Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 59
Filtrar
1.
J Neuroimmunol ; 393: 578396, 2024 Aug 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38908330

RESUMEN

Susac syndrome (SuS) presents with encephalopathy, visual disturbances, and hearing loss from immune-mediated microvascular occlusion. While acute SuS is well-described, long-term cognitive outcomes with current treatments are underknown. We assessed ten SuS patients treated in accordance with evidence-based guidelines using immunotherapies targeting humoral and cell-mediated pathways. Patients were followed for a median 3.6 years. Initially, cognition inversely correlated with corpus callosum lesions on MRI. All reported cognitive improvement; 5/10 patients had residual deficits in visual attention and executive function. Early, aggressive treatment was associated with good outcomes; extensive early corpus callosum lesions may identify patients at-risk of persistent cognitive deficits.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Susac , Humanos , Síndrome de Susac/complicaciones , Síndrome de Susac/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios de Seguimiento , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Adolescente , Resultado del Tratamiento
2.
Psychol Aging ; 39(3): 324-336, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38829343

RESUMEN

Judging the perspective of others often requires ignoring one's own accessible knowledge. Aging increases reliance on the most available knowledge and may decrease the adjustment of this knowledge to adopt another perspective. Using a dominant language also decreases control demands, while using a nondominant language promotes deliberation. We examined whether aging and language dominance shape the way in which individuals judge someone else's interpretation of ambiguous messages. Russian-Hebrew bilinguals (N = 237, ages 19-80) read 20 ambiguous messages and judged how a recipient would interpret them. Half of the texts contained information that suggested that the message was sincere, and half of the texts contained information that implied that the message was sarcastic. This information was available only to the participant and should not have affected the recipient's interpretation. An egocentric bias emerged in both languages since participants could not ignore their own knowledge when judging the recipient's perspective. Aging was associated with a greater bias, but the results were similar in both languages. A second study included 60 younger (ages 18-39) and 62 older (ages 60-80) Israeli-born participants, who performed the same task as well as a flanker task (i.e., judging the direction of a central arrow flanked by congruent and incongruent distractors). Age interacted with the egocentric bias, but there was no correlation between the flanker effect and perspective judgment. Thus, decreased inhibition, as measured by the flanker task, cannot account for the egocentric bias. We suggest that the findings reflect difficulty in overriding highly accessible information, especially in older age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Juicio , Multilingüismo , Humanos , Anciano , Adulto , Persona de Mediana Edad , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Envejecimiento/psicología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Adolescente , Israel , Federación de Rusia , Percepción Social
3.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37971818

RESUMEN

The solution and verification of single-digit multiplication problems vary in speed and accuracy. The current study examines whether the number of different digits in a problem accounts for this variance. In Experiment 1, 41 participants solved all 2-9 multiplication problems. In Experiment 2, 43 participants verified these problems. In Experiment 3, 26 participants solved 10 problems that differed in shared-digit network (SDN) size and matched in problem size. In Experiment 4, 24 participants verified these matched sets. Results show faster and more accurate responses to problems that include fewer different digits relative to problems with more different digits, and faster and more accurate responses to problems whose SDN is small relative to problems whose SDN is large. We thus show that the number of different digits in a problem, including the operands and the solution, determines the speed and accuracy of its solution and verification. This parsimonious account also explains why responses to five and tie problems, which include fewer different digits relative to nonfive and nontie problems, are faster and more accurate than responses to other problems. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

