Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 63
Filtrar
1.
Eur J Sport Sci ; 24(6): 750-757, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38874996

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to clarify the temporal coordination between gaze, head, and arm movements during forehand rallies in table tennis. Collegiate male table tennis players (n = 7) conducted forehand rallies at a constant tempo (100, 120, and 150 bpm) using a metronome. In each tempo condition, participants performed 30 strokes (a total of 90 strokes). Gaze, head, and dominant arm (shoulder, elbow, and wrist) movements were recorded with an eye-tracking device equipped with a Gyro sensor and a 3-D motion capture system. The results showed that the effect of head movements relative to gaze movements was significantly higher than that of eye movements in the three tempo conditions. Our results indicate that head movements are closely associated with gaze movements during rallies. Furthermore, cross-correlation coefficients (CCs) between head and arm movements were more than 0.96 (maximum coefficient: 0.99). In addition, head and arm movements were synchronized during rallies. Finally, CCs between gaze and arm movements were more than 0.74 (maximum coefficient: 0.99), indicating that gaze movements are temporally coordinated with arm movements. Taken together, head movements could play important roles not only in gaze tracking but also in the temporal coordination with arm movements during table tennis forehand rallies.


Asunto(s)
Brazo , Movimientos Oculares , Movimientos de la Cabeza , Movimiento , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tenis , Humanos , Masculino , Brazo/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Movimientos de la Cabeza/fisiología , Tenis/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Cabeza/fisiología
2.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 8582, 2024 04 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38615053

RESUMEN

Human movements are adjusted by motor adaptation in order to maintain their accuracy. There are two systems in motor adaptation, referred to as explicit or implicit adaptation. It has been suggested that the implicit adaptation is based on the prediction error and has been used in a number of motor adaptation studies. This study aimed to examine the effect of visual memory on prediction error in implicit visuomotor adaptation by comparing visually- and memory-guided reaching tasks. The visually-guided task is thought to be implicit learning based on prediction error, whereas the memory-guided task requires more cognitive processes. We observed the adaptation to visuomotor rotation feedback that is gradually rotated. We found that the adaptation and retention rates were higher in the visually-guided task than in the memory-guided task. Furthermore, the delta-band power obtained by electroencephalography (EEG) in the visually-guided task was increased immediately following the visual feedback, which indicates that the prediction error was larger in the visually-guided task. Our results show that the visuomotor adaptation is enhanced in the visually-guided task because the prediction error, which contributes update of the internal model, was more reliable than in the memory-guided task. Therefore, we suggest that the processing of the prediction error is affected by the task-type, which in turn affects the rate of the visuomotor adaptation.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Retroalimentación Sensorial , Humanos , Aprendizaje , Memoria , Movimiento
3.
Vision (Basel) ; 8(2)2024 Apr 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38651441

RESUMEN

Visual motion information plays an important role in the control of movements in sports. Skilled ball players are thought to acquire accurate visual information by using an effective visual search strategy with eye and head movements. However, differences in catching ability and gaze movements due to sports experience and expertise have not been clarified. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to determine the characteristics of gaze strategies based on eye and head movements during a ball-catching task in athlete and novice groups. Participants were softball and tennis players and college students who were not experienced in ball sports (novice). They performed a one-handed catching task using a tennis ball-shooting machine, which was placed at 9 m in front of the participants, and two conditions were set depending on the height of the ball trajectory (high and low conditions). Their head and eye velocities were detected using a gyroscope and electrooculography (EOG) during the task. Our results showed that the upward head velocity and the downward eye velocity were lower in the softball group than in the tennis and novice groups. When the head was pitched upward, the downward eye velocity was induced from the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) during ball catching. Therefore, it is suggested that skilled ball players have relatively stable head and eye movements, which may lead to an effective gaze strategy. An advantage of the stationary gaze in the softball group could be to acquire visual information about the surroundings other than the ball.

