervention or not (passive control). The intervention is comprised of weekly sessions over an 8-week period, supplemented by two individual sessions. The primary outcomes, psychopathology symptoms and mental wellbeing, will be assessed pre- and post-intervention, and at a 1-year follow-up. Secondary outcomes are task-based assessments of emotion processing, social network data based on peer nominations, and subjective ratings of social relationships. These measures will be taken at baseline, post-intervention and 1-year follow-up. A subgroup of participants and stakeholders will be invited to take part in focus groups to assess the acceptability of the intervention. DISCUSSION: This project adopts a theory-based approach to the development of a new intervention designed to target the close connections between young people's emotions and their interpersonal relationships. By embedding the intervention within a school setting and using a cluster-randomised design, we aim to develop and test a feasible, scalable intervention to prevent the onset of psychopathology in adolescence. TRIAL REGISTRATION: ISRCTN88585916. Trial registration date: 20/04/2023.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos Mentales , Resiliencia Psicológica , Humanos , Adolescente , Emociones , Instituciones Académicas , Ensayos Clínicos Controlados Aleatorios como Asunto
8.
Biol Psychol ; 186: 108761, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38309512

RESUMEN

Previous research suggests that the processing of internal body sensations (interoception) affects how we experience pain. Some evidence suggests that people with fibromyalgia syndrome (FMS) - a condition characterised by chronic pain and fatigue - may have altered interoceptive processing. However, extant findings are inconclusive, and some tasks previously used to measure interoception are of questionable validity. Here, we used an alternative measure - the Phase Adjustment Task (PAT) - to examine cardiac interoceptive accuracy in adults with FMS. We examined: (i) the tolerability of the PAT in an FMS sample (N = 154); (ii) if there are differences in facets of interoception (PAT performance, PAT-related confidence, and scores on the Private Body Consciousness Scale) between an FMS sample and an age- and gender-matched pain-free sample (N = 94); and (iii) if subgroups of participants with FMS are identifiable according to interoceptive accuracy levels. We found the PAT was tolerable in the FMS sample, with additional task breaks and a recommended hand posture. The FMS sample were more likely to be classified as 'interoceptive' on the PAT, and had significantly higher self-reported interoception compared to the pain-free sample. Within the FMS sample, we identified a subgroup who demonstrated very strong evidence of being interoceptive, and concurrently had lower fibromyalgia symptom impact (although the effect size was small). Conversely, self-reported interoception was positively correlated with FMS symptom severity and impact. Overall, interoception may be an important factor to consider in understanding and managing FMS symptoms. We recommend future longitudinal work to better understand associations between fluctuating FMS symptoms and interoception.


Asunto(s)
Fibromialgia , Interocepción , Adulto , Humanos , Fibromialgia/complicaciones , Fibromialgia/diagnóstico , Concienciación , Dolor , Fatiga , Frecuencia Cardíaca
10.
Cortex ; 171: 308-318, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38070386

RESUMEN

Self-related processing is thought to be altered in autism, with several studies reporting that autistic individuals show a diminished neural response relative to neurotypicals for their own name and face. However, evidence remains scarce and is mostly based on event-related potential studies. Here, we used EEG to measure the neural activity of autistic adults (20 for faces, 27 for names) and neurotypical adults (24 for faces, 25 for names) while they were watching rapidly alternating faces and names, through a relatively new technique called Fast Periodic Visual Stimulation. We presented strangers' faces or names at a base frequency of 5.77 Hz, while one's own, a close other's, and a specific stranger's face/name was presented at an oddball frequency of 1.154 Hz. The neurotypical group showed a significantly greater response to their own face than both close other and stranger faces, and a greater response for close other than for stranger faces. In contrast, in the autism group, own and close other faces showed stronger responses than the stranger's face, but the difference between own and close other faces was not significant in a bilateral parieto-occipital cluster. No group differences in the enhanced response to familiar names were found. These results replicate and extend results obtained using traditional electroencephalographic techniques which suggest atypical responses to self-relevant stimuli in autism.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico , Nombres , Adulto , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Electroencefalografía
11.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 77(4): 893-897, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37278595

RESUMEN

A recently published test of face perception, the Oxford Face Matching Test, asks participants to make two judgements: whether two faces are of the same individual and how perceptually similar the two faces are. In this study, we sought to determine to what extent the test can be shortened by removing the perceptual similarity judgements and whether this affects test performance. In Experiment 1, participants completed two versions of the test, with and without similarity judgements, in separate sessions in counterbalanced order. The version without similarity judgements took approximately 40% less time to complete. Performance on the matching judgements did not differ across versions and the correlation in accuracy across the two versions was comparable with the originally reported test-retest reliability value. Experiment 2 validated the version without similarity judgements against other measures, demonstrating moderate relationships with other face matching, memory, and self-report face perception measures. These data indicate that a test version without the similarity judgements can substantially reduce administration time without affecting test performance.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Autoinforme , Juicio
12.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 30(6): 2240-2249, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37312015

