Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 31
Filtrar
Más filtros












Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Vision Res ; 40(6): 677-93, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10824269

RESUMEN

The spatial arrangement of L and M cones in the human peripheral retina was estimated from red-green color naming of small test flashes (0.86 min of arc, 555 nm, constant intensity) presented at different locations (grid with 1.5 min of arc steps) centered at 17 degrees temporal eccentricity. Simulated red-green color naming ratings were generated by a model based on an ideal observer for all possible patterns of placement and relative numerosities of L and M cones, constrained by the anatomical data on the statistics of cone spacing at this retinal location. The best matching simulated performance as compared to the human observer's data determined the cone array most likely to produce that observer's color naming results. The mosaics for two color normal observers showed L and M cones randomly arrayed over this retinal region. Consequences of random cone placements for spectral sampling and color opponency are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Retina/anatomía & histología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/anatomía & histología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Humanos
2.
Vision Res ; 40(4): 359-64, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10820615

RESUMEN

Models of stereoscopic depth perception for both natural and random-dot images have focused mainly on the matching of achromatic features of binocular images. Recently, a growing body of research has investigated whether chromatic features can also contribute to the construction of stereoscopic depth. Here we present experiments yielding color-contingent depth aftereffects comparable in magnitude to those measured after adaptation to achromatic stimuli as evidence of neural mechanisms tuned to both color and depth. Furthermore, we report that the locus of the combined processing of color and depth is likely to lie beyond the site of binocular matching.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Humanos
3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 17(3): 615-27, 2000 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10708043

RESUMEN

The relative numerosity of the long-wavelength-sensitive (L) and middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones and the red-green color appearance, as assessed by means of unique yellow, are stable from fovea to midperiphery (+/- 28 deg nasotemporal). As foveal tests decrease in size, unique yellow progressively shifts toward longer wavelengths, favoring a model of red-green opponency carried by cells whose centers receive input from either L or M cones and whose surrounds receive mixed contributions from both. Individual differences in unique yellow over a 20-nm range and the relative numerosity of L and M cones can be linked by means of this model, suggesting that the relative number of L and M cones is a factor that regulates individual variations in red-green color appearance.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología , Retina/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/citología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Adulto , Color , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos
4.
Vision Res ; 38(17): 2575-89, 1998 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12116704

RESUMEN

Experiments designed to estimate the placement of L and M cones in fovea centralis of the living human eye are presented. Hyperacuity performances for two observers were measured for the full and the separate L and M cone submosaics using 2-dot chromatic stimuli on cone-selective adapting backgrounds. Simulated performances, based on an ideal observer model, were generated for all possible mosaics by varying L and M cone relative numerosity and spatial configuration. The best match between the simulated and measured performances determined the solution mosaic. Each observer's solution mosaic contained more L than M cones, randomly arrayed as assessed by statistical tests.


Asunto(s)
Fóvea Central/anatomía & histología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/anatomía & histología , Agudeza Visual , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica
5.
Perception ; 26(11): 1367-80, 1997.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9616467

RESUMEN

'Color from motion' describes the perception of a spread of subjective color over achromatic regions seen as moving. The effect can be produced in a display of multiple frames shown in quick succession, each frame consisting of a fixed, random placement of colored dots on a high-luminance white background with color assignments of some dots, but not dot locations, changing from frame to frame. Evidence is presented that the perception of apparent motion and the spread of subjective color can be activated by binocular combination of disjoint signals to each eye. The dichoptic presentation of every odd-numbered frame of the full stimulus sequence presented to one eye and, out of phase, every even-numbered frame to the other eye produces a compelling perception of color from motion equal to that seen with the full sequence presented to each eye alone. This is consistent with the idea that color from motion is regulated in sites at or beyond the convergence of monocular pathways. When the background field in the stimulus display is of low luminance, an amodally complete object, fully colored and matching the dots defining the moving region in hue and saturation, is seen to move behind a partially occluding screen. Observers do not perceive such an object in still view. Hence, color from motion can be used by the visual system to produce amodal completion, which suggests that it may play a role in enhancing the visibility of camouflaged objects.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Percepción de Movimiento , Ilusiones Ópticas , Humanos , Visión Ocular
6.
Perception ; 26(11): 1381-96, 1997.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9616468

