Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 2.252
Filtrar
2.
J Psychiatr Res ; 178: 88-93, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39128220

RESUMEN

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) is a prevalent and disabling disorder with a high degree of comorbidity. Clinical studies have focused on hallucinations, which could be associated with the severity of the disorder and treatment resistance. Auditory illusions have received little attention so far, possibly because they are particularly difficult to assess. However, they may impact functioning, and underdiagnosis could impair prognosis. To provide a clearer understanding of PTSD psychopathology, this paper proposes to focus on these auditory illusions. A monocentric case-control study was conducted on 30 subjects with PTSD and 30 controls, based on an original design. False recognitions were estimated during a sound test created with a vocoder. Additionally, differences in emotional valence, dissociation, hyperarousal, and reliving were assessed. The study found that individuals with PTSD experience a higher frequency of auditory illusions compared to healthy controls (65% versus 20%, p < 0.001). Additionally, the emotional valence of these illusions was more negative in individuals with PTSD than in controls. The study also identified a correlation between dissociation symptoms, hyperarousal and reliving with auditory illusions. These findings are in line with neurobiological studies of PTSD, as well as cognitive predictive models, and support the hypothesis of a significant prevalence of auditory illusions in PTSD. Like hallucinations, auditory illusions may be influenced by dissociation. Although these results are preliminary, they suggest a need for further investigation into auditory illusions in PTSD and their effect on prognosis.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático , Humanos , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/fisiopatología , Trastornos por Estrés Postraumático/diagnóstico , Femenino , Masculino , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Adulto , Ilusiones/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Alucinaciones/fisiopatología , Alucinaciones/etiología , Alucinaciones/diagnóstico , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Adulto Joven
3.
J Appl Physiol (1985) ; 137(4): 800-813, 2024 Oct 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39116345

RESUMEN

Local vibration (LV) mainly stimulates primary afferents (Ia) and can induce a tonic vibration reflex (TVR) and an illusion of movement. This study aimed to evaluate the effect of these two phenomena on maximal voluntary isometric contraction (MVIC) capacity. LV (80 Hz) was applied to the wrist flexor muscles in two randomized experiments for 6 min. LV conditions were adjusted to promote either TVR (visual focus on the vibrated wrist) or ILLUSION [hand hidden, visual focus on electromyographic activity of the flexor carpi radialis muscle (FCR)]. Mechanical and electromyographic (EMG) responses of the FCR and extensor carpi radialis muscles were recorded during MVIC in flexion and extension and during electrically evoked contractions at supramaximal intensity. Measurements were performed before (10 min and just before) and after (0 and 30 min) LV protocol. An increase in FCR EMG was observed during LV in the TVR condition (+340%) compared with the illusion condition (P = 0.003). In contrast, the movement illusion was greater in the ILLUSION condition (assessed through subjective scales) (P = 0.004). MVIC was reduced in flexion only after the TVR condition ([Formula: see text], all P < 0.034). Moreover, the decrease in force was correlated with the amount of TVR recorded on the FCR muscle (r = -0.64, P = 0.005). Although potentiated doublets of each muscle did not evolve differently between conditions, a decrease was observed between the first and the last measure. In conclusion, when conducting research to assess maximal strength, it is necessary to have better control and reporting of the phenomena induced during LV.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The maximal force production of the vibrated muscle is reduced after 6 min of LV only in TVR condition. Furthermore, the amount of TVR is negatively correlated with this force decrease. When measuring the effects of LV on maximal force production, it is important to control and report any phenomena induced during vibration, such as TVR or movement illusion, which can be achieved by recording EMG activity of vibrated muscle and quantifying illusion.


Asunto(s)
Electromiografía , Ilusiones , Contracción Isométrica , Movimiento , Músculo Esquelético , Reflejo , Vibración , Muñeca , Humanos , Masculino , Muñeca/fisiología , Adulto , Músculo Esquelético/fisiología , Reflejo/fisiología , Contracción Isométrica/fisiología , Electromiografía/métodos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Femenino , Adulto Joven
4.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 249: 104386, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39174407