4.
Physiol Rep ; 11(22): e15877, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37985195

RESUMEN

The purpose of the current study was to clarify the effect of eccentricity on visual motion prediction using a time-to-contact (TTC) task. TTC indicates the predictive ability to accurately estimate the time-to-contact of a moving object based on visual motion perception. We also measured motion reaction time (motion RT) as an indicator of the speed of visual motion perception. The TTC task was to press a button when the moving target would arrive at the stationary goal. In the occluded condition, the target dot was occluded 500 ms before the time to contact. The motion RT task was to press a button as soon as the target moved. The visual targets were randomly presented at five different eccentricities (4°, 6°, 8°, 10°, 12°) and moved on a circular trajectory at a constant tangent velocity (8°/s) to keep the eccentricity constant. Our results showed that TTC in the occluded condition showed an earlier response as the eccentricity increased. Furthermore, the motion RT became longer as the eccentricity increased. Therefore, it is most likely that a slower speed perception in peripheral vision delays the perceived speed of motion onset and leads to an earlier response in the TTC task.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Percepción Visual , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Visión Ocular , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Movimiento (Física)
5.
Hum Mov Sci ; 90: 103124, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37478682

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to clarify the properties of gaze and head movements during forehand stroke in table tennis. Collegiate table tennis players (n = 12) conducted forehand strokes toward a ball launched by a skilled experimenter. A total of ten trials were conducted for the experimental task. Horizontal and vertical movements of the ball, gaze, head and eye were analyzed from the image recorded by an eye tracking device. The results showed that participants did not always keep their gaze and head position on the ball throughout the entire ball path. Our results indicate that table tennis players tend to gaze at the ball in the initial ball-tracking phase. Furthermore, there was a significant negative correlation between eye and head position especially in the vertical direction. This result suggests that horizontal VOR is suppressed more than vertical VOR in ball-tracking during table tennis forehand stroke. Finally, multiple regression analysis showed that the effect of head position to gaze position was significantly higher than that of eye position. This result indicates that gaze position during forehand stroke could be associated with head position rather than eye position. Taken together, head movements may play an important role in maintaining the ball in a constant egocentric direction in table tennis forehand stroke.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos de la Cabeza , Tenis , Humanos , Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular
6.
PLoS One ; 18(3): e0283018, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36928499

RESUMEN

For motion in depth, even if the target moves at a constant speed in the real-world (physically), it would appear to be moving with acceleration on the retina. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to determine whether real-world and retinal motion affect speed perception in depth and to verify the influence of eye movements on both motion signals in judging speed in depth. We used a two-alternative forced-choice paradigm with two types of tasks. One stimulus moved at a constant speed in the real-world (world constant task) with three conditions: 80-60 cm (far), 60-40 cm (middle), and 40-20 cm (near) from the participant. The other stimulus moved at a constant speed on the retina (retinal constant task) with three conditions: 4-8 deg (far), 8-12 deg (middle), and 12-16 deg (near) as the vergence angle. The results showed that stimulus speed was perceived faster in the near condition than in the middle and far conditions for the world constant task, regardless of whether it was during fixation or convergence eye movements. In contrast, stimulus speed was perceived faster in the order of the far, middle, and near conditions for the retinal constant task. Our results indicate that speed perception of a visual target approaching the observer depends on real-world motion when the target position is relatively far from the observer. In contrast, retinal motion may influence speed perception when the target position is close to the observer. Our results also indicate that the effects of real-world and retinal motion on speed perception for motion in depth are similar with or without convergence eye movements. Therefore, it is suggested that when the visual target moves from far to near, the effects of real-world and retinal motion on speed perception are different depending on the initial target position.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Movimientos Oculares , Retina , Movimiento (Física) , Aceleración , Percepción de Profundidad
7.
Int J Mol Sci ; 24(3)2023 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36769144

RESUMEN

Surfactants are functional molecules utilized in various situations. The self-assembling property of surfactants enables several molecular arrangements that can be employed to build up nanometer-sized architectures. This is beneficial in the construction of functional inorganic-organic hybrids holding the merits of both inorganic and organic components. Among several surfactants, bolaamphiphile surfactants with two hydrophilic heads are effective, as they have multiple connecting or coordinating sites in one molecule. Here, a functional polyoxotungstate inorganic anion was successfully hybridized with a bolaamphiphile to form single crystals with anisotropic one-dimensional alignment of polyoxotungstate. Keggin-type metatungstate ([H2W12O40]6-, H2W12) was employed as an inorganic anion, and 1,12-dodecamethylenediammonium (C12N2) derived from 1,12-dodecanediamine was combined as an organic counterpart. A simple and general ion-exchange reaction provided a hybrid crystal consisting of H2W12 and C12N2 (C12N2-H2W12). Single crystal X-ray structure analyses revealed a characteristic honeycomb structure in the C12N2-H2W12 hybrid crystal, which is possibly effective for the emergence of conductivity due to the dissociative protons of C12N2.