RESUMEN

'Color from motion' describes the perception of a spread of subjective color over achromatic regions seen as moving. The effect is produced with a stimulus display consisting of colored dots, randomly placed upon a white field, with dots in the test region differing in both chromaticity and luminance from those in the surround. Evidence is presented suggesting that color from motion may be regulated by mechanisms different from those for contour formation and color contrast. (1) Results based on ratings show that, in the absence of luminance differences between the dots in the test and those in the surround regions, chromaticity differences alone are sufficient to produce color spread from motion. As the equiluminance point is approached, subjective color spread is seen despite a reduction in the strength of the subjective contour. Thus, contour formation is not likely to be a prerequisite for color from motion. (2) Color matches show that the hue and saturation of the subjective color spread are determined largely by the chromaticity and the luminance of the dots in the test region, not by those of the dots in the surround for the values explored. This suggests that color from motion may arise in sites distinct from those responsible for the regulation of color contrast.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Percepción de Movimiento , Ilusiones Ópticas , Color , Humanos , Luz
7.
Percept Psychophys ; 57(6): 761-77, 1995 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7651801

RESUMEN

We introduce and explore a color phenomenon which requires the prior perception of motion to produce a spread of color over a region defined by motion. We call this motion-induced spread of color dynamic color spreading. The perception of dynamic color spreading is yoked to the perception of apparent motion: As the ratings of perceived motion increase, the ratings of color spreading increase. The effect is most pronounced if the region defined by motion is near 1 degree of visual angle. As the luminance contrast between the region defined by motion and the surround changes, perceived saturation of color spreading changes while perceived hue remains roughly constant. Dynamic color spreading is sometimes, but not always, bounded by a subjective contour. We discuss these findings in terms of interactions between color and motion pathways.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Percepción de Movimiento , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Humanos
8.
Vision Res ; 32(5): 879-88, 1992 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1604856

RESUMEN

We present psychophysically-based estimates of the relative numbers of long-wavelength-sensitive (L) and middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones in the parafovea of three color-normal trichromats. Using methods previously applied to the fovea centralis, we obtained estimates of the relative numbers of L and M cones at retinal eccentricities of 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 3.0, and 4.0 deg along the horizontal meridian of the temporal retina. Results for three observers indicate that the L to M cone ratio remains approximately invariant from the fovea to 4.0 deg eccentricity, with a mean ratio near 2:1.


Asunto(s)
Células Fotorreceptoras/citología , Retina/citología , Recuento de Células , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Femenino , Fóvea Central/citología , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Espectrofotometría
9.
Vision Res ; 30(5): 693-7, 1990.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2378062

RESUMEN

We present new psychophysical estimates of the size of the rod pool for bleaching adaptation in the human retina. We estimate that at 5 deg nasal eccentricity in the human retina the size of the adaptation pool for rods is between 5 and 7.5 min arc. This estimate is compatible with the extent of the dendritic spread of rod bipolars located in this region of the primate retina and with the area occupied by roughly 50 rods in this parafoveal region of the human retina. Thus a candidate for the site of adaptation is the bipolar cell whose receptive field is comprised of approx. 50 rods. These estimates represents the lowest measurements to date of the size of the adaptation pool for rods.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Ocular/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras/fisiología , Acomodación Ocular , Adaptación a la Oscuridad , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Umbral Sensorial , Factores de Tiempo
10.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 7(1): 106-12, 1990 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2299441

RESUMEN

Tests of additivity of the postreceptoral pathways that mediate the perception of blackness were conducted under conditions of spatial contrast. Observers increased the radiance of a surrounding annulus until a broadband (white) test center appeared completely black. Additivity tests with heterochromatic flicker photometry (HFP) and direct brightness matching were also conducted for each observer. The results indicated that the luminance level of the annulus required to induce blackness did not change with variations in spectral composition. Results consistent with additivity were also obtained for HFP, but the results from brightness matching were not consistent with additivity. The data support the view that the perception of blackness is mediated by neural mechanisms that additively combine the input of middle- and long-wave photoreceptors.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Femenino , Humanos , Luz
11.
Vision Res ; 30(11): 1603-15, 1990.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2288078