RESUMEN

Virtual Reality has significantly improved the understanding of body experience, through techniques such as Body Illusion. Body Illusion allows individuals to perceive an artificial body as their own, changing body perceptual and affective components. Prior research has predominantly focused on female participants, leaving the impact of Body Illusion on males less understood. This study seeks to fill this gap by examining the nuanced bodily experiences of men in comparison to women. 40 participants (20 females and 20 males) were proposed with visuo-tactile synchronous and asynchronous Body Illusion to explore changes in body satisfaction and body size estimation across three critical areas: shoulders, hips, and waist. Results revealed significant initial disparities, with females displaying greater body dissatisfaction and a tendency to overestimate body size. After Body Illusion, females adjusted the hips perceived body size closer to that of the virtual body and reported increased body satisfaction independent of the condition. Conversely, males showed changes only in waist size estimation only after synchronous stimulation without significant shifts in body satisfaction. These results suggest a higher sensitivity of women to embodied experiences, potentially due to societal influences and a greater inclination towards self-objectification. These insights pave the way for creating more refined and effective interventions for body image issues, highlighting the importance of incorporating gender-specific considerations in VR-based prevention and therapeutical programs.


Asunto(s)
Imagen Corporal , Ilusiones , Realidad Virtual , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Factores Sexuales , Satisfacción Personal , Tamaño Corporal/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Insatisfacción Corporal
5.
Cognition ; 253: 105874, 2024 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39216190

RESUMEN

Perception has long been envisioned to use an internal model of the world to explain the causes of sensory signals. However, such accounts have historically not been testable, typically requiring intractable search through the space of possible explanations. Using auditory scenes as a case study, we leveraged contemporary computational tools to infer explanations of sounds in a candidate internal generative model of the auditory world (ecologically inspired audio synthesizers). Model inferences accounted for many classic illusions. Unlike traditional accounts of auditory illusions, the model is applicable to any sound, and exhibited human-like perceptual organization for real-world sound mixtures. The combination of stimulus-computability and interpretable model structure enabled 'rich falsification', revealing additional assumptions about sound generation needed to account for perception. The results show how generative models can account for the perception of both classic illusions and everyday sensory signals, and illustrate the opportunities and challenges involved in incorporating them into theories of perception.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Humanos , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos , Estimulación Acústica
6.
Multisens Res ; 37(4-5): 341-363, 2024 Aug 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39191410

RESUMEN

Congruent visual speech improves speech perception accuracy, particularly in noisy environments. Conversely, mismatched visual speech can alter what is heard, leading to an illusory percept that differs from the auditory and visual components, known as the McGurk effect. While prior transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and neuroimaging studies have identified the left posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) as a causal region involved in the generation of the McGurk effect, it remains unclear whether this region is critical only for this illusion or also for the more general benefits of congruent visual speech (e.g., increased accuracy and faster reaction times). Indeed, recent correlative research suggests that the benefits of congruent visual speech and the McGurk effect rely on largely independent mechanisms. To better understand how these different features of audiovisual integration are causally generated by the left pSTS, we used single-pulse TMS to temporarily disrupt processing within this region while subjects were presented with either congruent or incongruent (McGurk) audiovisual combinations. Consistent with past research, we observed that TMS to the left pSTS reduced the strength of the McGurk effect. Importantly, however, left pSTS stimulation had no effect on the positive benefits of congruent audiovisual speech (increased accuracy and faster reaction times), demonstrating a causal dissociation between the two processes. Our results are consistent with models proposing that the pSTS is but one of multiple critical areas supporting audiovisual speech interactions. Moreover, these data add to a growing body of evidence suggesting that the McGurk effect is an imperfect surrogate measure for more general and ecologically valid audiovisual speech behaviors.


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Habla , Lóbulo Temporal , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Estimulación Acústica , Ilusiones/fisiología
7.
Brain Behav ; 14(8): e3570, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39192611

