Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 170
Filtrar
Más filtros












Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Cognition ; 251: 105916, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39128324

RESUMEN

Despite our wealth of experience with our bodies, our perceptions of our body size are far from veridical. For example, when estimating the relative proportions of their body part lengths, using the hand as a metric, individuals tend to exhibit systematic distortions which vary across body parts. Whilst extensive research with healthy populations has focused on perceptions of body part length, less is known about perceptions of the width of individual body parts and the various components comprising these representations. Across four experiments, representations of the relative proportions of body part width were investigated for both the self and other, and when using both the hand, or a hand-sized stick as the metric. Overall, we found distortions in the perceived width of body parts; however, different patterns of distortions were observed across all experiments. Moreover, the variability across experiments appears not to be moderated by the type of metric used or individuals' posture at the time of estimation. Consequently, findings suggest that, unlike perceptions of body part length, assessed using an identical methodology, our representations of the width of the body parts measured in this task are not fixed and vary across individuals and context. We propose that, as stored width representations of these parts are not necessarily required for navigating our environments, these may not be maintained by our perceptual systems, and thus variable task performance reflects the engagement of idiosyncratic guessing strategies.


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Tamaño , Humanos , Femenino , Adulto , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Imagen Corporal , Tamaño Corporal/fisiología , Adolescente
2.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 50(10): 989-999, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39146047

RESUMEN

When we interact with objects using our hands, we derive their size through our skin. Prolonged exposure to an object leads to a perceptual size aftereffect: adapting to a larger/smaller object makes a subsequently perceived object to appear smaller/larger than its actual size. This phenomenon has been described as haptic as tactile sensations with kinesthetic feedback are involved. However, the exact role of different haptic components in generating this aftereffect remains largely underexplored. Here, we investigated how different aspects of haptic touch influence size perception. After adaptation to a large sphere with one hand and a small sphere with the other, participants touched two test spheres of equal or different sizes and judged which one felt larger. Similar haptic size adaption aftereffects were observed (a) when participants repeatedly grasped on and off the adapters, (b) when they simply continued to grasp the adapters without further hand movements, and (c) when the adapters were grasped without involving the fingers. All these conditions produced stronger aftereffects than a condition where the palms were simply resting on the adapter. Our findings suggest that the inclusion of grasp markedly increased the aftereffects, highlighting the pivotal role of haptic interactions in determining perceptual size adaptation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Mano , Percepción del Tamaño , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Adulto , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Mano/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 291(2026): 20241200, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38981520

RESUMEN

Fingernails are specialized features of the primate hand, which are believed to contribute to manual dexterity. The sensorimotor functions of fingernails, however, remain poorly understood. This study investigates the ability of humans to precisely localize touches applied to the fingernail plate. Nine different locations on the fingernail were touched and participants judged the location by clicking a mouse cursor on a photograph of their finger. Performance in this condition was compared with stimuli applied to the skin of the fingertip. The results showed that participants are able to localize touch on the fingernails at substantially higher than chance levels. Moreover, the precision of this ability is not appreciably lower than that of the fingertips. These results show that the fingernail is a highly sensitive sensory organ, which is capable of providing rich spatial information about tactile stimuli.


Asunto(s)
Dedos , Uñas , Tacto , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto , Dedos/fisiología , Dedos/anatomía & histología , Percepción del Tacto , Adulto Joven
4.
Brain Sci ; 14(6)2024 Jun 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38928579

RESUMEN

Interoceptive dysfunctions are increasingly implicated in a number of physical and mental health conditions. Accordingly, there is a pertinent need for therapeutic interventions which target interoceptive deficits. Heartrate and heartrate variability biofeedback therapy (HR(V)-BF), interventions which train individuals to regulate their cardiovascular signals and constrain these within optimal parameters through breathing, could enhance the functioning of interoceptive pathways via stimulation of the vagus nerve. Consequently, this narrative systematic review sought to synthesise the current state of the literature with regard to the potential of HR(V)-BF as an interoceptive intervention across behavioural, physiological and neural outcome measures related to interoception. In total, 77 papers were included in this review, with the majority using physiological outcome measures. Overall, findings were mixed with respect to improvements in the outcome measures after HR(V)-BF. However, trends suggested that effects on measures related to interoception were stronger when resonance frequency breathing and an intense treatment protocol were employed. Based on these findings, we propose a three-stage model by which HR(V)-BF may improve interoception which draws upon principles of interoceptive inference and predictive coding. Furthermore, we provide specific directions for future research, which will serve to advance the current knowledge state.