5.
BMC Neurol ; 23(1): 151, 2023 Apr 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37069531

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease (CJD) is the most common prion disease in humans causing a rapidly progressive neurological decline and dementia and is invariably fatal. The familial forms (genetic CJD, gCJD) are caused by mutations in the PRNP gene encoding for the prion protein (PrP). In Israel, there is a large cluster of gCJD cases, carriers of an E200K mutation in the PRNP gene, and therefore the largest population of at-risk individuals in the world. The mutation is not necessarily sufficient for the formation and accumulation of the pathological prion protein (PrPsc), suggesting that other, genetic and non-genetic factors affect the age at symptoms onset. Here we present the protocol of a cross-sectional and longitudinal natural history study of gCJD patients and first-degree relatives of gCJD patients, aiming to identify biological markers of preclinical CJD and risk factors for phenoconversion. METHODS: The study has two groups: Patients diagnosed with gCJD, and first-degree healthy relatives (HR) (both carriers and non-carriers of the E200K mutation in the PRNP gene) of patients diagnosed with gCJD. At baseline, and at the end of every year, healthy participants are invited for an "in-depth" visit, which includes a clinical evaluation, blood and urine collection, gait assessment, brain MRI, lumbar puncture (LP), and Polysomnography (PSG). At 6 months from baseline, and then halfway through each year, participants are invited for a "brief" visit, which includes a clinical evaluation, short cognitive assessment, and blood and urine collection. gCJD patients will be invited for one "in-depth" visit, similar to the baseline visit of healthy relatives. DISCUSSION: This continuous follow-up of the participants and the frequent assessments will allow early identification and diagnosis in case of conversion into disease. The knowledge generated from this study is likely to advance the understanding of the underlying clinicopathological processes that occur at the very beginning of CJD, as well as potential genetic and environmental risk factors for the development of the disease, therefore advancing the development of safe and efficient interventions. TRIAL REGISTRATION: The study is an observational study. It has registered retrospectively in https://clinicaltrials.gov/ and has been assigned an identification number NCT05746715.


Asunto(s)
Síndrome de Creutzfeldt-Jakob , Priones , Humanos , Síndrome de Creutzfeldt-Jakob/patología , Proteínas Priónicas/genética , Estudios Transversales , Estudios Longitudinales , Estudios Prospectivos , Estudios Retrospectivos , Priones/genética , Priones/metabolismo , Mutación/genética , Estudios Observacionales como Asunto
6.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 38(1): 126-130, 2023 Jan 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35905452

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: This study aimed to examine which verbal fluency task is most useful in assessing adolescents with reading disorders (RD). METHOD: Eighty-three Hebrew-speaking adolescents (ages 12-15), 42 of them with RD, completed semantic and phonemic fluency tasks, and their scores were converted to standardized scores according to population norms. RESULTS: Scores on the semantic task were similar in the RD and the control group, unlike scores on the phonemic task, which were significantly lower in the RD group. The RD group demonstrated higher semantic than phonemic scores, unlike the control group whose standardized scores on both tasks were similar. Phonemic but not semantic fluency scores predicted spelling scores within the RD group. CONCLUSIONS: Adolescents with RD have no difficulty on a semantic fluency task, but perform below expected age-matched levels on the phonemic fluency task. To document this task-difference, practitioners must administer both fluency tasks when assessing RD.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia , Conducta Verbal , Humanos , Adolescente , Niño , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Semántica , Lenguaje , Dislexia/complicaciones , Dislexia/diagnóstico , Fonética
7.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36415955

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Vocabulary scores increase until approximately age 65 years and then remain stable or decrease slightly, unlike scores on tests of other cognitive abilities that decline significantly with age. AIMS: To review the findings on ageing-related changes in vocabulary, and to discuss four methodological issues: research design; test type; measurement; and vocabulary ability as a proxy for general intelligence. MAIN CONTRIBUTION: A discussion of cross-sectional and longitudinal research designs shows that cohort membership accounts for some but not all ageing-related changes in vocabulary, and that drop-out and test-retest effects do not alter conclusions regarding these changes. Test type affects age trends in vocabulary, and if researchers use only one test, they should choose a multiple-choice synonym test. While some authors suggest that vocabulary tests do not measure the same underlying ability in younger and older adults, more research of this suggestion is needed. A brief examination of the use of vocabulary ability as a proxy for general intelligence in healthy ageing and for premorbid abilities in dementia indicates that such practice is often questionable. CONCLUSIONS: Vocabulary knowledge increases through the mid-60s regardless of measurement method. However, there is little information on how word knowledge serves other verbal skills in old age, how and when adults learn new words, or how much exposure is necessary for meanings to remain in storage for a lifetime. Research of these issues may require new methodologies, as well as novel theoretical accounts of ageing-related effects on vocabulary. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS: What is already known on this subject Unlike many cognitive abilities that decline with ageing, vocabulary knowledge continues to increase until approximately age 65, and then remains stable or decreases slightly. These findings have been replicated in different research designs and across languages. What this paper adds to existing knowledge The article presents a summary of findings on changes in vocabulary across adulthood, and a discussion of four key methodological issues: research design, test type, measurement, and the use of vocabulary ability as a proxy for general intelligence. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? To better understand changes in vocabulary knowledge across adulthood, clinicians must be aware of methodological considerations that affect the field. Such considerations have direct clinical implications regarding the choice of vocabulary tests and their use as a proxy for other abilities in both healthy older adults and in individuals with dementia.