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) refers to individuals' perceived decline in memory and/or other cognitive abilities relative to their previous level of performance. Sensory decline is one of the main manifestations of decline in older adults with SCD. The efficient integration of visual and auditory information, known as audiovisual integration, is a crucial perceptual process. This study aims to evaluate audiovisual integration in older adults with SCD. METHODS: We adopted the audiovisual detection task, the Colavita task, and the Sound-Induced Flash Illusion (SIFI) task to evaluate the audiovisual integration by examining both redundant and illusory effects. Older adults diagnosed with SCD (N = 50, mean age = 67.8 years) and a control group of non-SCD older adults (N = 51, mean age = 66.5 years) were recruited. All participants took part in the three aforementioned experiments. RESULTS: The outcomes showed that a redundant effect occurred in both SCD and non-SCD older adults, with SCD older adults gaining more benefits in audiovisual detection task. Moreover, an equivalent amount of the visual dominance effect was observed among both SCD and non-SCD older adults in Colavita task. In addition, older adults with SCD perceived an equal fission illusion but a bigger fusion illusion compared with non-SCD older adults in SIFI task. CONCLUSIONS: Overall, older adults with SCD exhibit increased audiovisual redundant effects and stronger fusion illusion susceptibility compared to non-SCD older adults. Besides, visual dominance was observed in both groups via the Colavita task, with no significant difference between non-SCD and SCD older adults. These findings implied that audiovisual integration might offer a potential way for the identification of SCD.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Disfunción Cognitiva , Ilusiones , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Anciano , Femenino , Masculino , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad
8.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(8)2024 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39110411

RESUMEN

Speech perception requires the binding of spatiotemporally disjoint auditory-visual cues. The corresponding brain network-level information processing can be characterized by two complementary mechanisms: functional segregation which refers to the localization of processing in either isolated or distributed modules across the brain, and integration which pertains to cooperation among relevant functional modules. Here, we demonstrate using functional magnetic resonance imaging recordings that subjective perceptual experience of multisensory speech stimuli, real and illusory, are represented in differential states of segregation-integration. We controlled the inter-subject variability of illusory/cross-modal perception parametrically, by introducing temporal lags in the incongruent auditory-visual articulations of speech sounds within the McGurk paradigm. The states of segregation-integration balance were captured using two alternative computational approaches. First, the module responsible for cross-modal binding of sensory signals defined as the perceptual binding network (PBN) was identified using standardized parametric statistical approaches and their temporal correlations with all other brain areas were computed. With increasing illusory perception, the majority of the nodes of PBN showed decreased cooperation with the rest of the brain, reflecting states of high segregation but reduced global integration. Second, using graph theoretic measures, the altered patterns of segregation-integration were cross-validated.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Percepción del Habla , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Estimulación Acústica , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología
10.
Pain ; 165(10): 2370-2378, 2024 Oct 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39185673

RESUMEN

ABSTRACT: The thermal grill illusion (TGI), a phenomenon in which the juxtaposition of innocuous warm and cold temperatures on the skin elicits a burning sensation, offers a unique perspective to how pain occurs in response to harmless stimuli. We investigated the role of the spinal cord in the generation of the TGI across 2 experiments (total n = 80). We applied heat and cold stimuli to dermatomes, areas of skin innervated by a single spinal nerve, that mapped onto adjacent or nonadjacent spinal segments. Enhanced warm and burning ratings during the TGI were observed when cold and warm stimuli were confined within the same dermatome. Furthermore, we found the spatial organisation of warm and cold stimuli within and across dermatomes affected TGI perception. Perceived warmth and burning intensity increased when the cold stimulus projected to the segment more caudal to the warm stimulus, whereas perceived cold during the TGI decreased compared with the opposite spatial arrangement. This suggests that the perception of TGI is enhanced when cold afferents are projected to spinal segments positioned caudally in relation to those receiving warm afferents. Our results indicate distinct interaction of sensory pathways based on the segmental arrangement of afferent fibres and are consistent with current interpretations of the spread and integration of thermosensory information along the spinal cord.


Asunto(s)
Frío , Calor , Ilusiones , Médula Espinal , Sensación Térmica , Humanos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Sensación Térmica/fisiología , Adulto , Calor/efectos adversos , Médula Espinal/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Piel/inervación , Dolor/fisiopatología , Dimensión del Dolor/métodos , Percepción del Dolor/fisiología
11.
eNeuro ; 11(7)2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38997143