RESUMEN

We investigated the spread of bleaching adaptation for human cone vision in the central fovea and at an eccentricity of 5 deg in the nasal retina. Cone thresholds measured after adaptation to a grating bleach were compared to those measured after a uniform bleach. We conclude that the foveal and parafoveal cone systems show excellent localization of the effects of adaptation. For areas 2.5-5 min removed from the bleach, our measurement show only small sensitivity losses amounting to between 0.10 and 0.25 log unit elevation in threshold, after taking account of optical scatter.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Ocular/fisiología , Fóvea Central/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras/fisiología , Adaptación a la Oscuridad/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Luz , Dispersión de Radiación , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
12.
Vision Res ; 29(11): 1587-95, 1989.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2635482

RESUMEN

We present estimates, based on psychophysical measurements, of the density of cones in the fovea centralis of human dichromats. The estimates for a group of three protanopes and three deuteranopes (this study) were compared to the estimates of the density of cones in a group of six color normal trichromats from previous studies (Cicerone & Nerger, 1985, 1989). The results support the conclusion that the density of cones in the fovea centralis of the dichromat is comparable to that of the color normal trichomat. These results tend not to support a model of dichromacy in which a class of cones as well as the associated pigment are lost in the dichromatic eye. Instead, dichromacy appears to involve a loss of one of the three visual pigments associated with human trichromacy, with a retention of the full numbers of cones.


Asunto(s)
Defectos de la Visión Cromática/patología , Fóvea Central/patología , Mácula Lútea/patología , Células Fotorreceptoras/patología , Recuento de Células/métodos , Pruebas de Percepción de Colores , Humanos , Pigmentos Biológicos/deficiencia , Psicofísica , Pruebas de Visión/instrumentación , Pruebas de Visión/métodos
13.
Vision Res ; 29(1): 115-28, 1989.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2773329

RESUMEN

The determination of the relative numbers of different cone types in the human retina is fundamental to our understanding of visual sensitivity and color vision; yet direct measurement which provide this basic information have not previously been made for all cone types. Here we present a model which links the detection of a test light of small dimension to the number of cones contributing to detection of the light. We selectively isolated either the long-wavelength-sensitive (L) or the middle-wavelength-sensitive (M) cones, by choosing combinations of wavelengths of adapting backgrounds and tests to favor detection by the cone class of interest. Our model was applied to the detection functions measured for six color normal observers to obtain estimates of the relative numbers of L to M cones. Our estimates ranged between 1.46 and 2.36 for our observers with a mean value near two L cones for every M cone in human fovea centralis.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Fóvea Central/citología , Mácula Lútea/citología , Células Fotorreceptoras/fisiología , Femenino , Fusión de Flicker/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Modelos Neurológicos , Espectrofotometría
14.
Vision Res ; 29(3): 303-7, 1989.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2773340

RESUMEN

We measured the pupillary light reflex (PLR) in 5 pigmented, Long Evans rats (under urethan sedation) in three conditions: direct stimulation, consensual stimulation, and a control condition designed to measure the effects of stray light. The average constriction (maximal amplitude) produced by a ganzfeld stimulus delivering 1.6 log quanta absorbed per rod per sec for a duration of 3 sec was measured to be 0.78 +/- 0.07 mm for the direct PLR, 0.67 +/- 0.06 mm for the consensual PLR, and 0.07 +/- 0.029 mm for the control condition. We corrected the consensual measurement for each rat by subtracting the value of the control (stray-light induced) constriction. A comparison of the corrected consensual constriction to the direct constriction showed that, on average, the consensual constriction attained an amplitude of 78% of the direct constriction. Our findings contradict claims that the consensual pupillary light reflex is absent in rodents. Although our results are in agreement with findings showing bilateral projections of the retina to the pretectum (which subserves the pupillary light reflex) in the rat, the consensual-to-direct ratio we report is higher than might be expected from anatomical estimates of the overall proportion of uncrossed to crossed optic fibers in the rat.


