Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 46
Filtrar
Más filtros












Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; : 17470218241254119, 2024 May 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38684487

RESUMEN

This study investigates the self-reference effect (SRE) with an ownership memory task across several age groups, providing the first age exploration of implicit ownership memory biases from adolescence to older adulthood (N = 159). Using a well-established ownership task, participants were required to sort images of grocery items as belonging to themselves or to a fictitious unnamed Other. After sorting and a brief distractor task, participants completed a surprise one-step source memory test. Overall, there was a robust SRE, with greater source memory accuracy for self-owned items. The SRE attenuated with age, such that the magnitude of difference between self and other memory diminished into older adulthood. Importantly, these findings were not due to a deterioration of memory for self-owned items, but rather an increase in memory performance for other-owned items. Linear mixed effects analyses showed self-biases in reaction times, such that self-owned items were identified more rapidly compared with other owned items. Again, age interacted with this effect showing that the responses of older adults were slowed, especially for other-owned items. Several theoretical implications were drawn from these findings, but we suggest that older adults may not experience ownership-related biases to the same degree as younger adults. Consequently, SREs through the lens of mere ownership may attenuate with age.

2.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(3): 931-941, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38418807

RESUMEN

There is an increasing body of evidence suggesting that there are low-level perceptual processes involved in crossmodal correspondences. In this study, we investigate the involvement of the superior colliculi in three basic crossmodal correspondences: elevation/pitch, lightness/pitch, and size/pitch. Using a psychophysical design, we modulate visual input to the superior colliculus to test whether the superior colliculus is required for behavioural crossmodal congruency effects to manifest in an unspeeded multisensory discrimination task. In the elevation/pitch task, superior colliculus involvement is required for a behavioural elevation/pitch congruency effect to manifest in the task. In the lightness/pitch and size/pitch task, we observed a behavioural elevation/pitch congruency effect regardless of superior colliculus involvement. These results suggest that the elevation/pitch correspondence may be processed differently to other low-level crossmodal correspondences. The implications of a distributed model of crossmodal correspondence processing in the brain are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Colículos Superiores , Humanos , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Discriminación de la Altura Tonal/fisiología , Asociación , Psicoacústica , Orientación/fisiología
3.
Brain Sci ; 14(1)2024 Jan 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38275518

RESUMEN

Looming motion interacts with threatening emotional cues in the initial stages of visual processing. However, the underlying neural networks are unclear. The current study investigated if the interactive effect of threat elicited by angry and looming faces is favoured by rapid, magnocellular neural pathways and if exogenous or endogenous attention influences such processing. Here, EEG/ERP techniques were used to explore the early ERP responses to moving emotional faces filtered for high spatial frequencies (HSF) and low spatial frequencies (LSF). Experiment 1 applied a passive-viewing paradigm, presenting filtered angry and neutral faces in static, approaching, or receding motions on a depth-cued background. In the second experiment, broadband faces (BSF) were included, and endogenous attention was directed to the expression of faces. Our main results showed that regardless of attentional control, P1 was enhanced by BSF angry faces, but neither HSF nor LSF faces drove the effect of facial expressions. Such findings indicate that looming motion and threatening expressions are integrated rapidly at the P1 level but that this processing relies neither on LSF nor on HSF information in isolation. The N170 was enhanced for BSF angry faces regardless of attention but was enhanced for LSF angry faces during passive viewing. These results suggest the involvement of a neural pathway reliant on LSF information at the N170 level. Taken together with previous reports from the literature, this may indicate the involvement of multiple parallel neural pathways during early visual processing of approaching emotional faces.

