Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 16 de 16
Filtrar
Más filtros












Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Assist Reprod Genet ; 40(3): 617-626, 2023 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36652117

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: While delayed parenthood is increasing worldwide, the effect of paternal age on in vitro fertilization (IVF) outcomes remains unclear. The egg donation model appears to be relevant to studying the independent impact of paternal age on clinical outcome, but the available studies are heterogeneous and contradictory. This systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to assess the relationship between paternal age and live birth rate (LBR) in egg donation cycles. METHODS: A systematic search of the literature was conducted in PubMed, Embase, and the Cochrane Library from inception to June 30, 2021. All studies on egg donation cycles where LBR is reported according to male age were included. Study selection, bias assessment, and data extraction were performed by two independent reviewers according to the Cochrane methods. RESULTS: Eleven studies involving 10,527 egg donation cycles were finally included. The meta-analysis showed a slight but significant and linear decrease in LBR with increasing paternal age (estimate - 0.0055; 95% CI (- 0.0093; - 0.0016), p = 0.006), with low heterogeneity (I2 = 25%). No specific threshold was identified. A similar trend toward decreased clinical pregnancy rate with advancing paternal age was found but did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.07). CONCLUSION: This meta-analysis demonstrates that increasing paternal age is associated with a slight but significant and linear decrease in the live birth rate in egg donation cycles, with no apparent threshold effect. Although this requires further confirmation, this information is important for counseling men who are considering delayed childbearing.


Asunto(s)
Tasa de Natalidad , Edad Paterna , Embarazo , Femenino , Masculino , Humanos , Índice de Embarazo , Fertilización In Vitro/métodos , Oocitos , Nacimiento Vivo/epidemiología , Estudios Retrospectivos , Donación de Oocito/métodos
2.
Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) ; 13: 892753, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35757393

RESUMEN

Progesterone plays a key role in implantation. Several studies reported that lower luteal progesterone levels might be related to decreased chances of pregnancy. This systematic review was conducted using appropriate key words, on MEDLINE, EMBASE, and the Cochrane Library, from 1990 up to March 2021 to assess if luteal serum progesterone levels are associated with ongoing pregnancy (OP) and live birth (LB) rates (primary outcomes) and miscarriage rate (secondary outcome), according to the number of corpora lutea (CLs). Overall 2,632 non-duplicate records were identified, of which 32 relevant studies were available for quantitative analysis. In artificial cycles with no CL, OP and LB rates were significantly decreased when the luteal progesterone level falls below a certain threshold (risk ratio [RR] 0.72; 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.62-0.84 and 0.73; 95% CI 0.59-0.90, respectively), while the miscarriage rate was increased (RR 1.48; 95% CI 1.17-1.86). In stimulated cycles with several CLs, the mean luteal progesterone level in the no OP and no LB groups was significantly lower than in the OP and LB groups [difference in means 68.8 (95% CI 45.6-92.0) and 272.4 (95% CI 10.8-533.9), ng/ml, respectively]. Monitoring luteal serum progesterone levels could help in individualizing progesterone administration to enhance OP and LB rates, especially in cycles without corpus luteum. Systematic Review Registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=139019, identifier 139019.


Asunto(s)
Aborto Espontáneo , Progesterona , Tasa de Natalidad , Cuerpo Lúteo , Femenino , Humanos , Fase Luteínica , Embarazo , Índice de Embarazo
4.
Reprod Biomed Online ; 40(4): 525-529, 2020 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32201114

RESUMEN

RESEARCH QUESTION: What is the real-world effectiveness of Fertistartkit® in women undergoing assisted reproductive technology (ART)? DESIGN: Retrospective cohort study including anonymized data of women undergoing ovarian stimulation for ART with Fertistartkit between April 2016 and November 2017 and follow-up of clinical outcomes up to February 2018. Data were collected from the electronic patient databases of 12 French ART centres. The main outcome was number of oocytes retrieved. All data were categorized according to female age (<25, 25-29, 30-34, 35-37, 38-39 and >39 years). RESULTS: A total of 1006 cycles from 914 women treated with Fertistartkit were included. At the time of first ovarian stimulation in the study, women were 34.9 ± 5.0 years old, with a median body mass index of 22.7 kg/m². Couples had been infertile for more than 4 years, with all patterns of causes of infertility. Ovarian stimulation was started with a median dose of 300 IU (interquartile range [IQR]: 150-300 IU) of Fertistartkit for 10 days (IQR: 9-11 days), so a median total dose of 2700 IU (IQR: 1800-3300 IU). The mean number of oocytes retrieved per cycle was 9.5 ± 6.8, and the mean number of mature oocytes per cycle was 7.4 ± 5.5. The obtained ongoing pregnancy per started cycle was 26.0% (95% confidence interval [CI]: 24.1-27.9) and the obtained ongoing pregnancy per puncture was 27.0% (95% CI: 25.0-29.0). CONCLUSIONS: This is the first cohort to describe Fertistartkit treatment management in real-life conditions. The real-world data show that Fertistartkit is an effective option for ovarian stimulation.


