Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 22
Filtrar
Más filtros












Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Fluency Disord ; 80: 106059, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38640516

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Preschool-age children use mental state verbs (MSVs; e.g., think, know) to reference thoughts and other cognitive states. In play-based language, MSV use requires conversational flexibility, as speakers shift from discussion of actions happening in the here-and-now to more abstract discussion of mental states. Some evidence suggests that children who stutter (CWS) demonstrate subtle differences in shifting on experimental tasks of cognitive flexibility, differences which may extend to conversational flexibility. This study explored MSV use in conversational language between CWS and their mothers. METHODS: Thirty-five preschool-age CWS and 35 age- and gender-matched children who do not stutter (CWNS), all performing within the typical range on standardized language testing, conversed with their mothers during play. Samples were transcribed and coded for MSV use. RESULTS: No between-group differences were observed in MSV use, either between the CWS and CWNS or between the groups of mothers. Age and language skills were positively associated with MSV use in the CWNS group only. For both groups of dyads, mothers' MSV use corresponded at least to some extent to their children's language skills. Finally, correspondence between CWNS and their mothers was observed for two conversational language measures, representing lexical diversity and morphosyntax; this overall pattern was not observed in the CWS dyad group. CONCLUSIONS: Although these findings point to similar use of MSVs among the groups of children and their mothers, for the CWS group, the patterns of use in relation to age and language skills are somewhat different from developmental expectations.


Asunto(s)
Madres , Juego e Implementos de Juego , Tartamudeo , Humanos , Tartamudeo/psicología , Preescolar , Femenino , Masculino , Madres/psicología , Juego e Implementos de Juego/psicología , Relaciones Madre-Hijo/psicología , Lenguaje Infantil , Estudios de Casos y Controles
3.
Semin Speech Lang ; 43(5): 406-425, 2022 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36288737

RESUMEN

School-age children encounter expository discourse daily in the classroom, and skilled understanding and production of expository language is critical for academic success. The purpose of this study was to compare children's production of two types of expository discourse, generation and retell, while employing a scaffolded note-taking procedure to assist children in developing their samples. Twenty-six typically developing children, 9 to 12 years of age, participated in the study. For the expository generation task, children gave an explanation of a favorite activity, and for the retell task they viewed a video and provided an explanation of the information in the video. Overall, expository generation samples were longer and richer in content, but expository retell samples demonstrated greater lexical diversity. In addition, generation samples contained fewer grammatical errors than retell samples (trend), but measures of syntactic complexity, which were positively related across tasks, were not significantly different between sample types. Findings suggest that using a scaffolded procedure for supporting expository production resulted in (1) samples that were long enough for valid analysis of the children's language and (2) the production of utterances that, on average, were longer than reported in similar studies without scaffolding. The potential impact of comprehension on retell task performance is discussed.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Lenguaje , Niño , Humanos
4.
J Fluency Disord ; 66: 105792, 2020 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33032169

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Attention develops gradually from infancy to the preschool years and beyond. Exogenous attention, consisting of automatic responses to salient stimuli, develops in infancy, whereas endogenous attention, or voluntary attention, begins to develop later, in the preschool years. The purpose of this study was to examine (a) exogenous and endogenous attention in young children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS) through two conditions of a visual sustained selective attention task, and (b) visual short-term memory (STM) between groups within the context of this task. METHOD: 42 CWS and 42 CWNS, ages 3;0-5;5 (years;months), were pair-matched in age, gender (31 males, 11 females per group), and socioeconomic status. Children completed a visual tracking task (Track-It Task; Fisher et al., 2013) requiring sustained selective attention and engaging exogenous and endogenous processes. Following each item, children were asked to recall the item they had tracked, as a memory check. RESULTS: The CWS group demonstrated significantly less accuracy in overall tracking and visual memory for the tracked stimuli, compared to the CWNS group. Across groups, the children performed better in sustained selective attention when the target stimuli were more salient (the condition tapping both exogenous and endogenous attention) than when stimuli were less so (the condition tapping primarily endogenous processes). CONCLUSIONS: Relative to peers, preschool-age CWS, as a group, display weaknesses in visual sustained selective attention and visual STM.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Tartamudeo/fisiopatología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Tartamudeo/diagnóstico , Tartamudeo/psicología
5.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 63(11): 3659-3679, 2020 11 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33108236

RESUMEN

Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine cognitive flexibility for semantic and perceptual information in preschool children who stutter (CWS) and who do not stutter (CWNS). Method Participants were 44 CWS and 44 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11 (years;months). Cognitive flexibility was measured using semantic and perceptual categorization tasks. In each task, children were required to match a target object with two different semantic or perceptual associates. Main dependent variables were reaction time and accuracy. Results The accuracy with which CWS and CWNS shifted between one semantic and perceptual representation to another was similar, but the CWS did so significantly more slowly. Both groups of children had more difficulty switching between perceptual representations than semantic ones. Conclusion CWS are less efficient (slower), though not less accurate, than CWNS in their ability to switch between different representations in both the verbal and nonverbal domains.