Asunto(s)
Pupila/fisiología , Reflejo Pupilar , Animales , Estimulación Luminosa , Ratas , Ratas Endogámicas , Dispersión de Radiación
15.
Vision Res ; 27(6): 915-8, 1987.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3660652

RESUMEN

Hereditary retinal degeneration in the Royal College of Surgeons (RCS) strain of rat has been shown to produce extensive loss of photoreceptors and a corresponding decline of the electroretinogram, ganglion cell sensitivity, and the sensitivity of the pupillary light reflex. The behaviorally measured thresholds of RCS rats, on the other hand, are reported to be comparable to those for age-matched controls. We report here, that our own behavioral measurements show a clear difference between RCS rats and age-matched controls between four to twelve months of age. The difference in thresholds between RCS and control rats is about three long units at four months of age, and this difference progressively increases until at twelve months, we measure threshold differences of over seven log units.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Degeneración Retiniana/fisiopatología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Luz , Ratas , Degeneración Retiniana/genética , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
16.
Vision Res ; 27(6): 983-91, 1987.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3499030

RESUMEN

It is generally held that protanopes and deuteranopes see only regions of blues and yellows in the visible spectrum, with an achromatic point, called the neutral point, separating these regions. Considerations of a zone model of color vision for the dichromatic observer led us to predict that a reduced form of red/green discrimination would allow equilibrium blue judgements to be made by protanopes. We show that protanopes can make equilibrium blue determinations with as much reliability as they make neutral point settings. Our results indicate that protanopes but not deuteranopes are able to rely on a reduced form of red/green discrimination in the short wavelength part of the spectrum. Protanopes describe wavelengths longer than the neutral point as yellow. Between the neutral point and equilibrium blue, different wavelengths are described as having varying aspects of blue and green; and short of equilibrium blue they appear reddish blue. For dueteranopes, the spectrum longer than the neutral point appears yellow, and short of it, blue. The results of our experiments showing that the protanopic equilibrium blue is invariant with intensity variations, as it is in the trichromat, add support to the idea of a reduced form of red/green discrimination for protanopes. Our results also allow the evaluation of various models of protanopia.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Defectos de la Visión Cromática/fisiopatología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos
17.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 3(4): 432-6, 1986 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3701481

RESUMEN

We sought to measure the mechanisms underlying the perception of blackness in the following way. A central spot (45') of fixed luminance was surrounded by a dark ring (7.5'), and surrounding all was an annular zone (30') of light. This stimulus was presented in Maxwellian view for 0.5 sec every 3 sec. The radiance of the annulus required to make the central area (spot and ring) appear uniformly black was measured for different wavelengths (440-660 nm) of the annulus. These measurements were made for test spots that were either broadband or of wavelength 480, 500, 580, or 660 nm. In all conditions the measured spectral efficiency of induced blackness matched the inverse of the V lambda function. Using the same stimulus, we have also measured increment-threshold functions. For a fixed luminance of the spot, the radiance of the surrounding annulus required to bring the central spot to threshold was measured. These increment-threshold functions do not match the V lambda or blackness functions. Our results show that induced blackness is inversely related to the luminous efficiency function and that the spectral efficiency of induced blackness is distinct from the increment-threshold function measured under these conditions. Furthermore, blackness appears to be independent of the wavelengths of the inducing annulus as well as of the central spot. Thus these results link induced blackness to the luminance pathway and argue against the involvement of the chromatic pathways in the perception of blackness.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Femenino , Fusión de Flicker , Humanos , Psicofísica , Umbral Sensorial
18.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 1(9): 981-6, 1984 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6481505

RESUMEN

The spectral efficiency of blackness induction was measured in three normal trichromatic observers and in one deuteranomalous observer. The psychophysical task was to adjust the radiance of a monochromatic 60-120' annulus until a 45' central broadband field just turned black and its contour became indiscriminable from a dark surrounding gap that separated it from the annulus. The reciprocal of the radiance required to induce blackness with annulus wavelengths between 420 and 680 nm was used to define a spectral-efficiency function for the blackness component of the achromatic process. For each observer, the shape of this blackness-sensitivity function agreed with the spectral-efficiency function based on heterochromatic flicker photometry when measured with the same 60-120' annulus. Both of these functions matched the Commission Internationale de l'Eclairage V lambda function except at short wavelengths. Ancillary measurements showed that the latter difference in sensitivity can be ascribed to nonuniformities of preretinal absorption, since the annular field excluded the central 60' of the fovea. Thus our evidence indicates that, at least to a good first approximation, induced blackness is inversely related to the spectral-luminosity function. These findings are consistent with a model that separates the achromatic and the chromatic pathways.