Asunto(s)
Fertilización In Vitro/métodos , Recuperación del Oocito , Inducción de la Ovulación/métodos , Técnicas Reproductivas Asistidas , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Embarazo , Índice de Embarazo , Estudios Retrospectivos , Resultado del Tratamiento , Adulto Joven
5.
PLoS One ; 12(9): e0184822, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28922421

RESUMEN

The development of novel therapeutics to prevent cognitive decline of Alzheimer's disease (AD) is facing paramount difficulties since the translational efficacy of rodent models did not resulted in better clinical results. Currently approved treatments, including the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor donepezil (DON) and the N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist memantine (MEM) provide marginal therapeutic benefits to AD patients. There is an urgent need to develop a predictive animal model that is phylogenetically proximal to humans to achieve better translation. The non-human primate grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus) is increasingly used in aging research, but there is no published results related to the impact of known pharmacological treatments on age-related cognitive impairment observed in this primate. In the present study we investigated the effects of DON and MEM on sleep-deprivation (SD)-induced memory impairment in young and aged male mouse lemurs. In particular, spatial memory impairment was evaluated using a circular platform task after 8 h of total SD. Acute single doses of DON or MEM (0.1 and 1mg/kg) or vehicle were administered intraperitoneally 3 h before the cognitive task during the SD procedure. Results indicated that both doses of DON were able to prevent the SD-induced deficits in retrieval of spatial memory as compared to vehicle-treated animals, both in young and aged animals Likewise, MEM show a similar profile at 1 mg/kg but not at 0.1mg/kg. Taken together, these results indicate that two widely used drugs for mitigating cognitive deficits in AD were partially effective in sleep deprived mouse lemurs, which further support the translational potential of this animal model. Our findings demonstrate the utility of this primate model for further testing cognitive enhancing drugs in development for AD or other neuropsychiatric conditions.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/efectos de los fármacos , Indanos/farmacología , Memantina/farmacología , Trastornos de la Memoria/tratamiento farmacológico , Piperidinas/farmacología , Privación de Sueño/tratamiento farmacológico , Memoria Espacial/efectos de los fármacos , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/complicaciones , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/tratamiento farmacológico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/fisiopatología , Animales , Cheirogaleidae , Modelos Animales de Enfermedad , Donepezilo , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Privación de Sueño/complicaciones , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología
6.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 8: 446, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25620921

RESUMEN

Owing to a similar cerebral neuro-anatomy, non-human primates are viewed as the most valid models for understanding cognitive deficits. This study evaluated psychomotor and mnesic functions of 41 young to old mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). Psychomotor capacities and anxiety-related behaviors decreased abruptly from middle to late adulthood. However, mnesic functions were not affected in the same way with increasing age. While results of the spontaneous alternation task point to a progressive and widespread age-related decline of spatial working memory, both spatial reference and novel object recognition (NOR) memory tasks did not reveal any tendency due to large inter-individual variability in the middle-aged and old animals. Indeed, some of the aged animals performed as well as younger ones, whereas some others had bad performances in the Barnes maze and in the object recognition test. Hierarchical cluster analysis revealed that declarative-like memory was strongly impaired only in 7 out of 25 middle-aged/old animals. These results suggest that this analysis allows to distinguish elder populations of good and bad performers in this non-human primate model and to closely compare this to human aging.

7.
Oxid Med Cell Longev ; 2013: 187301, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23983895

RESUMEN

In several species, resveratrol, a polyphenolic compound, activates sirtuin proteins implicated in the regulation of energy balance and biological clock processes. To demonstrate the effect of resveratrol on clock function in an aged primate, young and aged mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) were studied over a 4-week dietary supplementation with resveratrol. Spontaneous locomotor activity and daily variations in body temperature were continuously recorded. Reduction in locomotor activity onset and changes in body temperature rhythm in resveratrol-supplemented aged animals suggest an improved synchronisation on the light-dark cycle. Resveratrol could be a good candidate to restore the circadian rhythms in the elderly.