Asunto(s)
Tensoactivos , Conformación Molecular , Tensoactivos/química
8.
Front Sports Act Living ; 4: 897373, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35655529

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to clarify the properties of visual strategies for gaze, eye, and head movements in skilled table tennis players during rallies. Collegiate expert and semi-expert table tennis players conducted forehand rallies at a constant tempo using a metronome. Two tempo conditions were used in the order of 130 and 150 bpm. Participants conducted a 20-stroke rally under each tempo condition. Horizontal and vertical angles between the gaze point and ball positions at the time the ball bounced (gaze-ball angle) were analyzed with the image that was recorded by an eye tracking device equipped with Gyro sensor. Eye and head movements during rallies were also recorded with the eye tracking device and Gyro sensor, respectively. The results showed that the gaze-ball angle of expert players was significantly larger than that of semi-expert players. This result indicates that expert players tended to keep their gaze position on the ball shorter than semi-expert players. We also found that eye movements of expert players were significantly smaller than that of semi-expert players. Furthermore, as the result of multiple regression analysis, the effect of eye movements on the gaze-ball angle was significantly higher than that of head movements. This result indicates that the gaze-ball angle during table tennis rallies could be associated with eye movements rather than head movements. Our findings suggest that the visual strategies used during table tennis rallies are different between expert and semi-expert players, even though they both have more than 10 years of experience.

9.
J Mot Behav ; 54(6): 755-762, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35410588

RESUMEN

Online motor control is often required to correct errors in rapid adjustments during reaching movements. It has been established that the initial arm trajectory during reaching is corrected by a target displacement. Since this corrective response occurs without perception of target perturbation, this is regarded as an automatic response. However, an object rarely "jumps" in daily life, rather it often "moves" as a chronological change of the position that causes visual motion. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to investigate whether the implicit visuomotor response is induced by target motion stimuli and to clarify the effects of target motion velocity on initial arm trajectory. Participants were asked to move a cursor from a start circle to a visual target. The target moved either leftward or rightward when the cursor passed 20 mm from the start circle. Four target velocities (10, 20, 30, 40 deg/s) were randomly presented. Our results showed that the initial velocity (first 50 ms) of the fast corrective response increased with the target velocity. Therefore, it is indicated that the fast corrective response is induced by the target motion stimulus with a short latency and its amplitude is dependent on the target velocity.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Desempeño Psicomotor , Humanos , Movimiento/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
10.
Physiol Behav ; 250: 113801, 2022 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35395251

RESUMEN

The online control system allows for automatic corrective response to unexpected perturbation. This corrective response may involve a prediction error between the sensory prediction by the motor command and the actual feedback signal. Therefore, we attempted to investigate the effect of motor command accuracy on the automatic corrective response. Participants were asked to move a cursor displayed on a monitor and required to reach the center of a Gaussian blob target as accurately as possible for small and large Gaussian blob conditions. The accuracy of the motor command was manipulated by the size of the Gaussian blob. In half of the trials, a perturbation occurred in which the cursor position jumped 10 mm to either the left or right from the actual position, which induced an automatic corrective response. This corrective response was detected by the acceleration signal on the lateral axis. In addition, the prediction error was estimated by the amplitude of the N1 event-related potential (ERP) of the EEG signal. We found that the automatic response and N1 ERP were significantly larger in the small Gaussian blob conditions than in the large one. This result indicates that the automatic corrective response is affected by the certainty of the motor command manipulated by the Gaussian blob. Furthermore, the linear mixed-effect model (LME) indicated that the response is associated with the N1 ERP. Therefore, we suggest that the motor command accuracy affects the prediction error, which in turn modulates the automatic corrective response.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados , Desempeño Psicomotor , Retroalimentación , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 127(4): 1198-1207, 2022 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35353633