Asunto(s)
Luz , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Adulto , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Iluminación , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Fotometría , Retina/fisiología
19.
Brain Res ; 300(1): 49-62, 1984 May 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6733467

RESUMEN

Extracellular microelectrode recordings from 148 single cells in the pretectum of the hooded rat were classified according to their temporal response properties to light stimulation of their retinal receptive fields. Fifty-six cells were classified as tonic-on cells, 22 cells were classified as tonic-off cells, and 53 cells were classified as phasic cells. Seventeen cells could not be assigned to one of these 3 groups. The diameters of the receptive field centers of the tonic-on pretectal cell were clustered about a mean of 31 degrees and the temporal response of these cells was sustained. Constriction of the contralateral pupil was produced by electrical stimulation through the recording electrode at sites containing tonic-on pretectal cells, but not at sites containing tonic-off pretectal cells or phasic pretectal cells. For this reason, we argue that tonic-on cells are likely to mediate constriction in the light reflex of the rat's pupil. Receptive field maps together with electrolytic marking lesions at recording and stimulation sites showed that tonic-on pretectal cells are retinotopically organized and are aggregated in a strip running from the dorso-medial tip of the pretectum to the ventro-lateral boundary. The anatomical distribution of these cells is coextensive with the region known as the pretectal olivary nucleus (PO) in the rat. Using fine microelectrodes, recordings were obtained from 27 axons presumed to be of optic origin (fibers). Of these, 14 were tonic-on, 10 were phasic, 2 were tonic-off, and 2 were unclassified. Recordings from tonic-on fibers were obtained near tonic-on pretectal cells, typically in the most dorsal light-responsive region of the pretectum. These fibers were activated by single pulse electrical stimulation of the optic chiasm. The mean receptive field center diameter of 6 tonic-on fibers was 10.1 degrees, or about a factor of 3 less than that of pretectal tonic-on cells. The mean conduction velocity of 14 tonic-on fibers was 3.1 m/s. We argue that the tonic-on cells of the PO serve to integrate signals from tonic-on center retinal ganglion cells with adjacent receptive fields to provide signals for constriction of the pupil to neurons in the oculomotor nucleus.


Asunto(s)
Mesencéfalo/fisiología , Núcleo Olivar/fisiología , Reflejo Pupilar , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Estimulación Eléctrica , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Luz , Ratas , Retina/fisiología , Campos Visuales , Vías Visuales
20.
Vision Res ; 22(9): 1163-71, 1982.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7147727

RESUMEN

The effects of retinal degeneration on the sensitivity of the retina were studied in the Royal College of Surgeons (RCS) rat by measuring the light reflex of the pupil in response to ganzfeld (full field) flashes. Light reflex thresholds were measured for animals from 32 to 683 days of age, and an age-related decrease in sensitivity of 5.2 log units (maximum) was measured. In contrast, thresholds for non-dystrophic albino controls increased only slightly during a comparable period. RCS rat thresholds increased more for short wavelength light than for long wavelength light. The end result was an altered action spectrum of the light reflex which largely, but not exclusively, reflected cone function. Even in cases of advanced degeneration the light reflex thresholds we measured showed significant input from rods. Pupiliary dark adaptation measured following ganzfeld bleaches (10%) with test stimuli of two different wavelengths revealed two mechanisms; a photopic mechanism (gamma max = 520) determined thresholds early in dark adaptation, but later a scotopic mechanism (gamma max = 500) participated in the light reflex.


Asunto(s)
Reflejo Pupilar , Degeneración Retiniana/fisiopatología , Envejecimiento , Animales , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Adaptación a la Oscuridad , Estimulación Luminosa , Fotometría , Ratas , Ratas Endogámicas , Retina/fisiopatología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...