Asunto(s)
Temperatura Corporal/efectos de los fármacos , Actividad Motora/efectos de los fármacos , Estilbenos/farmacología , Animales , Cheirogaleidae , Ritmo Circadiano/efectos de los fármacos , Metabolismo Energético/efectos de los fármacos , Femenino , Masculino , Resveratrol
8.
PLoS One ; 8(5): e64493, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23717620

RESUMEN

A bulk of studies in rodents and humans suggest that sleep facilitates different phases of learning and memory process, while sleep deprivation (SD) impairs these processes. Here we tested the hypothesis that SD could alter spatial learning and memory processing in a non-human primate, the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus), which is an interesting model of aging and Alzheimer's disease (AD). Two sets of experiments were performed. In a first set of experiments, we investigated the effects of SD on spatial learning and memory retrieval after one day of training in a circular platform task. Eleven male mouse lemurs aged between 2 to 3 years were tested in three different conditions: without SD as a baseline reference, 8 h of SD before the training and 8 h of SD before the testing. The SD was confirmed by electroencephalographic recordings. Results showed no effect of SD on learning when SD was applied before the training. When the SD was applied before the testing, it induced an increase of the amount of errors and of the latency prior to reach the target. In a second set of experiments, we tested the effect of 8 h of SD on spatial memory retrieval after 3 days of training. Twenty male mouse lemurs aged between 2 to 3 years were tested in this set of experiments. In this condition, the SD did not affect memory retrieval. This is the first study that documents the disruptive effects of the SD on spatial memory retrieval in this primate which may serve as a new validated challenge to investigate the effects of new compounds along physiological and pathological aging.


Asunto(s)
Cheirogaleidae/fisiología , Aprendizaje , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología , Animales , Electroencefalografía , Masculino
9.
Behav Brain Res ; 235(2): 280-6, 2012 Dec 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22921374

RESUMEN

The data are inconsistent about the ability of dietary omega-3 fatty acids to prevent age-associated cognitive decline. Indeed, most clinical trials have failed to demonstrate a protective effect of omega-3 fatty acids against cognitive decline, and methodological issues are still under debate. In contrast to human studies, experiments performed in adult rodents clearly indicate that omega-3 fatty acids supplement can improve behavioural and cognitive functions. The inconsistent observations between human and rodent studies highlight the importance of the use of non-human primate models. The aim of the present study was to address the impact of omega-3 fatty acids (given in the form of dietary fish oil) on exploratory activity, emotional status and spatial reference memory in the aged mouse lemur, a non-human primate. Aged animals fed fish oil exhibited decreased exploratory activity, as manifested by an increase in the latency to move and a reduced distance travelled in an open-field. The fish oil-supplemented animals exhibited no change in the anxiety level, but they were more reactive to go into the dark arms of a light/dark plus-maze. In addition, we found that fish oil supplementation did not significantly improve the spatial memory performance in the Barnes maze task. This study demonstrated for the first time that a fish oil diet initiated late in life specifically modifies the exploratory behaviour without improving the spatial memory of aged non-human primates. Omega-3 fatty acid supplementation may be effective when started early in life but less effective when started at later ages.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/efectos de los fármacos , Suplementos Dietéticos , Emociones/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta Exploratoria/efectos de los fármacos , Ácidos Grasos Omega-3/administración & dosificación , Memoria/efectos de los fármacos , Adaptación Psicológica/efectos de los fármacos , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Animales , Ansiedad/dietoterapia , Cheirogaleidae , Conducta Exploratoria/fisiología , Aceites de Pescado/química , Metabolismo de los Lípidos/efectos de los fármacos , Lípidos/sangre , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/efectos de los fármacos , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta Espacial/efectos de los fármacos , Factores de Tiempo
10.
Learn Mem ; 18(6): 401-4, 2011 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21602320

RESUMEN

The mammary pheromone promotes the acquisition of novel odorants (CS1) in newborn rabbits. Here, experiments pinpoint that CS1 becomes able to support neonatal learning of other odorants (CS2). We therefore evaluated whether these first- and second-order memories remained dependent after reactivation. Amnesia induced after CS2 recall selectively blocked this memory, when recall and amnesia of CS1 left the souvenir of CS2 safe; this finding partially differed from results obtained in adult mammals. Thus, in this model of neonatal appetitive odor learning, second-order memory seems to depend on first-order memory for its formation but not for its maintenance.