RESUMEN

Expectations about forthcoming visual motion shaped by observers' experiences are known to induce anticipatory smooth eye movements (ASEMs) and changes in visual perception. Previous studies have demonstrated discrete effects of expectations on the control of ASEM and perception. However, the tasks designed in those studies were not able to segregate the effects of expectations and execution of ASEM itself on perception. In the present study, we attempted to directly examine the effect of ASEM itself on visual speed perception with a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task, in which observers were asked to track a pair of sequentially presented visual motion stimuli with their eyes and to judge whether the second stimulus (test stimulus) was faster or slower than the first (reference stimulus). Our results showed that observers' visual speed perception, quantified by a psychometric function, shifted according to ASEM velocity. This was the case even though there was no difference in the steady-state eye velocity. Further analyses revealed that the observers' perceptual decisions could be explained by a difference in the magnitude of retinal slip velocity in the initial phase of ocular tracking when the reference and test stimuli were presented, rather than in the steady-state phase. Our results provide psychophysical evidence of the importance of initial ocular tracking in visual speed perception and the strong impact of ASEM.NEW & NOTEWORTHY We provide psychophysical evidence that the execution of anticipatory smooth eye movement (ASEM) leads to underestimation of visual speed perception, that is, observers perceive the object motion velocity as slower than when ASEM is not induced, even though the performance of subsequent ocular tracking is comparable. Moreover, our results showed that such perceptual decisions regarding object motion velocity were derived from the ASEM-induced decrease in retinal slip velocity during the initial phase of ocular tracking.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Percepción de Movimiento , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Percepción Visual
12.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 22327, 2021 11 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34785718

RESUMEN

The smooth pursuit system has the ability to perform predictive feedforward control of eye movements. This study attempted to examine how stimulus and behavioral histories of past trials affect the control of predictive pursuit of target motion with randomized velocities. We used sequential ramp stimuli where the rightward velocity was fixed at 16 deg/s while the leftward velocity was either fixed (predictable) at one of seven velocities (4, 8, 12, 16, 20, 24, or 28 deg/s) or randomized (unpredictable). As a result, predictive pursuit responses were observed not only in the predictable condition but also in the unpredictable condition. Linear mixed-effects (LME) models showed that both stimulus and behavioral histories of the previous two or three trials influenced the predictive pursuit responses in the unpredictable condition. Intriguingly, the goodness of fit of the LME model was improved when both historical effects were fitted simultaneously rather than when each type of historical data was fitted alone. Our results suggest that predictive pursuit systems allow us to track randomized target motion using weighted averaging of the information of target velocity (stimulus) and motor output (behavior) in past time sequences.


Asunto(s)
Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
13.
Vision Res ; 189: 27-32, 2021 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34509706

RESUMEN

Predictive deceleration of eye motion during smooth pursuit is induced by explicit cues indicating the timing of the visual target offset. The first aim of this study (experiment 1) was to determine whether the timing of the onset of cue-based predictive pursuit termination depends on spatial or temporal information using three target velocities. The second aim (experiment 2) was to examine whether an unexpected offset of the target affects the pursuit termination. We conducted a pursuit termination task where participants tracked a moving target and then stopped tracking after the target disappeared. The results of experiment 1 showed that the onset times of predictive eye deceleration were consistent regardless of target velocity, indicating that its timing is controlled by the temporal estimation, rather than the spatial distance between the target and cue positions. In experiment 2, we compared pursuit termination between the following two conditions. One condition did not present any cues (unknown condition), whereas a second condition included a same cue as experiment 1 but the target disappeared 500 ms before the timing indicated by the cue unpredictably (unexpected condition). As a result, the unexpected condition showed significant delays in the onset of eye deceleration, but no difference in the total time for completion of pursuit termination. Therefore, our findings suggest that the cue-based pursuit termination is controlled by the predictive pursuit system, and an unexpected offset of the target yields delays in the onset of eye deceleration, while does not affect the duration of pursuit termination.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Señales (Psicología) , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 239(7): 2151-2158, 2021 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33977362