5.
PLoS One ; 19(2): e0293920, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38300951

RESUMEN

Faces are a primary means of conveying social information between humans. One important factor modulating the perception of human faces is emotional expression. Face inversion also affects perception, including judgments of emotional expression, possibly through the disruption of configural processing. One intriguing inversion effect is an illusion whereby faces appear to be physically smaller when upright than when inverted. This illusion appears to be highly selective for faces. In this study, we investigated whether the emotional expression of a face (neutral, happy, afraid, and angry) modulates the magnitude of this size illusion. Results showed that for all four expressions, there was a clear bias for inverted stimuli to be judged as larger than upright ones. This demonstrates that there is no influence of emotional expression on the size underestimation of upright faces, a surprising result given that recognition of different emotional expressions is known to be affected unevenly by inversion. Results are discussed considering recent neuroimaging research which used population receptive field (pRF) mapping to investigate the neural mechanisms underlying face perception features and which may provide an explanation for how an upright face appears smaller than an inverted one. Elucidation of this effect would lead to a greater understanding of how humans communicate.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Ilusiones , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Ira , Felicidad , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos
6.
Cancer Res Commun ; 4(1): 65-80, 2024 01 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38214542

RESUMEN

Neuroblastoma is the most common extracranial tumor, accounting for 15% of all childhood cancer-related deaths. The long-term survival of patients with high-risk tumors is less than 40%, and MYCN amplification is one of the most common indicators of poor outcomes. Zika virus (ZIKV) is a mosquito-borne flavivirus associated with mild constitutional symptoms outside the fetal period. Our published data showed that high-risk and recurrent neuroblastoma cells are permissive to ZIKV infection, resulting in cell type-specific lysis. In this study, we assessed the efficacy of ZIKV as an oncolytic treatment for high-risk neuroblastoma using in vivo tumor models. Utilizing both MYCN-amplified and non-amplified models, we demonstrated that the application of ZIKV had a rapid tumoricidal effect. This led to a nearly total loss of the tumor mass without evidence of recurrence, offering a robust survival advantage to the host. Detection of the viral NS1 protein within the tumors confirmed that a permissive infection preceded tissue necrosis. Despite robust titers within the tumor, viral shedding to the host was poor and diminished rapidly, correlating with no detectable side effects to the murine host. Assessments from both primary pretreatment and recurrent posttreatment isolates confirmed that permissive sensitivity to ZIKV killing was dependent on the expression of CD24, which was highly expressed in neuroblastomas and conferred a proliferative advantage to tumor growth. Exploiting this viral sensitivity to CD24 offers the possibility of its use as a prognostic target for a broad population of expressing cancers, many of which have shown resistance to current clinical therapies. SIGNIFICANCE: Sensitivity to the tumoricidal effect of ZIKV on high-risk neuroblastoma tumors is dependent on CD24 expression, offering a prognostic marker for this oncolytic therapy in an extensive array of CD24-expressing cancers.


Asunto(s)
Neuroblastoma , Viroterapia Oncolítica , Virus Zika , Animales , Humanos , Ratones , Antígeno CD24/genética , Proteína Proto-Oncogénica N-Myc , Recurrencia Local de Neoplasia , Neuroblastoma/terapia , Virus Zika/genética
7.
J Lipid Res ; 65(2): 100497, 2024 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38216056

RESUMEN

Atherosclerotic cardiovascular disease (ASCVD) remains the leading cause of disease burden in the world and is highly correlated with chronic elevations of LDL-C. LDL-C-lowering drugs, such as statins or monoclonal antibodies against proprotein convertase subtilisin/kexin type 9 (PCSK9), are known to reduce the risk of cardiovascular diseases; however, statins are associated with limited efficacy and poor adherence to treatment, whereas PCSK9 inhibitors are only prescribed to a "high-risk" patient population or those who have failed other therapies. Based on the proven efficacy and safety profile of existing monoclonal antibodies, we have developed a peptide-based vaccine against PCSK9, VXX-401, as an alternative option to treat hypercholesterolemia and prevent ASCVD. VXX-401 is designed to trigger a safe humoral immune response against PCSK9, resulting in the production of endogenous antibodies and a subsequent 30-40% reduction in blood LDL-C. In this article, VXX-401 demonstrates robust immunogenicity and sustained serum LDL-C-lowering effects in nonhuman primates. In addition, antibodies induced by VXX-401 bind to human PCSK9 with high affinity and block the inhibitory effect of PCSK9 on LDL-C uptake in a hepatic cell model. A repeat-dose toxicity study conducted in nonhuman primates under good laboratory practices toxicity indicated a suitable safety and tolerability profile, with injection site reactions being the main findings. As a promising safe and effective LDL-C-lowering therapy, VXX-401 may represent a broadly accessible and convenient option to treat hypercholesterolemia and prevent ASCVD.