Asunto(s)
Semántica , Tartamudeo , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Cognición , Humanos , Lactante , Tiempo de Reacción
6.
J Child Lang ; 47(5): 1006-1029, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32252839

RESUMEN

Purpose: Although school-age children learn most new word meanings from surrounding context, the joint roles of language ability and executive function (EF) in the word learning process remain unclear. This study examined children's acquisition of word meanings from context in relation to oral language ability and three EF skills (working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility). Method: Typically developing school-age children completed measures of language and EF, then read and listened to short stories containing unfamiliar target words. A multiple-choice pretest-posttest measure assessed children's target word knowledge gains. Results: Regression analyses showed that language and cognitive flexibility were both related to word knowledge gains; each skill assumed greater importance among children with relative weakness in the other skill. Conclusion: Language ability and cognitive flexibility may each play a direct role in contextual word learning among school-age children, with children naturally relying on one skill if the other is weaker.


Asunto(s)
Aptitud , Función Ejecutiva , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Aprendizaje Verbal , Niño , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Inhibición Psicológica , Trastornos del Desarrollo del Lenguaje/psicología , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Lectura , Instituciones Académicas
7.
J Fluency Disord ; 63: 105747, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32058092

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Numerous "small N" studies of language ability in children who stutter have produced differing conclusions. We combined test and spontaneous language data from a large cohort of children who stutter (CWS) and typically fluent peers, gathered from independent laboratories across the US, to appraise a variety of lexical measures. METHOD: Standardized receptive and expressive vocabulary test data and spontaneous language samples from 99 pairs of CWS (ages 25-100 months), and age-, gender-, and SES-matched children who do not stutter (CWNS) were compared. Language sample transcripts were analyzed with four measures of lexical diversity. Correlations between lexical diversity measures and expressive vocabulary scores were also calculated. RESULTS: On standardized tests of both receptive and expressive vocabulary, there were significant differences between CWS and CWNS. In contrast, on spontaneous language measures of lexical diversity, CWS did not differ in their lexical diversity, across analyses, compared to CWNS. Three of the four lexical diversity analyses, MATTR, VocD, and NDW, were significantly correlated with each other. CONCLUSIONS: We were able to confirm prior findings of relative disadvantage on standardized vocabulary tests for a very large sample of well-matched CWS. However, spontaneous language measures of lexical diversity did not distinguish the groups. This relative weakness in CWS may emerge from task differences: CWS are free to encode their own spontaneous utterances but must comply with explicit lexical prompts in standardized testing situations.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Tartamudeo , Vocabulario , Algoritmos , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Lingüística , Masculino
8.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 62(3): 644-667, 2019 03 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30950742

RESUMEN

Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine the verbal short-term memory skills of children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS) in 2 experiments, focusing on the influence of phonological and semantic similarity. Method Participants were 42 CWS and 42 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11 (years;months). In Experiment 1, children completed the phonological similarity task, in which they listened to lists of phonologically similar and dissimilar words and then repeated them when signaled to do so. In Experiment 2, children completed another forward span task, the semantic category task, which is similar to the phonological similarity task, except that it consisted of lists of semantically homogeneous and heterogeneous words. Main dependent variables were cumulative memory span, proportion of errors by type, and speech reaction time (SRT) for correct responses. Results The CWS exhibited significantly shorter memory spans for phonologically dissimilar words and were less affected by the phonological qualities of the words than the CWNS in Experiment 1, based on the findings of both between-groups and within-group analyses. In Experiment 2, although the groups did not differ in their performance in either condition, within-group analyses revealed that the CWNS benefitted from semantic similarity, whereas the CWS did not. The between-groups difference in absolute difference scores, however, did not reach significance. The CWS produced more omissions and false alarms than the CWNS in both experiments, but the 2 groups of children were otherwise comparable in SRT, although the CWS exhibited overall faster SRT than the CWNS in Experiment 2. Conclusions Verbal short-term memory is one domain-general cognitive process in which CWS display weakness relative to typically fluent peers. These weaknesses are likely due, in part, to differences in phonological and, perhaps, semantic processing of words to aid memory.


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo , Fonética , Semántica , Tartamudeo/psicología , Aprendizaje Verbal , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción
10.
J Fluency Disord ; 56: 18-32, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29443692

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The purpose of this study was to examine the executive function (EF) abilities of preschool children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS) using a parent-report questionnaire and a behavioral task. METHOD: Participants were 75 CWS and 75 CWNS between the ages of 3;0 and 5;11 (years; months). Parents rated their children's EF abilities using the Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P; Gioia, Espy, & Isquith, 2003). Children's ability to integrate cognitive flexibility, inhibitory control, and working memory was measured using a behavioral task, the Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders (HTKS; Cameron Ponitz, McClelland, Matthews, & Morrison, 2009). RESULTS: The CWS were judged by their parents as being less proficient in working memory, shift/flexibility, and overall EF than the parents of the CWNS. Children in the CWS group were also 2½ to 7 times more likely than children in the CWNS group to exhibit clinically significant difficulties with EF. Behavioral task findings revealed that 3-year old CWS performed more poorly than their peers on the HTKS. Parental ratings of executive function and working memory were significantly and moderately correlated with receptive and expressive vocabulary skills only for the CWNS group. CONCLUSION: CWS have more difficulty with EF in everyday life and may experience early delays in their ability to integrate aspects of attention and EF compared to CWNS.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Tartamudeo/diagnóstico , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Padres
11.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 60(4): 836-852, 2017 04 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28384673

RESUMEN

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to examine (a) explicit and implicit verbal response inhibition in preschool children who do stutter (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS) and (b) the relationship between response inhibition and language skills. Method: Participants were 41 CWS and 41 CWNS between the ages of 3;1 and 6;1 (years;months). Explicit verbal response inhibition was measured using a computerized version of the grass-snow task (Carlson & Moses, 2001), and implicit verbal response inhibition was measured using the baa-meow task. Main dependent variables were reaction time and accuracy. Results: The CWS were significantly less accurate than the CWNS on the implicit task, but not the explicit task. The CWS also exhibited slower reaction times than the CWNS on both tasks. Between-group differences in performance could not be attributed to working memory demands. Overall, children's performance on the inhibition tasks corresponded with parents' perceptions of their children's inhibition skills in daily life. Conclusions: CWS are less effective and efficient than CWNS in suppressing a dominant response while executing a conflicting response in the verbal domain.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Inhibición Psicológica , Tartamudeo , Conducta Verbal , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Preescolar , Conflicto Psicológico , Femenino , Pruebas Auditivas , Humanos , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Destreza Motora , Padres , Tiempo de Reacción
13.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 24(1): 60-71, 2015 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25409978

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Incidental reading provides a powerful opportunity for partial word knowledge growth in the school-age years. The extent to which children of differing language abilities can use reading experiences to glean partial knowledge of words is not well understood. The purpose of this study was to compare semantic-syntactic partial word knowledge growth of children with higher language skills (HL group; overall language standard scores of 85 or higher) to that of children with relatively lower language skills (LL group; overall receptive or expressive standard score below 85). METHOD: Thirty-two children, 16 per group, silently read stories containing unfamiliar nouns and verbs 3 times over a 1-week period. Semantic-syntactic partial word knowledge growth was assessed after each reading and 2-3 days later to assess retention. RESULTS: Over time, both groups showed significant partial word knowledge growth, with the HL group showing significantly more growth. In addition, both groups retained knowledge several days later. CONCLUSION: Regardless of language skill level, children benefit from multiple exposures to unfamiliar words in reading in their development and retention of semantic-syntactic partial word knowledge growth.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Lenguaje/terapia , Fonética , Psicolingüística , Lectura , Semántica , Aprendizaje Verbal , Vocabulario , Logro , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Trastornos del Lenguaje/psicología , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Valores de Referencia , Retención en Psicología
14.
J Fluency Disord ; 37(4): 314-24, 2012 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23218214

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: The aim of this study was to identify whether different patterns of errors exist in irregular past-tense verbs in children who stutter (CWS) and children who do not stutter (CWNS). METHOD: Spontaneous language samples of thirty-one age- and gender-matched pairs of children (total N=62) between the ages of 24 months and 59 months were analyzed. RESULTS: Results indicated that children who do and do not stutter over-regularize irregular past-tense verbs (i.e., saying runned for ran) with comparable frequency. However, two nonsignificant trends which suggest possible intra-group differences were noted. First, irregular past tense verbs represented a greater portion of total verbs for CWS than for CWNS. Second, CWS appeared to double-mark (i.e., say ranned for ran) more often than CWNS. Results are discussed in light of theories about the acquisition of the irregular past-tense and about differences in language skills between CWS and CWNS. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: After reading this article, the reader will be able to: (a) summarize previous findings about connections between stuttering and language in CWS and CWNS; (b) describe similarities and differences between irregular past-tense verb use and errors in CWS and CWNS; (c) discuss possible connections between the declarative-procedural model and stuttering.


Asunto(s)
Habla , Tartamudeo/psicología , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Lingüística , Masculino
15.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 21(2): 140-53, 2012 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22411772

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Natural reading experiences provide an opportunity for the development of orthographic word knowledge as well as other forms of partial word knowledge. The purpose of this study was to compare the orthographic word knowledge growth of school-age children with relatively low language skills (LL group) to that of age- and gender-matched peers with high language skills (HL group). METHOD: Thirty-two children, 16 per group, read stories containing rare words 3 times, 2-3 days apart. Posttesting, completed at the end of each session, required participants to indicate recognition of the rare words encountered in the stories while not indicating recognition of orthographically similar nonwords. RESULTS: Over time, both groups showed significant growth in recognition of the orthographic forms of the rare words. However, the groups differed in the extent to which they indicated that the orthographically similar nonwords were words, with the LL group indicating that significantly more of the nonwords were words. CONCLUSION: Results provide some preliminary evidence that children with relatively weaker language skills are able to develop orthographic knowledge of unfamiliar words through reading experiences, but their orthographic representations may not be as well defined as those of children with stronger language skills.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Lectura , Vocabulario , Adolescente , Niño , Evaluación Educacional , Femenino , Humanos , Pruebas del Lenguaje , Masculino , Narración , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Estudiantes
16.
J Fluency Disord ; 35(3): 216-34, 2010 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20831969

RESUMEN

UNLABELLED: Relatively recently, experimental studies of linguistic processing speed in children who stutter (CWS) have emerged, some of which suggest differences in performance among CWS compared to children who do not stutter (CWNS). What is not yet well understood is the extent to which underlying cognitive skills may impact performance on timed tasks of linguistic performance. The purpose of this study was to explore possible relationships between measures of linguistic processing speed and two aspects of cognition: phonological working memory and attention. Participants were 9 CWS and 14 CWNS between the ages of 3;6 and 5;2. Children participated in a computerized picture naming task (an index of linguistic processing speed) and a nonword repetition task (an index of phonological working memory). Parents completed a temperament behavior questionnaire, from which information about the children's attentional skills was collected. Findings revealed that the groups did not differ from each other on speed of picture naming or attention; however, the CWS performed significantly worse in nonword repetition. In addition, after partialling out the effects of age, (a) for CWS only, there was a significant negative relationship between picture naming speed and nonword repetition; (b) there were no significant relationships for either group between aspects of attention and picture naming speed; and (c) only the CWNS showed a significant relationship between nonword repetition and focused attentional skills. These results underscore the need to consider the underlying skills associated with lexically related aspects of language production when examining the task performances of CWS and CWNS. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able to: (a) summarize findings from previous studies examining the speech and language performance of children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS); (b) describe findings of previous studies related to nonword repetition and attention in CWS; (c) compare the results of the present study with previous work in this area; and (d) discuss speculations concerning the relationship between linguistic processing speed, phonological working memory, and attention in CWS and CWNS.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Habla , Tartamudeo/psicología , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lingüística , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Temperamento
17.
Mo Med ; 106(2): 136-40, 2009.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19397114

RESUMEN

People hold many stereotypes about the development of language and reading. We review current data concerning three stereotypes: (1) Girls learn language faster than boys; (2) Biological factors determine the time course of language acquisition; and (3) Dyslexia is a visual problem involving the reversal of letters. In each case, recent data leads to a different picture of the acquisition of language and literacy and deepens our understanding of the processes involved.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia/fisiopatología , Escolaridad , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Lectura , Estereotipo , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino
18.
J Fluency Disord ; 34(4): 242-56, 2009 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20113769

RESUMEN

UNLABELLED: Young children with typical fluency demonstrate a range of disfluencies, or speech disruptions. One type of disruption, revision, appears to increase in frequency as syntactic skills develop. To date, this phenomenon has not been studied in children who stutter (CWS). Rispoli, Hadley, and Holt (2008) suggest a schema for categorizing speech disruptions in terms of revisions and stalls. The purpose of this exploratory study was to use this schema to evaluate whether CWS show a pattern over time in their production of stuttering, revisions, and stalls. Nine CWS, ages 2;1 to 4;11, participated in the study, producing language samples each month for 10 months. MLU and vocd analyses were performed for samples across three time periods. Active declarative sentences within these samples were examined for the presence of disruptions. Results indicated that the proportion of sentences containing revisions increased over time, but proportions for stalls and stuttering did not. Visual inspection revealed that more stuttering and stalls occurred on longer utterances than on shorter utterances. Upon examination of individual children's language, it appears two-thirds of the children showed a pattern in which, as MLU increased, revisions increased as well. Findings are similar to studies of children with typical fluency, suggesting that, despite the fact that CWS display more (and different) disfluencies relative to typically fluent peers, revisions appear to increase over time and correspond to increases in MLU, just as is the case with peers. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able to: (1) describe the three types of speech disruptions assessed in this article; (2) compare present findings of disruptions in children who stutter to findings of previous research with children who are typically fluent; and (3) discuss future directions in this area of research, given the findings and implications of this study.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje Infantil , Habla , Tartamudeo , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Psicolingüística , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Medición de la Producción del Habla , Factores de Tiempo
19.
J Fluency Disord ; 32(2): 79-94, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17499123

RESUMEN

UNLABELLED: Several recent studies have suggested that young children who stutter (CWS) tend to show depressed lexical performance relative to peers. Given the developmental literature as well as several studies of verb processing in individuals who stutter, verbs may pose a particular challenge for this group. The purpose of the present study was to examine verb use in CWS. In theory, if young CWS differ in their production of verbs, this finding would partially explain the findings of studies that probed conversational vocabulary skills more generally. Fifteen CWS and 15 children who do not stutter (CWNS) participated in a play-based conversational sample with a parent. Samples were analyzed for the total number of verbs, the number of different verbs, and the proportion of general all-purpose (GAP) verbs within the samples. CWS produced significantly fewer different verbs and total verbs than the CWNS. However, previously reported near-significant differences in utterance length between groups would appear to temper the robustness of this finding. The groups did not differ in the proportion of GAP verbs used, suggesting that the CWS did not over-rely on GAP verbs in conversational language production but rather used these verbs to the same extent as their peers. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: As a result of this activity, the participant will be able to: (1) relate the purpose and rationale for examining verb use in children who stutter (CWS); (2) summarize the procedures used to assess verb use and GAP verb use in the present study; (3) explain the findings of the present study; (4) relate findings to the extant literature on lexical diversity in CWS.


Asunto(s)
Lingüística/estadística & datos numéricos , Periodicidad , Tartamudeo/diagnóstico , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Vocabulario
20.
J Fluency Disord ; 31(3): 177-99, 2006.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16814376

RESUMEN

UNLABELLED: The purpose of this study was to assess the nonword repetition skills of 24 children who do (CWS; n = 12) and do not stutter (CWNS; n = 12) between the ages of 3;0 and 5;2. Findings revealed that CWS produced significantly fewer correct two- and three-syllable nonword repetitions and made significantly more phoneme errors on three-syllable nonwords relative to CWNS. In addition, there was a significant relationship between performance on a test of expressive phonology and nonword repetition for CWS, but not CWNS. Findings further revealed no significant fluctuation in fluency as nonwords increased in length. Taken together, findings lend support to previous work, suggesting that nonword repetition skills differ for CWS compared with CWNS, and that these findings cannot be attributed to (a) weak language performance on the part of CWS, or (b) the occurrence of stuttering in the course of nonword production. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: After reading this article, the learner will be able to: (a) describe one common means of assessing phonological working memory in children; (b) summarize the performance differences of children who stutter compared to peers on a nonword repetition task; (c) compare the results of the present study with previous work in this area.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Fonética , Tartamudeo/fisiopatología , Conducta Verbal , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Análisis de Regresión , Medición de la Producción del Habla
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...