RESUMEN

It has been well established that an implicit motor response can be elicited by a target perturbation or a visual background motion during a reaching movement. Computational studies have suggested that the mechanism of this response is based on the error signal between the efference copy and the actual sensory feedback. If the implicit motor response is based on the efference copy, the motor command accuracy would affect the amount of the modulation of the motor response. Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to investigate the relationship between the implicit motor response and the motor planning accuracy. We used a memory-guided reaching task and a manual following response (MFR) which is induced by visual grating motion. Participants performed reaching movements toward a memorized-target location with a beep cue which was presented 0 or 3 s after the target disappeared (0-s delay and 3-s delay conditions). Leftward or rightward visual grating motion was applied 400 ms after the cue. In addition, an event-related potential (ERP) was recorded during the reaching task, which reflects the motor command accuracy. Our results showed that the N170 ERP amplitude in the parietal electrodes and the MFR amplitude were significantly larger for the 3-s delay condition than the 0-s delay condition. These results suggest that the motor planning accuracy affects the amount of the implicit visuomotor response. Furthermore, there was a significant within-subjects correlation between the MFR and the N170 amplitude, which could corroborate the relationship between the implicit motor response and the motor planning accuracy.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Desempeño Psicomotor , Potenciales Evocados , Retroalimentación Sensorial , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción
15.
Physiol Rep ; 9(9): e14833, 2021 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33991449

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to determine whether smooth pursuit eye movements affect visual motion prediction using a time-to-contact task where observers anticipate the exact instant that a partially occluded target would coincide with a stationary object. Moreover, we attempted to clarify the influence of second-order motion on visual motion prediction during smooth pursuit. One target object moved to another stationary object (6 deg apart) at constant velocity of 3, 4, and 5 deg/s, and then the two objects disappeared 500 ms after the onset of target motion. The observers estimated the moment the moving object would overlap the stationary object and pressed a button. For the pursuit condition, both a Gaussian window and a random dots texture moved in the same direction at the same speed for the first-order motion, whereas a Gaussian window moved over a static background composed of random dots texture for the second-order motion. The results showed that the constant error of the time-to-contact shifted to a later response for the pursuit condition compared to the fixation condition, regardless of the object velocity. In addition, during smooth pursuit, the constant error for the second-order motion shifted to an earlier response compared to the first-order motion when the object velocity was 3 deg/s, whereas no significant difference was found at 4 and 5 deg/s. Therefore, our results suggest that visual motion prediction using a time-to-contact task is affected by both eye movements and motion configuration such as second-order motion.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Anticipación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Visión Binocular , Adulto Joven
16.
Physiol Behav ; 235: 113397, 2021 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33775660

RESUMEN

Visual information based on eye movements influences sports performance in 3-D space. The purpose of this study was to clarify the properties of fast vergence eye movements and horizontal saccades in athletes. Thirty-four college students were classified into three groups: twelve baseball players (BAS), ten track and field athletes (TRA) and twelve non-athletes (NON). The oculomotor tasks consisted of vergence eye movements (vergence task) including convergence and divergence, and horizontal saccades (saccade task). The participants made a rapid gaze shift toward a visual target. For the vergence task, the target was irradiated at 5 different vergence angles (3, 5, 10, 15, 20°) randomly. For the saccade task, the target was irradiated at 5 different saccade amplitudes (0, ±5, ±10°) randomly. The results showed that the peak eye velocity was higher in order of saccade, convergence, and divergence eye movements. The gain of convergence and saccades was higher than that of divergence. Furthermore, the latency of saccades was shorter than that of convergence and divergence. These results suggest that saccades and convergence are superior compared with divergence, and the dynamic property of convergence is partially analogous to horizontal saccades. The results of the comparison between the three groups showed that the latency of convergence was shorter for BAS than for NON. Therefore, different visual experience and baseball training could influence the gaze-shift dynamics during convergence eye movements.


Asunto(s)
Rendimiento Atlético , Béisbol , Atletas , Convergencia Ocular , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos , Visión Binocular
17.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 315, 2021 01 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33431981

RESUMEN

This study investigated whether the eight standard tests of sports vision used in Japan appropriately reflect sports vision; whether all eight tests are necessary; and if not, which combination yields the optimal model. Participants were kendo practitioners (exercise group, n = 41) and those with no exercise habits (non-exercise group, n = 65). The performance of the two groups on all eight tests were compared. The groups differed in static visual acuity, kinetic visual acuity, and eye/hand coordination. A high correlation (r = 0.75) was observed between static visual acuity and kinetic visual acuity, while contrast sensitivity was moderately correlated with static visual acuity and kinetic visual acuity (r = 0.6), and dynamic visual acuity was moderately correlated with eye/hand coordination (r = - 0.66). Logistic regression analysis indicated that it is not necessary to conduct all eight tests; the optimal model included static visual acuity, visual reaction time, and eye/hand coordination. Our results suggest that static visual acuity, visual reaction time, and eye/hand coordination are sufficient for assessing visual function in kendo practitioners. For other sports, it may be possible to construct discriminative models using the same method and determine which aspects of visual function and which training methods to emphasise in a given sport.