Asunto(s)
Anticolesterolemiantes , Aterosclerosis , Inhibidores de Hidroximetilglutaril-CoA Reductasas , Hipercolesterolemia , Animales , Humanos , Proproteína Convertasa 9 , Hipercolesterolemia/tratamiento farmacológico , Inhibidores de Hidroximetilglutaril-CoA Reductasas/uso terapéutico , LDL-Colesterol , Macaca fascicularis , Anticolesterolemiantes/farmacología , Anticolesterolemiantes/uso terapéutico , Anticuerpos Monoclonales/uso terapéutico , Aterosclerosis/metabolismo
8.
Cognition ; 245: 105733, 2024 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38281395

RESUMEN

Words are the primary means by which we communicate meaning and ideas, while faces provide important social cues. Studying visual illusions involving faces and words can elucidate the hierarchical processing of information as different regions of the brain are specialised for face recognition and word processing. A size illusion has previously been demonstrated for faces, whereby an inverted face is perceived as larger than the same stimulus upright. Here, two experiments replicate the face size illusion, and investigate whether the illusion is also present for individual letters (Experiment 1), and visual words and pseudowords (Experiment 2). Results confirm a robust size Illusion for faces. Letters, words and pseudowords and unfamiliar letters all show a reverse size illusion, as we previously demonstrated for human bodies. Overall, results indicate the illusion occurs in early perceptual stages upstream of semantic processing. Results are consistent with the idea of a general-purpose mechanism that encodes curvilinear shapes found in both scripts and our environment. Word and face perception rely on specialised, independent cognitive processes. The underestimation of the size of upright stimuli is specific to faces. Opposite size illusions may reflect differences in how size information is encoded and represented in stimulus-specialised neural networks, resulting in contrasting perceptual effects. Though words and faces differ visually, there is both symmetry and asymmetry in how the brain 'reads' them.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Ilusiones , Humanos , Cara , Encéfalo , Cuerpo Humano , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos
9.
Perception ; 52(11-12): 774-781, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37691587

RESUMEN

Several features of tactile stimuli modulate the perceived distance between touches. In particular, distances are perceived as farther apart when the time interval between them is longer, than when it is shorter. Such effects have been interpreted as a form of 'psychological relativity', analogous to Einstein's conception of a four-dimensional space-time. We investigated whether similar effects occur for stimulus features other than time, specifically stimulus intensity. We hypothesised that perceived distance would be increased when the two stimuli differed in intensity, since they would then be farther apart in a multi-dimensional feature space. Participants made verbal estimates of the perceived distance between two touches on their left hand. Intensity was manipulated such that both stimuli could be intense, both could be light, or one could be intense and the other light. We found no evidence for change in perceived tactile distance when stimuli intensity mis-matched. In contrast, there were clear effects of average stimulus intensity on perceived distance. Intense stimuli were judged as farther apart than light stimuli, and mixed stimuli were intermediate. These results are consistent with theories of general magnitude representation, which argue that multiple dimensions of magnitude are dependent on a shared underlying representation of domain-general magnitude.


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Tiempo , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Tacto , Mano
10.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 152(12): 3433-3439, 2023 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37561498

RESUMEN

When reproducing the remembered location of dots within a circle, judgments are biased toward the center of imaginary quadrants formed by imaginary vertical and horizontal axes. This effect may result from the heightened precision in the visual system for these orientations in a retinotopic reference frame, or alternately on the internal representation of gravity. We dissociated reference frames defined by the retina and by gravity by having participants locate dots from memory in a circle when their head was upright (aligned with gravity) versus tilted 30° to the left (misaligned with gravity). We mapped the structure of spatial prototypes in a data-driven way using a novel "imaging" procedure. We calculated the rotation of the prototype maps which maximized the similarity between postures, letting us quantify the contribution of each reference frame. Spatial categories are determined by a combination of reference frames, with clear contributions from both gravitational and retinal factors. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Postura , Memoria Espacial , Humanos , Gravitación , Recuerdo Mental , Orientación , Percepción Espacial
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 241(9): 2371-2379, 2023 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37620437

RESUMEN

Adaptation aftereffects for features such as identity and gender have been shown to transfer between faces and bodies, and faces and body parts, i.e. hands. However, no studies have investigated transfer of adaptation aftereffects between whole bodies and body parts. The present study investigated whether visual adaptation aftereffects transfer between hands and whole bodies in the context of adiposity judgements (i.e. how thin or fat a body is). On each trial, participants had to decide whether the body they saw was thinner or fatter than average. Participants performed the task before and after exposure to a thin/fat hand. Consistent with body adaptation studies, after exposure to a slim hand participants judged subsequently presented bodies to be fatter than after adaptation to a fat hand. These results suggest that there may be links between visual representations of body adiposity for whole bodies and body parts.