Asunto(s)
Animales Recién Nacidos/psicología , Memoria/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Amnesia/psicología , Animales , Anisomicina/farmacología , Condicionamiento Operante/fisiología , Femenino , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Masculino , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Motivación/fisiología , Odorantes , Feromonas/farmacología , Inhibidores de la Síntesis de la Proteína/farmacología , Conejos , Olfato/fisiología
11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20574828

RESUMEN

Birth is part of a continuum and is a major developmental change. Newborns need to adapt rapidly to the environment in terms of physiology and behaviour, and ability to locate the maternal source of milk is vital. Mechanisms have evolved resulting in the emission of olfactory cues by the mother and the processing of these cues by the young. Here, we focus on some sensory, cognitive and behavioural strategies developed by the European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) that optimize the early development of offspring. In this species, chemosensory communication between the mother and young plays a critical role in eliciting adaptive neonatal responses. In particular, lactating females release a molecule, the mammary pheromone, which has several functional impacts. It triggers orocephalic responses involved in the quick localization of nipples and sucking. Moreover, this unconditioned signal promotes rapid appetitive learning of novel odorants, acting as a potent organizer of neonatal cognition. The mammary-pheromone-induced odour memory requires consolidation/reconsolidation processes to be maintained in the long term. Finally, as this mode of conditioning also promotes learning of mixtures of odorants, it supports investigations related to the capacity of neonatal olfaction to extract biological value from the complex environment.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación Animal , Animales Lactantes/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Glándulas Mamarias Animales/metabolismo , Feromonas/metabolismo , Olfato/fisiología , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos/fisiología , Femenino , Conducta Materna/fisiología , Conejos
12.
Behav Brain Res ; 207(2): 515-9, 2010 Mar 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19891993

RESUMEN

During the course of ontogenesis does long-lasting memory emerge progressively or abruptly, and when? To examine this question, rat pups were conditioned at different ages (3-, 10-, 12-, 15- or 18-day-old) and tested at different retention intervals: from 3 days to 1 year. Conditioned aversion memory established before 12-day-old lasts for only 1 week, but when acquired after 15 days, memory survives for more than 1 year. This defines a short temporal window of 3 days for sudden emergence of a remote memory. Our result offers a precise temporal target to explore the mechanisms involved in long maintenance of memory.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Reacción de Prevención , Condicionamiento Clásico , Memoria , Percepción del Gusto , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Animales Lactantes , Extinción Psicológica , Femenino , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Ratas , Factores de Tiempo
13.
Eur J Neurosci ; 30(10): 1923-30, 2009 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19895565

RESUMEN

The ability to form long-term memories exists very early during ontogeny; however, the properties of early memory processes, brain structures involved and underlying cellular mechanisms are poorly defined. Here, we examine the role of extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK), a member of the mitogen-activated protein kinase/ERK signaling cascade, which is crucial for adult memory, in the consolidation and reconsolidation of an early memory using a conditioned taste aversion paradigm in 3-day-old rat pups. We show that intraperitoneal injection of SL327, the upstream mitogen-activated protein kinase kinase inhibitor, impairs both consolidation and reconsolidation of early memory, leaving short-term memory after acquisition and after reactivation intact. The amnesic effect of SL327 diminishes with increasing delays after acquisition and reactivation. Biochemical analyses revealed ERK hyperphosphorylation in the amygdala but not the hippocampus following acquisition, suggesting functional activation of the amygdala as early as post-natal day 3, although there was no clear evidence for amygdalar ERK activation after reactivation. These results indicate that, despite an immature brain, the basic properties of memory and at least some of the molecular mechanisms and brain structures implicated in aversion memory share a number of similarities with the adult and emerge very early during ontogeny.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Reacción de Prevención/fisiología , Quinasas MAP Reguladas por Señal Extracelular/metabolismo , Memoria/fisiología , Gusto/fisiología , Aminoacetonitrilo/análogos & derivados , Aminoacetonitrilo/farmacología , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Reacción de Prevención/efectos de los fármacos , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología , Encéfalo/efectos de los fármacos , Encéfalo/enzimología , Activación Enzimática/efectos de los fármacos , Activación Enzimática/fisiología , Inhibidores Enzimáticos/farmacología , Femenino , Masculino , Memoria/efectos de los fármacos , Fosforilación/efectos de los fármacos , Ratas , Transducción de Señal/efectos de los fármacos , Transducción de Señal/fisiología , Gusto/efectos de los fármacos , Factores de Tiempo
14.
Learn Mem ; 16(8): 470-3, 2009 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19625578