4.
Biol Psychol ; 176: 108479, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36566011

RESUMEN

Recent evidence suggests that looming emotional faces are processed rapidly by the neural system, and that this apparent approach further interacts with emotion, causing an enhanced neural response for angry expressions. However, previous research has not demonstrated unequivocally if these effects are due to low-level visual features, or if they are indeed due to the emotional content of the stimuli. To address this question, the current study presented upright and inverted angry and neutral faces, which either expanded or contracted in size on a constant depth-cued background, such that they appeared to approach or retreat from the viewer. EEG/ERP measures were used to identify the time course of brain activity for these stimuli. The results showed that when faces were upright, both the P1 and N170 were enhanced for angry expressions, with the P1 being further increased with looming angry faces. The inversion of the faces caused an increase in both the P1 and N170 amplitudes, but no modulation was found for emotions. These findings show an early modulation of brain activity for upright looming angry faces and rule out the influence of low-level visual features as a contributing factor.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Ira/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Expresión Facial
5.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 230: 103747, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36148738

RESUMEN

The present study was designed to gain a deeper understanding of the processing of biological motion stimuli. To this end, we investigated if the inhibition of return (IoR) effect emerges in initiation times and trajectories of pointing movements to targets in left and right space where the preceding cues were pointing movement of a human model or a dot with the same biological motion. Targets were randomly presented in the same or opposite side from the direction of the motion cue. It was hypothesised that the visuomotor system should resonate with the biological motion of a dot, but that the human model should exaggerate the effect. Thus, the human model should trigger stronger attention shifts compared with the dot model and lead to more robust IoR effects in both spatial (movement) and temporal parameters of the observer's pointing responses. Initiation times and the spatial parameters (angle of the hand trajectory) of the pointing movements were analysed. Results indicate that facilitation and IoR effects triggered by human and dot stimuli did not differ. Based on these findings, it seems that the crucial feature of motion cues that generate shifts in attention is biological motion, rather than human appearance per se.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Inhibición Psicológica , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Movimiento/fisiología
6.
Biol Psychol ; 170: 108308, 2022 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35271956

RESUMEN

Although the brain is known to process threatening emotional stimuli and looming motion rapidly, little is known about how the emotion and motion interact. To address this question, two experiments were carried out which presented angry and neutral emotional faces on a depth-cued background that induced the perception of distance, or a non-cued background. Furthermore, faces either expanded or contracted in size such that they appeared to approach or recede from the viewer. EEG/ERP measures were used to identify the time course of brain activity for these looming and receding, angry and neutral emotional faces. The results of both experiments revealed that the P1 was enhanced by looming angry faces on the depth-cued background, compared to neutral approaching faces, as well as all receding faces, indicating an early interaction of emotion and motion within 100 ms of presentation. Angry expressions were also found to enhance the N170 regardless of movement. These findings suggest that processing of threat and looming motion interact at the very early stages of visual processing. Furthermore, as the modulating effect of looming motion on angry expressions only arose on the depth-cued background, the findings highlight the importance of approaching movements rather than sole increases in the retinal size of the stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados , Expresión Facial , Ira/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Emociones/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Humanos
7.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 84(3): 915-925, 2022 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35233744

RESUMEN

Stimulus statistics can induce expectations that in turn can influence multisensory perception. In three experiments, we manipulate perceptual history by biasing stimulus statistics and examined the effect of implicit expectations on the perceptual resolution of a bistable visual stimulus that is modulated by sound. First, we found a general effect of expectation such that responses were biased in line with the biased statistics and interpret this as a bias towards an implicitly expected outcome. Second, expectation did not influence the perception of all types of stimuli. In both Experiment 1 and Experiment 2, integrated audio-visual stimuli were affected by expectation but visual-only and unintegrated audio-visual stimuli were not. In Experiment 3 we examined the sensory versus interpretational effects of expectation and found that contrary to our predictions, an expectation of audio-visually integrated stimuli was associated with impaired multisensory integration compared to visual-only or unintegrated audio-visual stimuli. Our findings suggest that perceptual experience implicitly creates expectations that influence multisensory perception, which appear to be about perceptual outcomes rather than sensory stimuli. Finally, in the case of resolving perceptual ambiguity, the expectation effect is an effect on cognitive rather than sensory processes.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Motivación , Estimulación Acústica , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Sonido , Percepción Visual/fisiología
8.
Multisens Res ; : 1-21, 2021 Jun 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34139670