RESUMEN

Multiple psychological processes are required in order for a face to be recognised from memory. However, when testing face memory using tasks such as the Cambridge Face Memory Task (CFMT), it is rare for studies to attempt to account for individual differences in face perception and face matching in order to isolate variance in face memory specifically. In Study 1, the Oxford Face Matching Test (OFMT) was used to assess face matching and face perception in a large sample of participants (N = 1,112). Results revealed independent contributions of face perception and matching to CFMT performance, and these results replicated with the Glasgow Face Matching Test. In Study 2, the same procedure was used to test face perception, face matching and face memory in a group of 57 autistic adults and a matched neurotypical control group. Results revealed impaired face perception and memory in the individuals with autism, but intact face matching. Face perception may therefore act as a potential intervention target for individuals with autism who exhibit face recognition impairments.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Autístico , Reconocimiento Facial , Adulto , Humanos , Individualidad , Cognición , Proyectos de Investigación
13.
Autism Res ; 16(7): 1321-1334, 2023 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37172211

RESUMEN

Autistic people report that their emotional expressions are sometimes misunderstood by non-autistic people. One explanation for these misunderstandings could be that the two neurotypes have different internal representations of emotion: Perhaps they have different expectations about what a facial expression showing a particular emotion looks like. In three well-powered studies with non-autistic college students in the United States (total N = 632), we investigated this possibility. In Study 1, participants recognized most facial expressions posed by autistic individuals more accurately than those posed by non-autistic individuals. Study 2 showed that one reason the autistic expressions were recognized more accurately was because they were better and more intense examples of the intended expressions than the non-autistic expressions. In Study 3, we used a set of expressions created by autistic and non-autistic individuals who could see their faces as they made the expressions, which could allow them to explicitly match the expression they produced with their internal representation of that emotional expression. Here, neither autistic expressions nor non-autistic expressions were consistently recognized more accurately. In short, these findings suggest that differences in internal representations of what emotional expressions look like are unlikely to play a major role in explaining why non-autistic people sometimes misunderstand the emotions autistic people are experiencing.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Humanos , Adulto , Expresión Facial , Trastorno Autístico/psicología , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Emociones , Estudiantes
14.
Psychophysiology ; 60(6): e14262, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36740720

RESUMEN

A central tenet of many theories of emotion is that emotional states are accompanied by distinct patterns of autonomic activity. However, experimental studies of coherence between subjective and autonomic responses during emotional states provide little evidence of coherence. Crucially, previous studies investigating coherence have either adopted univariate approaches or made limited use of multivariate analytic approaches by investigating subjective and autonomic responses separately. The current study addressed this question using a multivariate dimensional approach to build a common autonomic-subjective affective space incorporating subjective responses and three different autonomic signals (heart rate, skin conductance response, and pupil diameter), measured during an emotion-inducing task, in 51 participants. Results showed that autonomic and subjective responses could be adequately described in a two-dimensional affective space. The first dimension included contributions from subjective and autonomic responses, indicating coherence, while contributions to the second dimension were almost exclusively of autonomic covariance. Thus, while there was a degree of coherence between autonomic and subjective emotional responses, there was substantial structure in autonomic responses that did not covary with subjective emotional experience. This study, therefore, contributes new insights into the relationship between subjective and autonomic emotional responses, and provides a framework for future multimodal emotion research, enabling both hypothesis- and data-driven testing.


Asunto(s)
Sistema Nervioso Autónomo , Emociones , Humanos , Frecuencia Cardíaca
15.
Neuroimage ; 269: 119881, 2023 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36702212

RESUMEN

Every day we constantly observe other people receiving rewards. Theoretical accounts posit that vicarious reward processing might be linked to people's sensitivity to internal body states (interoception) and facilitates a tendency to act prosocially. However, the neural processes underlying the links between vicarious reward processing, interoception, and prosocial behaviour are poorly understood. Previous research has linked vicarious reward processing to the anterior cingulate gyrus (ACCg) and the anterior insula (AI). Can we predict someone's propensity to be prosocial or to be aware of interoceptive signals from variability in how the ACCg and AI process rewards? Here, participants monitored rewards being delivered to themselves or a stranger during functional magnetic resonance imaging. Later, they performed a task measuring their willingness to exert effort to obtain rewards for others, and a task measuring their propensity to be aware and use interoceptive respiratory signals. Using multivariate similarity analysis, we show that people's willingness to be prosocial is predicted by greater similarity between self and other representations in the ACCg. Moreover, greater dissimilarity in self-other representations in the AI is linked to interoceptive propensity. These findings highlight that vicarious reward is linked to bodily signals in AI, and foster prosocial tendencies through the ACCg.