RESUMEN

Mammary pheromone (MP)-induced odor memory is a new model of appetitive memory functioning early in a mammal, the newborn rabbit. Some properties of this associative memory are analyzed by the use of anisomycin as an amnesic agent. Long-term memory (LTM) was impaired by anisomycin delivered immediately, but not 4 h after either acquisition or reactivation. Thus, the results suggest that this form of neonatal memory requires both consolidation and reconsolidation. By extending these notions to appetitive memory, the results reveal that consolidation and reconsolidation processes are characteristics of associative memories of positive events not only in the adult, but also in the newborn.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Apetitiva/fisiología , Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Feromonas/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Anisomicina/farmacología , Conducta Apetitiva/efectos de los fármacos , Aprendizaje por Asociación/efectos de los fármacos , Encéfalo/efectos de los fármacos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Condicionamiento Clásico/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Inhibidores de la Síntesis de la Proteína/farmacología , Conejos , Reconocimiento en Psicología/efectos de los fármacos
15.
Behav Brain Res ; 202(2): 278-84, 2009 Sep 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19463713

RESUMEN

Ontogenetic modification of an early memory is relatively poorly understood. And an important question is whether the memory output is more determined by the age at acquisition or at retention? Here we explore the expression of odor-shock conditioning in the rat pup. Acquisition at post-natal day 6 (P6) leads to an approach response and at post-natal day 12 (P12) to an avoidance response when the retention test is 24h later. In both cases, anisomycin injected immediately post-acquisition induced a retrograde amnesia. Controls show that, in either case, short-term memory measured 4h after acquisition is not impaired and that anisomycin given after a 4h delay has no effect. Thus, at the two ages, memory involves a consolidation process. The main result is the spontaneous reversal of the conditioned response from approach acquired at P6 to avoidance when tested at P13. This phenomenon is robust as it is observed in three conditions. Moreover, amnesia induced at P6 is maintained at P13. Results are discussed in terms of maturation and/or competition of the memory traces.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento , Reacción de Prevención , Memoria , Amnesia Retrógrada/inducido químicamente , Análisis de Varianza , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Anisomicina/administración & dosificación , Reacción de Prevención/efectos de los fármacos , Condicionamiento Clásico/efectos de los fármacos , Electrochoque , Femenino , Masculino , Aprendizaje por Laberinto/efectos de los fármacos , Memoria/efectos de los fármacos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/efectos de los fármacos , Odorantes , Inhibidores de la Síntesis de la Proteína/administración & dosificación , Ratas , Ratas Wistar , Factores de Tiempo
16.
Learn Mem ; 15(6): 434-42, 2008 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18519544

RESUMEN

The temporal dynamics of consolidation and reconsolidation of taste/odor aversion memory are evaluated during rat pup growth at postnatal days 3, 10, and 18. This is assessed through the temporal gradients of efficacy of a protein synthesis inhibitor (anisomycin) in inducing amnesia after either acquisition (consolidation) or reactivation (reconsolidation). The results show a progressive reduction with age of the delay during which the inhibitor is able to induce amnesia. Control experiments rule out a reduction of anisomycin efficacy due to blood brain barrier growth or decrease in protein synthesis inhibition. Thus, these results present the first evidence that the protein synthesis-dependent phase of memory stabilization requires less time with age. This decrease occurs in parallel for consolidation and reconsolidation. Such changes in the dynamics of memory processing could contribute to the cognitive improvement associated with development.


Asunto(s)
Anisomicina/farmacología , Memoria/efectos de los fármacos , Memoria/fisiología , Inhibidores de la Síntesis de la Proteína/farmacología , Factores de Edad , Amnesia/inducido químicamente , Animales , Animales Recién Nacidos , Anisomicina/administración & dosificación , Anisomicina/efectos adversos , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Relación Dosis-Respuesta a Droga , Inhibidores de la Síntesis de la Proteína/administración & dosificación , Inhibidores de la Síntesis de la Proteína/efectos adversos , Ratas , Factores de Tiempo , Destete
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...