RESUMEN

We examine whether crossmodal correspondences (CMCs) modulate perceptual disambiguation by considering the influence of lightness/pitch congruency on the perceptual resolution of the Rubin face/vase (RFV). We randomly paired a black-and-white RFV (black faces and white vase, or vice versa) with either a high or low pitch tone and found that CMC congruency biases the dominant visual percept. The perceptual option that was CMC-congruent with the tone (white/high pitch or black/low pitch) was reported significantly more often than the perceptual option CMC-incongruent with the tone (white/low pitch or black/high pitch). However, the effect was only observed for stimuli presented for longer and not shorter durations suggesting a perceptual effect rather than a response bias, and moreover, we infer an effect on perceptual reversals rather than initial percepts. We found that the CMC congruency effect for longer-duration stimuli only occurred after prior exposure to the stimuli of several minutes, suggesting that the CMC congruency develops over time. These findings extend the observed effects of CMCs from relatively low-level feature-based effects to higher-level object-based perceptual effects (specifically, resolving ambiguity) and demonstrate that an entirely new category of crossmodal factors (CMC congruency) influence perceptual disambiguation in bistability.

10.
Psychol Res ; 85(4): 1391-1406, 2021 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32232562

RESUMEN

Recall of, and physical interaction with, self-owned items is privileged over items owned by other people (Constable et al. in Cognition 119(3):430-437, 2011; Cunningham et al. in Conscious Cognit 17(1):312-318, 2008). Here, we investigate approach (towards the item), compared with avoidance (away from the item) movements to images of self- and experimenter-owned items. We asked if initiation time and movement duration of button-press approach responses to self-owned items are associated with a systematic self-bias (overall faster responses), compared with avoidance movements, similar to findings of paradigms investigating affective evaluation of (unowned) items. Participants were gifted mugs to use, and after a few days they completed an approach-avoidance task (Chen and Bargh in Pers Soc Psychol Bull 25(2):215-224, 1999; Seibt et al. in J Exp Soc Psychol 44:713-720, 2008; Truong et al. in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 42(3), 375-385, 2016) to images of their own or the experimenter's mug, using either congruent or incongruent movement direction mappings. There was a self-bias effect for initiation time to the self-owned mug, for both congruent and incongruent mappings, and for movement duration in the congruent mapping. The effect was abolished in Experiment 2 when participants responded based on a shape on the handle rather than mug ownership. We speculate that ownership status requires conscious processing to modulate responses. Moreover, ownership status judgements and affective evaluation may employ different mechanisms.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Juicio/fisiología , Propiedad , Percepción Social , Adulto , Sesgo , Humanos , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental , Movimiento , Normas Sociales
11.
Adv Cogn Psychol ; 16(1): 1-12, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32537039

RESUMEN

Using evoked response potentials, we investigated the implicit detection of incongruity during target-source matching in pictorial metaphors of Chinese advertising. Participants saw an image of a product (the target in a visual metaphorical relationship), and then made a same-different judgment in response to a second image (the source in a visual metaphorical relationship) which was (in)congruous to the first image in terms of shape and/or function. We collected behavioral (button-press reaction time and accuracy), and neural (N270, delta and theta band activity) measures. The time-frequency analysis showed faster processing of incongruous visual information. Moreover, shape and conceptual incongruity were associated with increased N270 amplitude as well as delta (1-3 Hz) and theta (4-8 Hz) band power. Noticeably, compared with conceptual incongruity, shape incongruity evoked a larger N270 amplitude and stronger delta and theta band oscillation. In addition, the average topographical analysis revealed a frontal and central distribution of the power activity. The analysis of attitudes towards the advertising metaphor pictures also proved the supportive role played by incongruity. In conclusion, incongruity facilitates target-source matching in pictorial metaphors of Chinese advertising. The findings obtained from the study are important to metaphor designs of advertising pictures.