8.
Res Aging ; 44(7-8): 531-544, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35229687

RESUMEN

This cross-sectional study examined whether views of aging (VoA) relate to subjective cognitive complaints in two separate cohorts of older adults. Ageist attitudes, attitudes to aging (psychological loss, physical change, and psychological growth), subjective age, and subjective successful aging were examined. A moderating effect of chronological age was also examined. Samples included 572 adults aged 50 or older (Sample 1; mean age = 67.63, SD = 11.39, 49.4% female) and 224 adults aged 65 or older (Sample 2; mean age = 81.50, SD = 6.61, 75.3% female). More negative VoA (higher ageist attitudes, lower psychological growth, lower physical change, older subjective age, and less successful aging) were associated with more subjective cognitive complaints after controlling for covariates. An increase in chronological age strengthened some of these associations. Findings suggest that improving dimensions of VoA may have a complementary positive effect on subjective cognitive complaints in older adults.


Asunto(s)
Ageísmo , Actitud , Cognición , Envejecimiento Cognitivo , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Ageísmo/psicología , Envejecimiento Cognitivo/psicología , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
9.
PLoS One ; 17(2): e0262856, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35108309

RESUMEN

This study examines whether age associates with entrepreneurship tendencies across the lifespan, after taking into account aspects of personality that affect entrepreneurship. Participants (N = 963) aged 18-81, including 200 actual entrepreneurs, completed questionnaires about entrepreneurship tendency, personality traits, and attachment orientations. Results show that age is associated with a reduced tendency to engage in entrepreneurial activity. However, this decline is quite limited, it weakens with age, and is absent after age 50. In addition, the negative association of age with entrepreneurial tendency is smaller in participants with above-median entrepreneurship tendency scores relative to those with below-median scores, and it disappears in actual entrepreneurs. Furthermore, most of the traits that have been previously associated with entrepreneurial tendencies, especially Openness to Experience and Extraversion, remain unchanged with age, accounting for the stability of entrepreneurial tendency over time. The results have implications for policy makers who wish to encourage older adults to engage in entrepreneurial activity.


Asunto(s)
Emprendimiento/estadística & datos numéricos , Longevidad , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Extraversión Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Neuroticismo , Personalidad , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33208042

RESUMEN

Previous research has assumed that all types of semantic knowledge are similarly affected by aging. We investigate whether knowledge of vocabulary and math concepts show comparable lifetime change. A sample of 252 participants aged 17-91 completed two multiple-choice tasks that examined knowledge of infrequent word meanings and knowledge of basic math concepts. Up to age 64, vocabulary scores improved, whereas math scores remained stable. After that age, vocabulary scores remained stable, while math scores declined. We suggest that the fact that the learning and use of infrequent vocabulary are incidental, incremental, and contextual contributes to maintenance of word knowledge into old age. In contrast, learning of basic math concepts occurs relatively early in life in an intentional manner, and both learning and use of these concepts involve constrained contexts. Thus, the nature of the acquisition and use of semantic knowledge across the lifespan affects its fate in old age.