Asunto(s)
Altruismo , Interocepción , Humanos , Recompensa , Giro del Cíngulo , Concienciación , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética
16.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 53(12): 4731-4743, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36083393

RESUMEN

Unemployment and underemployment have consistently been shown to be higher in autistic adults relative to non-autistic adults. This may be due, in part, to a lack of workplace accommodations being made for autistic people. One factor that may contribute to employment inequalities in autistic people is differences in attitudes towards interpersonal touch. This study acts as a preliminary investigation into whether employed autistic and non-autistic participants differ in their attitudes towards touch in the workplace, and in their loneliness and wellbeing. The current dataset was drawn from a larger online survey (the Touch Test) designed to explore attitudes and experiences towards touch. We found that employed autistic participants had more negative attitudes to general, social and workplace touch relative to non-autistic participants. Autistic participants also experienced greater loneliness and reduced wellbeing. Attachment-related anxiety was the only significant predictor of wellbeing in employed autistic adults. However, attachment-related anxiety, general attitudes to touch and the role of touch in the workplace predicted wellbeing in employed non-autistic adults. With regards to loneliness, general attitudes to touch and the role of touch in the workplace predicted loneliness in autistic participants. We also replicated the finding that a greater proportion of autistic participants were unemployed relative to non-autistic participants. Collectively, this research highlights the importance of considering touch in research investigating employment, and its impact on loneliness and wellbeing, in autistic participants.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Percepción del Tacto , Adulto , Humanos , Tacto , Lugar de Trabajo
17.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 76(1): 70-83, 2023 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35045777

RESUMEN

The ability to identify others' actions and intentions, "action understanding," is crucial for successful social interaction. Under direct accounts, action understanding takes place without the involvement of inferential processes, a claim that has yet to be tested using behavioural measures. Using a dual-task paradigm, the present study aimed to establish whether the identification of others' actions and intentions depends on automatic or inferential processing, by manipulating working memory load during performance of a task designed to target the identification of actions and intentions. Experiment 1 tested a novel action understanding task targeting action identification and intention identification. This task was then combined with two working memory manipulations (cognitive: Experiment 2; perceptual: Experiment 3) to determine whether action identification and intention identification are disrupted by concurrent cognitive or perceptual load. Both action identification and intention identification were impaired by concurrent cognitive and perceptual processing, indicating that action understanding requires additional perceptual and cognitive resources. These findings contradict a direct account of action understanding.


Asunto(s)
Intención , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Humanos , Cognición
18.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 21133, 2022 12 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36477707

RESUMEN

Theory of mind (ToM), the ability to represent the mental states of oneself and others, is argued to be central to human social experience, and impairments in this ability are thought to underlie several psychiatric and developmental conditions. To examine the accuracy of mental state inferences, a novel ToM task was developed, requiring inferences to be made about the mental states of 'Targets', prior participants who took part in a videoed mock interview. Participants also made estimates of the Targets' personality traits. These inferences were compared to ground-truth data, provided by the Targets, of their true traits and mental states. Results from 55 adult participants demonstrated that trait inferences were used to derive mental state inferences, and that the accuracy of trait estimates predicted the accuracy of mental state inferences. Moreover, the size and direction of the association between trait accuracy and mental state accuracy varied according to the trait-mental state combination. The accuracy of trait inferences was predicted by the accuracy of participants' understanding of trait covariation at the population level. Findings are in accordance with the Mind-space theory, that representation of the Target mind is used in the inference of their mental states.


Asunto(s)
Teoría de la Mente , Humanos
19.
Cortex ; 157: 266-273, 2022 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36368179

RESUMEN

Individuals with developmental prosopagnosia (DP) all exhibit impairments in face memory, but the specificity of these face memory impairments is debated. One problem is that standard behavioural tasks are not able to provide independent measurement of face perception, face memory, and face matching (the decision process required to judge whether two instances of a face are of the same individual or different individuals). The present study utilised a new test of face matching, the Oxford Face Matching Test (OFMT), and a novel analysis strategy to derive these independent indices. Twenty-nine individuals with DP and the same number of matched neurotypical controls completed the OFMT, the Glasgow Face Matching Test, and the Cambridge Face Memory Test. Results revealed individuals with DP exhibit impairments in face perception, face memory and face matching. Collectively, these results suggest that face processing impairments in DP are more comprehensive than has previously been suggested.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Prosopagnosia , Humanos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Trastornos de la Memoria
20.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 141: 104864, 2022 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36087760

RESUMEN

Alexithymia, including the inability to identify and express one's own feelings, is a subclinical condition responsible for some of the socioemotional symptoms seen across a range of psychiatric conditions. The language hypothesis of alexithymia posits a language-mediated disruption in the development of discrete emotion concepts from ambiguous affective states, exacerbating the risk of developing alexithymia in language-impaired individuals. To provide a critical evaluation, a systematic review and meta-analysis of 29 empirical studies of language functioning in alexithymia was performed. A modest association was found between alexithymia and multi-domain language deficits (r = -0.14), including structural language, pragmatics, and propensity to use emotional language. A more theoretically-relevant subsample analysis comparing alexithymia levels in language-impaired and typical individuals revealed larger effects, but a limited number of studies adopted this approach. A synthesis of 11 emotional granularity studies also found an association between alexithymia and reduced emotional granularity (r = -0.10). Language impairments seem to increase the risk of alexithymia. Heterogeneous samples and methods suggest the need for studies with improved alexithymia assessments.


Asunto(s)
Síntomas Afectivos , Emociones , Síntomas Afectivos/psicología , Humanos , Lenguaje , Inventario de Personalidad
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...