Asunto(s)
Atletas , Artes Marciales , Pruebas de Visión/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino
18.
Physiol Behav ; 229: 113245, 2021 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33188790

RESUMEN

Current study attempted to determine whether repeated smooth pursuit trials using theta motion, in which the directions of retinal image-motion and object-motion are opposed, yield pursuit adaptation. Adaptation trials consisted of 350 step-ramp trials using theta motion, and pre- and post-trials using first-order motion were conducted. As a result, initial acceleration in post-adaptation increased significantly than pre-adaptation trials. This was the case even though there was no adaptive change throughout adaptation (350 trials) using theta motion. Our results suggest that smooth pursuit could adapt to theta motion even with challenges associated with opposite retinal slip.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Aceleración , Adaptación Fisiológica , Movimiento (Física) , Estimulación Luminosa
19.
PLoS One ; 15(12): e0243430, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33315877

RESUMEN

A large number of psychophysical and neurophysiological studies have demonstrated that smooth pursuit eye movements are tightly related to visual motion perception. This could be due to the fact that visual motion sensitive cortical areas such as meddle temporal (MT), medial superior temporal (MST) areas are involved in motion perception as well as pursuit initiation. Although the directional-discrimination and perceived target velocity tasks are used to evaluate visual motion perception, it is still uncertain whether the speed of visual motion perception, which is determined by visuomotor reaction time (RT) to a small target, is related to pursuit initiation. Therefore, we attempted to determine the relationship between pursuit latency/acceleration and the visual motion RT which was measured to the visual motion stimuli that moved leftward or rightward. The participants were instructed to fixate on a stationary target and press one of the buttons corresponding to the direction of target motion as soon as possible once the target starts to move. We applied five different visual motion stimuli including first- and second-order motion for smooth pursuit and visual motion RT tasks. It is well known that second-order motion induces lower retinal image motion, which elicits weaker responses in MT and MST compared to first-order motion stimuli. Our results showed that pursuit initiation including latency and initial eye acceleration were suppressed by second-order motion. In addition, second-order motion caused a delay in visual motion RT. The better performances in both pursuit initiation and visual motion RT were observed for first-order motion, whereas second-order (theta motion) induced remarkable deficits in both variables. Furthermore, significant Pearson's correlation and within-subjects correlation coefficients were obtained between visual motion RT and pursuit latency/acceleration. Our findings support the suggestion that there is a common neuronal pathway involved in both pursuit initiation and the speed of visual motion perception.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Animales , Femenino , Humanos , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Masculino , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
20.
Neurosci Lett ; 735: 135193, 2020 09 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32565221

RESUMEN

The purpose of current study was to clarify the influence of preceding muscle activity on the force production and movement-related cortical potential (MRCP) during superimposed ballistic contractions. The participants performed the ballistic force production at 40 % of maximum voluntary contraction (MVC) using the isometric abduction force of the metacarpophalangeal joint of the index finger. They were asked to match the peak of force curve with a horizontal target line displayed on the computer monitor. We compared the MRCP amplitude during force exertion detected from Fz, C4, C3, Cz and Pz electrodes during ballistic force production with (active condition) and without (resting condition) preceding muscle activity. The results showed that the MRCP amplitudes of Fz, C4, C3 and Cz electrodes were significantly smaller for the active condition than the resting condition. This was the case even though the peak force values during both conditions were identical. This result suggests that the facilitation of spinal motoneuron excitability by preceding muscle activity could reduce the required central motor command to produce the identical force level. In addition, we examined the MRCP amplitude during ballistic force production of the active condition without a visually displayed target. In this condition, the participants had to perform the force production based on aiming point of target force level (40 %MVC). As a result, the mean of peak force without a visual target was 54 %MVC, which overshot the aiming force level. However, the MRCP amplitudes of five electrodes during the 54 %MVC force production in the active condition were equivalent to the case of the 40 %MVC force production in the resting condition. These results suggest that the MRCP amplitude is consistent with participants' sense of effort involved in the force production, rather than the actual produced force level.


Asunto(s)
Contracción Isométrica/fisiología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados Motores/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA