Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
Más filtros












Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Neurosci Methods ; 393: 109881, 2023 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37172913

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Flavor is a mental representation that results from the brain's integration of at least odor and taste, and fMRI can highlight brain-related areas. However, delivering stimuli during fMRI can be challenging especially when administrating liquid stimuli in supine position. It remains unclear how and when odorants are released in the nose and how to improve odorant release. NEW METHOD: We used a proton transfer reaction mass spectrometer (PTR-MS) to monitor the in vivo release of odorants via the retronasal pathway during retronasal odor-taste stimulation in a supine position. We tested techniques to improve odorant release, including avoiding or delaying swallowing and velum open training (VOT). RESULTS: Odorant release was observed during retronasal stimulation, before swallowing, and in a supine position. VOT did not improve odorant release. Odorant release during stimulation had a latency more optimal for fitting with BOLD timing than after swallowing. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHOD(S): Previous in vivo measurements of odorant release under fMRI-like conditions showed that odorant release occurred only after swallowing. On the contrary, a second study found that aroma release could occur before swallowing, but participants were sitting. CONCLUSION: Our method shows optimal odorant release during the stimulation phase, meeting the criteria for high-quality brain imaging of flavor processing without swallowing-related motion artifacts. These findings provide an important advancement in understanding the mechanisms underlying flavor processing in the brain.


Asunto(s)
Odorantes , Olfato , Humanos , Olfato/fisiología , Gusto/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Nariz/fisiología
2.
Brain Cogn ; 159: 105864, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35397314

RESUMEN

Electroencephalography (EEG), and the measure of event-related potentials (ERPs) in particular, are useful methods to study the cognitive and cerebral mechanisms underlying the perception and processing of food cues. Further research on these aspects is necessary to better understand how cognitive functioning may influence food choices in different populations (e.g. obese individuals, individuals with eating disorders). To help researchers in designing future studies, this article provides an overview of the methods used in the current literature on ERPs and food-related cognition. Several methodological aspects are explored to outline interesting perspectives for future research, including discussions on the main experimental tasks used, the cognitive functions assessed (e.g. inhibitory control, attentional processing), the characteristics of the participants recruited (e.g. weight status, eating behaviors), and the stimuli selected (e.g. food pictures, odors). The issues generated by some of these methodological choices are discussed, and a few guidelines are provided.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados , Alimentos , Cognición , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Humanos
3.
Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol ; 279(7): 3503-3510, 2022 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34921612

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Olfactory adaptation is a peripheral (at the epithelium level) or a central (at the brain level) mechanism resulting from repeated or prolonged odorous exposure that can induce a perceptual decrease. The aim of this study was to assess whether a peripheral adaptation occurs when an odor is repeated ten times. Moreover, the specificity of the peripheral adaptation to the nature of the odorant was investigated. METHODS: Four odorants (eugenol, manzanate, ISO E Super and phenylethanol) were presented using precisely controlled air-dilution olfactometry. They differed in terms of their physicochemical properties. Electrophysiological recordings were made at the level of the olfactory mucosa, the so-called electro-olfactogram (EOG). Thirty-five right-handed participants were recruited. RESULTS: Sixty-nine percent of the participants presented at least one EOG, whatever the odor condition. The EOG amplitude did not significantly decrease over 10 repeated exposures to any odorant. The intensity ratings tended to decrease over stimulations for manzanate, PEA, and eugenol. No correlation was found between the mean EOG amplitudes and the mean intensity ratings. However, the presence of EOG amplitude decreases over stimulations for few subjects suggests that peripheral adaptation might exist. CONCLUSION: Overall, our results did not establish a clear peripheral adaptation measured with EOG but indicate the eventuality of such an effect.


Asunto(s)
Eugenol , Olfato , Humanos , Odorantes , Mucosa Olfatoria/fisiología , Olfato/fisiología
4.
Neuroscience ; 460: 130-144, 2021 04 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33482330

RESUMEN

Attentional automatic processes and cerebral activity may differ between individuals with different weight statuses in the presence of food stimuli (e.g. odors, pictures). In the present study, we used an implicit olfactory priming paradigm to test the influence of non-attentively perceived food odors on the cerebral activity underlying the processing of food pictures, in normal-weight, overweight, and obese adults. A pear odor and a pound cake odor were used as primes, respectively priming sweet low-energy-density foods and high-energy-density foods. Event-related potentials were recorded while the participants passively watched pictures of sweet low and high-energy-density foods, under the two priming conditions plus an odorless control condition. The amplitude and latency of several peaks were measured (P100, N100, P200, N400). As a major result, we found that weight status influences the cerebral activity underlying the processing of food cues outside of consciousness, as early as the first detectable P100 peak.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Odorantes , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Alimentos , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Neuroscience ; 452: 126-137, 2021 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33197506

RESUMEN

Flavor perception results from the integration of at least odor and taste. Evidence for such integration is that odors can have taste properties (odor-induced taste). Most brain areas involved in flavor perception are high-level areas; however, primary gustatory and olfactory areas also show activations in response to a combination of odor and taste. While the regions involved in flavor perception are now quite well identified, the network's organization is not yet understood. Using a close to real salty soup model with electroencephalography brain recording, we evaluated whether odor-induced saltiness enhancement would result in differences of amplitude and/or latency in late cognitive P3 peak mostly and/or in P1 early sensory peak. Three target solutions were created from the same base of green-pea soup: i) with a "usual" salt concentration (PPS2), ii) with "reduced" salt (PPS1: -50%), and iii) with reduced salt and a "beef stock" odor (PPS1B). Sensory data showed that the beef odor produced saltiness enhancement in PPS1B in comparison to PPS1. As the main EEG result, the late cognitive P3 peak was delayed by 25 ms in the odor-added solution PPS1B compared to PPS1. The odor alone did not explain this peak amplitude and higher latency in the P3 peak. These results support the classical view that high-level integratory areas process odor-taste interactions with potential top-down effects on primary sensory regions.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Olfatoria , Gusto , Animales , Encéfalo , Bovinos , Odorantes , Olfato , Percepción del Gusto
6.
Chem Senses ; 462021 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33347544

RESUMEN

Olfactory habituation corresponds to a decreased behavioral or perceptual response to an odor after a prolonged exposure to this odor. Our aim was to investigate whether long-term olfactory habituation and its recovery are similar in young (<35 years old) and older adults (>50). Fifty-seven participants were recruited for a 5-week longitudinal study. They were exposed to one of the two odors (manzanate alpha [MA], irone alpha [IA]) for 2 weeks at home. Olfactory detection thresholds for both odors were measured before and after exposure. Results showed that the two age groups behaved similarly. The long-term exposure to an odor led to a temporary increase of its detection threshold (lower sensitivity to the odor). IA thresholds were more sensitive to the duration of exposure with the odor than MA thresholds. One week after termination of exposure, participants fully recovered and even became more sensitive to both odors. No cross-habituation was found between the two odors. Our findings highlight that long-term habituation is specific to the odor exposed, behaves the same in young and older adults, and is fully reversible in both age groups after 1 week.


Asunto(s)
Habituación Psicofisiológica/fisiología , Norisoprenoides/química , Odorantes , Olfato/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología
7.
Physiol Behav ; 214: 112751, 2020 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31783050

RESUMEN

Previously published experiments established the time-course of olfactory habituation in humans, and extracted 3 centroid curves from clustering analysis that reflected high, middle and low habituation. The aim of the current theoretical study was to further analyse these previous experimental data by developing a mathematical modelling analysis designed for fitting the 3 curves from a general equation. After adjusting equation parameters for each curve, fitting equation outcomes on experimental data yielded high correlation coefficients of 0.9997 - 0.9995 - 0.9962, respectively. A model-based interpretation of olfactory-habituation centroid curves is proposed suggesting that they result from the effect of 2 separate processes that act simultaneously. We suggest that the first process is unlikely related to the olfactory habituation itself, and, rather, is of unclear origin. The second process seems to play a major role in the degree of OH and cannot be assigned, a priori, to either peripheral or central adaptation, respectively.


Asunto(s)
Habituación Psicofisiológica/fisiología , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos
8.
J Agric Food Chem ; 66(10): 2312-2318, 2018 Mar 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27951642

RESUMEN

The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of (1) the addition of trigeminal stimuli to an olfactory stimulus and (2) the congruence in the odorous mixture after repeated odor presentation. Twenty-five normosmic volunteers were enrolled and presented stimulation blocks, consisting of three habituation stimuli (H) (orange odor), one dishabituation (DH) (control condition, orange odor; congruent condition, orange odor + CO2; incongruent condition, orange odor + l-isopulegol), and one dishabituated stimulus (D) (orange odor). Olfactory event-related potentials were analyzed. Response amplitudes differed significantly in the incongruent condition (N1P2 between H3 and D; peak to peak N1P2 at electrode positions Cz, Fz, and Pz; response amplitudes between H3 and DH). The addition of CO2 modified the perception of orange odor, pronouncing a fruity note, whereas the addition of l-isopulegol as a DH pronounced the l-isopulegol note. This study provides evidence that incongruent trigeminal-olfactory stimulants increase the response to subsequent olfactory stimulus.


Asunto(s)
Citrus sinensis/metabolismo , Olfato , Adulto , Citrus sinensis/química , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes/análisis , Umbral Sensorial , Adulto Joven
9.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 38(12): 5958-5969, 2017 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28901711

RESUMEN

Semantic description of odors is a cognitively demanding task. Learning to name smells is, however, possible with training. This study set out to examine how improvement in olfactory semantic knowledge following training reorganizes the neural representation of smells. First, 19 nonexpert volunteers were trained for 3 days; they were exposed (i) to odorants presented without verbal labels (perceptual learning) and (ii) to other odorants paired with lexicosemantic labels (associative learning). Second, the same participants were tested in a brain imaging study (fMRI) measuring hemodynamic responses to learned odors presented in both the perceptual and associative learning conditions. The lexicosemantic training enhanced the ability to describe smells semantically. Neurally, this change was associated with enhanced activity in a set of heteromodal areas-including superior frontal gyrus-and parietal areas. These findings demonstrate that odor-name associative learning induces recruitment of brain areas involved in the integration and representation of semantic attributes of sensory events. They also offer new insights into the brain plasticity underlying the acquisition of olfactory expertise in lay people. Hum Brain Mapp 38:5958-5969, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Odorantes , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Semántica , Vocabulario , Adulto , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Mapeo Encefálico , Circulación Cerebrovascular/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Psicolingüística , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología
10.
Sci Rep ; 7: 41047, 2017 01 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28120877

RESUMEN

Habituation is a filter that optimizes the processing of information by our brain in all sensory modalities. It results in an unconscious reduced responsiveness to continuous or repetitive stimulation. In olfaction, the main question is whether habituation works the same way for any odorant or whether we habituate differently to each odorant? In particular, whether chemical, physical or perceptual cues can limit or increase habituation. To test this, the odour intensity of 32 odorants differing in physicochemical characteristics was rated by 58 participants continuously during 120s. Each odorant was delivered at a constant concentration. Results showed odorants differed significantly in habituation, highlighting the multifactoriality of habituation. Additionally habituation was predicted from 15 physico-chemical and perceptual characteristics of the odorants. The analysis highlighted the importance of trigeminality which is highly correlated to intensity and pleasantness. The vapour pressure, the molecular weight, the Odor Activity Value (OAV) and the number of double bonds mostly contributed to the modulation of habituation. Moreover, length of the carbon chain, number of conformers and hydrophobicity contributed to a lesser extent to the modulation of habituation. These results highlight new principles involved in the fundamental process of habituation, notably trigeminality and the physicochemical characteristics associated.


Asunto(s)
Habituación Psicofisiológica , Olfato , Adulto , Fenómenos Químicos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Odorantes , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...