Asunto(s)
Semántica , Vocabulario , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Humanos , Conocimiento , Aprendizaje , Matemática
11.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 77(1): 61-70, 2022 01 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34608493

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: This study examined the longitudinal relationships between subjective age (SA) and future functional status in later life, via depressive symptoms. Additionally, we assessed the role of subjective nearness to death (SNtD) as a potential moderator within these pathways. METHODS: Older adults (average age 81.14 years at T1) were interviewed once a year for 3 consecutive years (N = 224 at T1, N = 178 at T2, and N = 164 at T3). Participants reported their SA, SNtD, depressive symptoms, and functional status. Additionally, grip strength was employed as an objective measure of functional status. RESULTS: Data analysis revealed distinct pathways leading from T1 SA to T3 functional status through T2 depressive symptoms. Moreover, T1 SNtD was found to significantly moderate most of these indirect pathways, so that the mediation model of T1 SA-T2 depressive symptoms-T3 functional status was mostly significant among those who felt closer to death. DISCUSSION: The findings contribute to our understanding of the underlying mechanism through which SA predicts long-term functioning sequelae by underscoring the indirect effect of depressive symptoms. They further indicate the importance of gauging the effects of SNtD on these longitudinal relationships. Present results may further contribute to establishing an integrative model for predicting long-term functional outcomes based on older adults' earlier subjective views of aging.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Depresión/fisiopatología , Estado Funcional , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Modificador del Efecto Epidemiológico , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino
12.
Appl Neuropsychol Adult ; 29(6): 1492-1498, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33691536

RESUMEN

The current study compared the assessment of memory with a translated story recall test and its original published norms and an equivalent local test with local norms. Analyses used data from 232 individuals with memory complaints who underwent neuropsychological evaluation at an outpatient memory clinic. One group of participants completed a translated test (N = 126) and another group completed a local test (N = 106). Additionally, participants completed tasks of word list recall, picture naming, and verbal fluency, all having local norms. The results showed that raw scores on the delayed story recall test, and on all other cognitive tasks, did not differ across groups, and the cross-task correlations were significant and similar in size in both groups. Yet, there was an interaction between group and standardized tests scores, whereby the standardized scores on the translated story recall test were equivalent to population mean, whereas all other scores fell below the mean. Conversion of raw scores to the original norms indicated that the performance of individuals with memory complaints was intact, while conversion of scores on a local test to local norms revealed the expected memory impairment. The findings highlight the importance of using local tests and local norms in the assessment of memory.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Memoria , Recuerdo Mental , Humanos , Trastornos de la Memoria/diagnóstico , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
13.
Isr J Health Policy Res ; 10(1): 31, 2021 05 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33941268

RESUMEN

Neuropsychological assessment provides crucial information about cognitive, behavioral, and socioemotional functioning in medical, educational, legal, and social contexts. During the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic, the Israeli Ministry of Health initially mandated that all psychological assessments be postponed. However, as referrals to time-sensitive, high-need, and high-stakes assessments began to accumulate, it became necessary to consider remote solutions. In the current paper, we describe the considerations that affected the transition to remote activity in a prominent Israeli provider of neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation services, referring to technological and environmental conditions, cognitive requirements, and tasks, as well as to legal, regulatory, and funding issues. After discussing how assessments should be conducted to maximize feasibility and validity while minimizing risks to clients and clinicians, we propose a preliminary model for deciding whether specific referrals warrant remote administration. The model delineates key factors in decisions regarding remote assessment, emphasizing the distinct roles of the referring clinician and the neuropsychologist who conducts the assessment, and highlighting the need for collaboration between them. The abrupt need for remote assessments during the pandemic required a quick response with little preparation. The lessons learned from this process can be applied in the future, so that the need for remote services can be met with greater certainty and uniformity.


Asunto(s)
COVID-19/prevención & control , Toma de Decisiones Clínicas/métodos , Trastornos Mentales/terapia , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Telemedicina/métodos , Humanos , Israel , Pandemias , SARS-CoV-2
14.
J Alzheimers Dis ; 80(3): 1221-1229, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33646158

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Glucocerebrosidase (GBA) gene mutations and APOE polymorphisms are common in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), however their clinical impact is only partially elucidated. OBJECTIVE: To explore the clinical impact of mutations in the GBA gene and APOE polymorphisms separately and in combination, in a cohort of Ashkenazi Jewish (AJ) patients with DLB. METHODS: One hundred consecutively recruited AJ patients with clinically diagnosed DLB underwent genotyping for GBA mutations and APOE polymorphisms, and performed cognitive and motor clinical assessments. RESULTS: Thirty-two (32%) patients with DLB were carriers of GBA mutations and 33 (33%) carried an APOE ɛ4 allele. GBA mutation carriers had a younger age of onset (mean [SD] age, 67.2 years [8.9] versus 71.97 [5.91]; p = 0.03), poorer cognition as assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination (21.41 [6.9] versus 23.97 [5.18]; p < 0.005), and more severe parkinsonism as assessed with the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale motor part III (34.41 [13.49] versus 28.38 [11.21]; p = 0.01) compared to non-carriers. There were statistically significant interactions between the two genetic factors, so that patients who carried both a mild GBA mutation and the APOE ɛ4 allele (n = 9) had more severe cognitive (p = 0.048) and motor dysfunction (p = 0.037). CONCLUSION: We found a high frequency of both GBA mutations and the APOE ɛ4 allele among AJ patients with DLB, both of which have distinct effects on the clinical disease phenotype, separately and in combination.


Asunto(s)
Apolipoproteínas E/genética , Glucosilceramidasa/genética , Enfermedad por Cuerpos de Lewy/genética , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Genotipo , Heterocigoto , Humanos , Judíos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Mutación , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple
15.
Isr J Health Policy Res ; 9(1): 46, 2020 09 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32928292

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The current study examines self-reported professional practices and attitudes of Israeli neuropsychologists, in an attempt to understand how they contribute to funding of neuropsychological assessment (NPA) through the Israeli healthcare system. METHODS: Two hundred seventy-nine neuropsychologists (176 board-certified experts and 103 interns) participated in an online survey that targeted characteristics of NPA practice in Israel, attitudes toward NPA, and familiarity with healthcare referral procedures. RESULTS: Overall, 68% of respondents conducted NPA, with a smaller proportion of experts (56%) doing so than interns (88%). The most common purpose of NPA was to provide treatment recommendations, and respondents listed indications for NPA that matched indications for neuropsychological rehabilitation. Almost two thirds of respondents reported that none of the NPAs that they performed received healthcare funding. While all practitioners believed that the healthcare system should fund NPA, the majority demonstrated lack of familiarity with referral procedures. CONCLUSIONS: To increase referral rates and create effective neuropsychological services within the Israeli healthcare system, neuropsychologists should work more closely with physicians in integrated care teams. In addition, they should engage in greater advocacy activities that will emphasize the need for publicly funded NPA.


Asunto(s)
Atención a la Salud/organización & administración , Financiación de la Atención de la Salud , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Neuropsicología/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Atención a la Salud/economía , Femenino , Encuestas de Atención de la Salud , Humanos , Israel , Masculino , Trastornos Mentales/diagnóstico , Persona de Mediana Edad , Rehabilitación Neurológica , Neuropsicología/economía , Derivación y Consulta/estadística & datos numéricos
16.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 35(5): 553-561, 2020 Jul 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32129454

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The current study examines whether Israeli physicians are familiar with neuropsychological assessment (NPA) in general and with referral to NPA in particular. METHOD: In total, 274 physicians in relevant fields participated in an online survey that targeted levels of familiarity with NPA, beliefs about assessment needs, and actual referral practices. RESULTS: Israeli physicians see many patients with neuropsychological difficulties, but assess fewer patients for these difficulties by themselves. Approximately 80% of participants reported that they had heard of NPA before, but only 25% knew how to refer patients to NPA. Familiarity was greater among neurologists and neurosurgeons than among family doctors. CONCLUSIONS: Physicians had only general knowledge about NPA, with little practical understanding of how to refer patients to such a service. To increase referral rates and create consistent demand for effective neuropsychological services, neuropsychologists should engage in greater advocacy activity that will lead to clarification of referral procedures.


Asunto(s)
Competencia Clínica , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Médicos , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Derivación y Consulta , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
17.
Int J Geriatr Psychiatry ; 35(5): 581-588, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32011757

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: The aim of this study was to examine whether the discrepancy between participant and informant estimation of memory decline can predict MCI prognosis. METHODS: Analyses involved data from individuals with MCI enrolled in the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) who filled the Everyday Cognition questionnaire. Participants who underestimated (N = 112) and overestimated (N = 157) their memory decline were compared on memory tasks, brain volume, and cerebrospinal markers, at study entry and after 24 months. RESULTS: Individuals who underestimated their memory decline performed more poorly on memory tests, had smaller hippocampus volume, and greater Alzheimer's disease pathology than did individuals who overestimated their cognitive decline. Longitudinal comparisons demonstrated that individuals who underestimated their decline deteriorated more significantly in memory and in brain measures. CONCLUSIONS: Underestimation of memory decline should raise clinicians' suspicion of the existence of AD pathology in individuals with MCI.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/patología , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Disfunción Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Biomarcadores/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Disfunción Cognitiva/líquido cefalorraquídeo , Disfunción Cognitiva/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Neuroimagen , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Pronóstico
18.
J Commun Disord ; 83: 105968, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31835071

RESUMEN

Previous studies have provided inconsistent evidence concerning the association between delayed retrieval of story information from long-term memory and retrieval of words on verbal fluency tasks. The current study looks for shared retrieval mechanisms in these tasks from childhood to old age. Three-hundred and eighty participants (ages 5-91) performed tasks of story recall, digit span, phonemic fluency, and semantic fluency. Significant correlations between delayed story recall and both fluency tasks emerged in all age groups, and the contribution of delayed story recall to fluency performance remained significant when analyzing the combined effects of story recall and digit span. These findings suggest that retrieval of information through story recall and retrieval of words on fluency tasks may share similar cognitive components.


Asunto(s)
Memoria/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Semántica , Conducta Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Niño , Preescolar , Envejecimiento Cognitivo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Fonética , Adulto Joven
19.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 33(3): 279-281, 2019.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30640255

RESUMEN

Cognitive deficits beyond memory impairment, such as those affecting language production or executive functioning, can be useful in clinically distinguishing between dementia syndromes. We tested the hypothesis that Ashkenazi Jewish (AJ) patients who have dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) and carry glucocerebrosidase (GBA) mutations will have verbal fluency deficits different from those found in Alzheimer disease (AD), whereas AJ patients with DLB who have no GBA mutations will have similar deficits in verbal fluency to those found in AD. We compared performance in phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks in 44 AJ patients with DLB and 20 patients with AD, matched for age, education, and age of immigration. All groups were found to have a deficit in semantic verbal fluency. On conducting the phonemic task, patients with DLB who carried GBA mutations scored more poorly than patients with AD, whereas DLB-noncarriers performed similarly to patients with AD. We suggest that verbal fluency tasks could serve as a possible clinical marker to subtype patients with DLB, with phonemic fluency being a marker for GBA-associated DLB.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/genética , Glucosilceramidasa/genética , Judíos/genética , Enfermedad por Cuerpos de Lewy/genética , Pruebas de Estado Mental y Demencia/estadística & datos numéricos , Mutación , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Femenino , Genotipo , Humanos , Israel , Enfermedad por Cuerpos de Lewy/psicología , Masculino , Medición de la Producción del Habla/estadística & datos numéricos
20.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 74(1): 69-73, 2019 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29688534

RESUMEN

Objectives: The present study examines which factors older adults consider as important when rating their subjective nearness-to-death (SNtD), as well as the associations between corresponding variables as reported in a multidimensional questionnaire and responses on a SNtD question. In addition, we examine whether importance ratings fit or diverge from the actual associations between corresponding variables and SNtD. Method: Two hundred and seventy-two participants (average age 80.75) reported their health and functioning, their SNtD, and the importance of each of 13 preselected factors in evaluating SNtD. Results: Respondents considered physical functioning and psychological factors as the most important factors to their SNtD evaluation, and genetic factors (i.e., age, gender, parental longevity) as the least important. Ratings of importance were strongly and positively correlated with the strength of the associations between the corresponding variables and SNtD. Discussion: Older adults appear to have implicit knowledge of the factors that affect their SNtD. Yet, this knowledge is sometimes biased and does not necessarily represent variables that have been identified as related to actual longevity.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Actitud Frente a la Muerte , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Estado de Salud , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...