Asunto(s)
Adiposidad , Cuerpo Humano , Humanos , Mano , Extremidad Superior , Obesidad
12.
Curr Biol ; 33(14): R758-R759, 2023 07 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37490859

RESUMEN

According to Newton's laws, the weight of a body part is equal to its mass times gravitational acceleration. Our experience of body part weight, however, is constructed by the central nervous system. No sensory receptors directly specify the weight of body parts, and the factors influencing perceived weight remain unknown. The perceived weight of held objects has been linked to sensations of the magnitude of central motor commands sent to the muscles, what Helmholtz called the effort of will and has subsequently been called the sense of effort1. The link between the sense of effort and the perceived weight of objects is shown by studies demonstrating that held weights feel heavier when muscles are weakened by fatigue1, anaesthesia2, and following brain damage3. Similar drive to muscles is required to counteract the force of gravity on the limbs themselves, though few studies have investigated the perceived weight of body parts4. Stroke patients with hemiplegia frequently comment that their limbs feel heavy5, an effect linked to fatigue in the affected limb6. Similarly, amputees commonly complain of the weight of prosthetic limbs7, despite these typically weighing less than actual limbs. Here we report that healthy adult humans systematically underestimate hand weight. We used a psychophysical matching task to measure the experience of hand weight, which was underestimated on average by 49.4%. We further found that experimental induction of hand fatigue causes a systematic increase in perceived hand weight. Our results demonstrate that humans fail to experience the full weight of their body.


Asunto(s)
Amputados , Mano , Adulto , Humanos , Músculos/fisiología , Células Receptoras Sensoriales , Fatiga
13.
Autism Res ; 16(9): 1750-1764, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37409496

RESUMEN

People with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or higher levels of autistic traits have atypical characteristics in sensory processing. Atypicalities have been reported for proprioceptive judgments, which are tightly related to internal bodily representations underlying position sense. However, no research has directly investigated whether self-bodily representations are different in individuals with ASD. Implicit hand maps, estimated based on participants' proprioceptive sensations without sight of their hand, are known to be distorted such that the shape is stretched along the medio-lateral hand axis even for neurotypical participants. Here, with the view of ASD as falling on a continuous distribution among the general population, we explored differences in implicit body representations along with autistic traits by focusing on relationships between autistic traits and the magnitudes of the distortions in implicit hand maps (N ~ 100). We estimated the magnitudes of distortions in implicit hand maps both for fingers and hand surfaces on the dorsal and palmar sides of the hand. Autistic traits were measured by questionnaires (Autism Spectrum [AQ] and Empathy/Systemizing [EQ-SQ] Quotients). The distortions in implicit hand maps were replicated in our experimental situations. However, there were no significant relationships between autistic traits and the magnitudes of the distortions as well as within-individual variabilities in the maps and localization performances. Consistent results were observed from comparisons between IQ-matched samples of people with and without a diagnosis of ASD. Our findings suggest that there exist perceptual and neural processes for implicit body representations underlying position sense consistent across levels of autistic traits.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Trastorno Autístico , Humanos , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/complicaciones , Mano , Empatía , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 49(8): 1175-1179, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37347925

RESUMEN

The ability to localize touch on the skin is an important aspect of tactile perception. As our limbs move, the skin stretches flexibly, and research has found that signals specifying stretch affect perception of limb posture. Skin stretch also distorts the relative spatial position of different locations on the skin, posing potential problems for tactile localization. Here, we investigated the effects of skin stretch using an established test of tactile localization on the hand. Twenty participants completed a tactile localization task in no-stretch and stretched conditions, respectively, after giving informed consent. The current study found a clear distal and radial bias in both the no-stretch condition and the stretched condition. Indeed, the distal bias was even larger in the stretched condition than at baseline. Critically, however, this change in distal bias was entirely accounted for by changes in the actual location of stimulus as a result of skin stretch, with no corresponding change in the judged location. Thus, the somatosensory system appears to disregard stretch when calculating the location of tactile stimuli. These results mirror recent findings showing that tactile distance perception also fails to take skin stretch into account. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Tacto , Tacto , Humanos , Percepción Espacial , Mano , Postura
15.
Curr Biol ; 33(9): R366-R368, 2023 05 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37160096

RESUMEN

Capuchin monkeys are unique among New World monkeys for their manual dexterity and use of tools. New research using magical sleight of hand shows visual perception of others' actions paralleling their manual skills.