12.
Exp Brain Res ; 238(2): 355-367, 2020 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31925477

RESUMEN

We investigated whether embodied ownership is evident in early childhood. To do so, we gifted a drinking bottle to children (aged 24-48 months) to use for 2 weeks. They returned to perform reach-grasp-lift-replace actions with their own or the experimenter's bottle while we recorded their movements using motion capture. There were differences in motor interactions with self- vs experimenter-owned bottles, such that children positioned self-owned bottles significantly closer to themselves compared with the experimenter's bottle. Age did not modulate the positioning of the self-owned bottle relative to the experimenter-owned bottle. In contrast, the pattern was not evident in children who selected one of the two bottles to keep only after the task was completed, and thus did not 'own' it during the task (Experiment 2). These results extend similar findings in adults, confirming the importance of ownership in determining self-other differences and provide novel evidence that object ownership influences sensorimotor processes from as early as 2 years of age.


Asunto(s)
Factores de Edad , Cognición/fisiología , Propiedad , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
13.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 82(3): 1099-1111, 2020 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31414364

RESUMEN

Biological motion is salient to the human visual and motor systems and may be intrinsic to the perception of animacy. Evidence for the salience of visual stimuli moving with trajectories consistent with biological motion comes from studies showing that such stimuli can trigger shifts of attention in the direction of that motion. The present study was conducted to determine whether or not top-down beliefs about animacy can modify the salience of a nonbiologically moving stimulus to the visuomotor system. A nonpredictive cuing task was used in which a white dot moved from a central location toward a left- or right-sided target placeholder. The target randomly appeared at either location 200, 600, or 1,300 ms after the motion onset. Five groups of participants experienced different stimulus conditions: (1) biological motion, (2) inverted biological motion, (3) nonbiological motion, (4) animacy belief (paired with nonbiological motion), and (5) computer-generated belief (paired with nonbiological motion). Analysis of response times revealed that the motion in the biological motion and animacy belief groups, but not in the inverted and nonbiological motion groups, affected processing of the target information. These findings indicate that biological motion is salient to the visual system and that top-down beliefs regarding the animacy of the stimulus can tune the visual and motor systems to increase the salience of nonbiological motion.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción de Movimiento , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Movimiento (Física) , Estimulación Luminosa
14.
J Vis ; 19(5): 7, 2019 05 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31059568

RESUMEN

Previous research suggests that cognitive factors acting in a top-down manner influence the perceptual interpretation of ambiguous stimuli. To examine the temporal unfolding of these influences as a perceptual decision evolves, we have implemented a modified version of the stream-bounce display. Our novel approach allows us to track responses to stream-bounce stimuli dynamically over the entire course of the motion sequence rather than collecting a subjective report after the fact. Using a trackpad, we had participants control a cursor to track a stream-bounce target actively from start to end and measured tracking speed throughout as the dependent variable. Our paradigm replicated the typical effect of visual-only displays being associated with a streaming bias and audiovisual displays with a bouncing bias. Our main finding is a significant behavioral change preceding a perceptual decision that then predicts that decision. Specifically, for trials in which the sound was presented, tracking speeds were significantly slower starting 500 ms before the point of coincidence and presentation of the sound for bounce compared to stream responses. We suggest that behavioral response may reflect a cognitive expectation of a perceptual outcome that then biases action and the interpretation of sensory input to favor that forthcoming percept in a manner consistent with both the predictive-coding and common-coding theoretical frameworks. Our approach provides a novel behavioral corroboration of recent imaging studies that are suggestive of early brain activity in perception and action.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento (Física) , Estimulación Luminosa , Sonido , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 81(5): 1609-1623, 2019 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30697648

RESUMEN

We tested the sensory versus decisional origins of two established audiovisual crossmodal correspondences (CMCs; lightness/pitch and elevation/pitch), applying a signal discrimination paradigm to low-level stimulus features and controlling for attentional cueing. An audiovisual stimulus randomly varied along two visual dimensions (lightness: black/white; elevation: high/low) and one auditory dimension (pitch: high/low), and participants discriminated either only lightness, only elevation, or both lightness and elevation. The discrimination task and the stimulus duration varied between subjects. To investigate the influence of crossmodal congruency, we considered the effect of each CMC (lightness/pitch and elevation/pitch) on the sensitivity and criterion of each discrimination as a function of stimulus duration. There were three main findings. First, discrimination sensitivity was significantly higher for visual targets paired congruently (compared with incongruently) with tones while criterion was unaffected. Second, the sensitivity increase occurred for all stimulus durations, ruling out attentional cueing effects. Third, the sensitivity increase was feature specific such that only the CMC that related to the feature being discriminated influenced sensitivity (i.e. lightness congruency only influenced lightness discrimination and elevation congruency only influenced elevation discrimination in the single and dual task conditions). We suggest that these congruency effects reflect low-level sensory processes.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Sensación/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
16.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(7): 1589-1600, 2019 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30282529

RESUMEN

Previous studies report that viewing exaggerated, high-lifting reaches (versus direct reaches) primes higher vertical deviation in wrist trajectory in the observer's subsequent reaches (trajectory priming), but it is unclear to what extent this effect depends upon task instructions relevant to top-down attention. In two experiments, participants were instructed to gaze at a dot presented on a large monitor for a colour-change go signal that cued them to execute a direct reach to a target. In the background, the monitor also displayed life-sized films of a human model. The films were of the model either remaining still or reaching to grasp a target with either a direct trajectory or an exaggerated, high-lifting trajectory. When the dot traced the human model's wrist throughout her movement, a robust trajectory priming effect emerged. When the dot remained stationary in a central location but the human model reached in the background, the human model's trajectory did not alter the participants' trajectories. Finally, when the dot traced exaggerated and direct trajectories and the human model remained stationary, the dot's movement produced an attenuated, non-significant trajectory priming effect. These findings show that top-down attentional factors modulate trajectory priming. In addition, a moving non-human stimulus does not produce the same degree of action priming when contextual factors make salient its independence of human agency and/or intention.


Asunto(s)
Función Ejecutiva , Actividad Motora , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto , Atención , Técnicas de Observación Conductual , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Fuerza de la Mano , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 237(2): 351-361, 2019 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30411222

RESUMEN

Preference for use of either the left or right hand ('handedness') has been linked with modulations of perception and sensory processing-both of space and the body. Here we ask whether multisensory integration of bodily information also varies as a function of handedness. We created a spatial disparity between visual and somatosensory hand position information using the rubber hand illusion, and use the magnitude of illusory shifts in hand position (proprioceptive 'drift') as a tool to probe the weighted integration of multisensory information. First, we found drift was significantly reduced when the illusion was performed on the dominant vs. non-dominant hand. We suggest increased manual dexterity of the dominant hand causes greater representational stability and thus an increased resistance to bias by the illusion induction. Second, drift was generally greatest when the hand was in its habitual action space (i.e., near the shoulder of origin), compared to when it laterally displaced towards, or across the midline. This linear effect, however, was only significant for the dominant hand-in both left- and right-handed groups. Thus, our results reveal patterns of habitual hand action modulate drift both within a hand (drift varies with proximity to action space), and between hands (differences in drift between the dominant and non-dominant hands). In contrast, we were unable to find conclusive evidence to support, or contradict, an overall difference between left- and right-handers in susceptibility to RHI drift (i.e., total drift, collapsed across hand positions). In sum, our results provide evidence that patterns of daily activity-and the subsequent patterns of sensory input-shape multisensory integration across space.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
18.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(4): 770-782, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28345944

RESUMEN

Previous research suggests integration of visual and somatosensory inputs is enhanced within reaching (peripersonal) space. In such experiments, somatosensory inputs are presented on the body while visual inputs are moved relatively closer to, or further from the body. It is unclear, therefore, whether enhanced integration in "peripersonal space" is truly due to proximity of visual inputs to the body space, or, simply the distance between the inputs (which also affects integration). Using a modified induction of the rubber hand illusion, here we measured proprioceptive drift as an index of visuosomatosensory integration when distance between the two inputs was constrained, and absolute distance from the body was varied. Further, we investigated whether integration varies with proximity of inputs to the habitual action space of the arm-rather than the actual arm itself. In Experiment 1, integration was enhanced with inputs proximal to habitual action space, and reduced with lateral distance from this space. This was not attributable to an attentional or perceptual bias of external space because the pattern of proprioceptive drift was opposite for left and right hand illusions, that is, consistently maximal at the shoulder of origin (Experiment 2). We conclude that habitual patterns of action modulate visuosomatosensory integration. It appears multisensory integration is modulated in locations of space that are functionally relevant for behavior, whether an actual body part resides within that space or not. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Espacio Personal , Propiocepción/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
19.
PLoS One ; 12(1): e0170542, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28125635

RESUMEN

We examined how factors related to the internal representation of the hands (handedness and grasping affordances) influence the distribution of visuospatial attention near the body. Left and right handed participants completed a covert visual cueing task, discriminating between two target shapes. In Experiment 1, participants responded with either their dominant or non-dominant hand. In Experiment 2, the non-responding hand was positioned below one of two target placeholders, aligned with the shoulder. In Experiment 3 the near-monitor hand was positioned under the placeholder in the opposite region of hemispace, crossed over the body midline. For Experiments 2 & 3, in blocked trials the palmar and back-of hand surfaces were directed towards the target placeholder such that targets appeared towards either the graspable or non-graspable space of the hand respectively. In Experiment 2, both left and right handers displayed larger accuracy cueing effects for targets near versus distant from the graspable space of the right hand. Right handers also displayed larger response time cueing effects for objects near the graspable versus non-graspable region of their dominant hand but not for their non-dominant hands. These effects were not evident for left-handers. In Experiment 3, for right handers, accuracy biases for near hand targets were still evident when the hand was crossed over the body midline, and reflected hand proximity but not functional orientation biases. These findings suggest that biased visuospatial attention enhances object identity discrimination near hands and that these effects are particularly enhanced for right-handers.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
20.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 70(9): 1892-1908, 2017 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27434754

RESUMEN

We examined how action goals influence the distribution of visuospatial attention near the body (Experiment 1) and how the temporal relationship between distractors and targets modifies shifts in visuospatial attention (Experiment 2). Targets were light emitting diodes (LEDs) in the left and right hemispace of a visual display. Following left or right target illumination, participants reached to point-to or grasp target object in blocked trials. Coincident with target onset, a distractor LED illuminated in the same or opposite hemispace between the initiation point and target, or no distractor appeared. In Experiment 1, during grasping there was a larger temporal interference effect (slower reach initiation) than with pointing. When grasping versus pointing, participants deviated more towards same-side distractors and away from opposite-side distractors. In Experiment 2, distractors onset 200ms prior to (-200-ms), coincident with (0 ms), or 200ms following (+200 ms) the target. For both reach types, -200-ms distractors had greater onset temporal interference than 0 ms and +200-ms distractors. For grasping, +200 ms distractors had larger temporal interference than 0 ms distractors. For -200-ms, reach trajectories deviated more towards opposite-side distractors and away from same-side distractors, the reverse of the pattern for 0 ms and +200-ms distractors.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Fuerza de la Mano , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Análisis de Varianza , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Rango del Movimiento Articular , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Factores de Tiempo , Percepción del Tiempo , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...