Asunto(s)
Cebus , Cognición , Animales , Percepción Visual , Mano
16.
Cognition ; 230: 105260, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36058103

RESUMEN

Body posture and configuration provide important visual cues about the emotion states of other people. We know that bodily form is processed holistically, however, emotion recognition may depend on different mechanisms; certain body parts, such as the hands, may be especially important for perceiving emotion. This study therefore compared participants' emotion recognition performance when shown images of full bodies, or of isolated hands, arms, heads and torsos. Across three experiments, emotion recognition accuracy was above chance for all body parts. While emotions were recognized most accurately from full bodies, recognition performance from the hands was more accurate than for other body parts. Representational similarity analysis further showed that the pattern of errors for the hands was related to that for full bodies. Performance was reduced when stimuli were inverted, showing a clear body inversion effect. The high performance for hands was not due only to the fact that there are two hands, as performance remained well above chance even when just one hand was shown. These results demonstrate that emotions can be decoded from body parts. Furthermore, certain features, such as the hands, are more important to emotion perception than others. STATEMENT OF RELEVANCE: Successful social interaction relies on accurately perceiving emotional information from others. Bodies provide an abundance of emotion cues; however, the way in which emotional bodies and body parts are perceived is unclear. We investigated this perceptual process by comparing emotion recognition for body parts with that for full bodies. Crucially, we found that while emotions were most accurately recognized from full bodies, emotions were also classified accurately when images of isolated hands, arms, heads and torsos were seen. Of the body parts shown, emotion recognition from the hands was most accurate. Furthermore, shared patterns of emotion classification for hands and full bodies suggested that emotion recognition mechanisms are shared for full bodies and body parts. That the hands are key to emotion perception is important evidence in its own right. It could also be applied to interventions for individuals who find it difficult to read emotions from faces and bodies.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Cuerpo Humano , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Señales (Psicología) , Mano , Expresión Facial
17.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 27(1): 9, 2023 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36418207
18.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 49(2): 226-235, 2023 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36480375

RESUMEN

Tactile distance perception is influenced by stimulus orientation. On the hands or face, effects of orientation may originate from the mostly oval shape of receptive fields (RF) of which the long axis aligns with the proximodistal body axis. As tactile distance estimation relies on the number of RFs between stimuli, their alignment leads to a distortion of perception with distances being perceived as shorter in the proximodistal than the mediolateral body axis. It is however unknown, how physical manipulations such as skin stretch affect distance perception. Participants judged which of two distances aligned with the mediolateral or proximodistal axis on their dorsal dominant hand felt larger in two conditions: without physical manipulation and with proximodistal skin stretch. Distances were perceived shorter in proximodistal direction in both the nonstretch and the stretch condition, which was significantly pronounced in the stretch condition. Skin stretch led to perception of tactile distances as smaller, possibly related to the removal of afferent nerve endings and corresponding somatosensory RFs in the same external reference frame between the two touches. Though skin stretch is represented centrally, our results likely show that no correctional top-down mechanism corrects for skin stretch when estimating tactile distances. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Emociones
20.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 48(12): 1427-1438, 2022 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36442041

RESUMEN

Orientation information contributes substantially to our tactile perception, such as feeling an object's shape on the skin. For vision, a perceptual adaptation aftereffect (tilt aftereffect; TAE), which is well explained by neural orientation selectivity, has been used to reveal fundamental perceptual properties of orientation processing. Neural orientation selectivity has been reported in somatosensory cortices. However, little research has investigated the perceptual characteristics of the tactile TAE. The aim of the current study was to provide the first demonstration of a tactile TAE on the hand and investigate the perceptual nature of tactile TAE on the hand surface. We used a 2-point stimulation with minimal input for orientation. We found clear TAEs on the hand surface: Adaptation induced shifts in subjective vertical sensation toward the orientation opposite to the adapted orientation. Further, adaptation aftereffects were purely based on orientation processing given that the effects transferred between different lengths across adaptor and test stimuli and type of stimuli. Finally, adaptation aftereffects were anchored to the hand: tactile TAE occurred independently of hand rotation and transferred from palm to dorsum sides of the hand, while the effects did not transfer between hands. Our findings demonstrate the existence of hand-centered perceptual processing for basic tactile orientation information. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Mano , Percepción del Tacto , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Emociones
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...