RESUMEN

Psychotic symptoms and delusional beliefs have been linked to dopamine transmission in both healthy and clinical samples and are assumed to result at least in part from perceiving illusory patterns in noise. However, the existing literature on the role of dopamine in detecting patterns in noise is inconclusive. To address this issue, we assessed the effect of manipulating dopaminergic neurotransmission on illusory pattern perception in healthy individuals (n = 48, n = 19 female) in a double-blind placebo-controlled within-subjects design (see preregistration at https://osf.io/a4k9j/). We predicted individuals on versus off ʟ-DOPA to be more likely to perceive illusory patterns, specifically objects in images containing only noise. Using a signal detection model, however, we found no credible evidence that ʟ-DOPA compared with placebo increased false alarm rates. Further, ʟ-DOPA did not reliably modulate measures of accuracy, discrimination sensitivity, and response bias. In all cases, Bayesian statistics revealed strong evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. The task design followed previous work on illusory pattern perception and comprised a limited number of items per condition. The results therefore need to be interpreted with caution, as power was limited. Future studies should address illusory pattern perception using more items and take into account potential dose-dependent effects and differential effects in healthy versus clinical samples.


Asunto(s)
Dopamina , Ilusiones , Levodopa , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Método Doble Ciego , Adulto , Ilusiones/fisiología , Ilusiones/efectos de los fármacos , Dopamina/metabolismo , Adulto Joven , Levodopa/farmacología , Levodopa/administración & dosificación , Transmisión Sináptica/efectos de los fármacos , Transmisión Sináptica/fisiología , Dopaminérgicos/farmacología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/efectos de los fármacos , Teorema de Bayes
12.
J Vis ; 24(7): 14, 2024 Jul 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39046721

RESUMEN

In humans, the eye pupils respond to both physical light sensed by the retina and mental representations of light produced by the brain. Notably, our pupils constrict when a visual stimulus is illusorily perceived brighter, even if retinal illumination is constant. However, it remains unclear whether such perceptual penetrability of pupil responses is an epiphenomenon unique to humans or whether it represents an adaptive mechanism shared with other animals to anticipate variations in retinal illumination between successive eye fixations. To address this issue, we measured the pupil responses of both humans and macaque monkeys exposed to three chromatic versions (cyan, magenta, and yellow) of the Asahi brightness illusion. We found that the stimuli illusorily perceived brighter or darker trigger differential pupil responses that are very similar in macaques and human participants. Additionally, we show that this phenomenon exhibits an analogous cyan bias in both primate species. Beyond evincing the macaque monkey as a relevant model to study the perceptual penetrability of pupil responses, our results suggest that this phenomenon is tuned to ecological conditions because the exposure to a "bright cyan-bluish sky" may be associated with increased risks of dazzle and retinal damages.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Luminosa , Pupila , Animales , Humanos , Pupila/fisiología , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Macaca mulatta , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Ilusiones Ópticas/fisiología , Luz , Reflejo Pupilar/fisiología
13.
Cognition ; 250: 105867, 2024 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38954903

RESUMEN

The "end of history" illusion in adults (Quoidbach et al., 2013) is an asymmetrical pattern in which people accept that they've changed in the past but don't believe they will change in the future. We explore here whether the same psychological forces that cause the illusion in adults exist in the minds of children. Two studies with 4- to 11-year-olds (N = 256) suggest that they do, even in a within-subject design where the same child is asked questions about the past and the future. A third study (N = 83) finds that this illusion does not persist when children are asked about other people. These studies suggest that even young children believe that although they used to be different in the past, from this point on, they will remain forever young.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Humanos , Niño , Ilusiones/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Preescolar , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología
14.
Cognition ; 251: 105880, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39018638

RESUMEN

The informal heuristic practices of the fine arts have much to offer to our understanding of the appearance of phenomenological reality. One interesting example is the use of exaggeration to enhance the illusion of liveliness in both living and nonliving subjects. This further eases the uncomfortable sense that the motion is somehow uncanny - especially with inanimate objects. We performed a series of experiments to test the effects of exaggeration on the phenomenological perception of simple animated objects - bouncing balls. A physically plausible model of a bouncing ball was augmented with a frequently used form of exaggeration known as squash and stretch. Observers were shown a series of animated balls, depicted using systematic parameterizations of the exaggeration model, and asked to rate their plausibility. A range of rendering styles provided varying levels of information as to the type of ball. In all cases, balls with small amounts of exaggeration were seen as plausible as those without any exaggeration (e.g., with veridical motion). Furthermore, when the type of ball was not specified, observers tolerated a large amount of exaggeration before judging them as implausible. When the type of ball was indicated, observers narrowed the range of acceptable exaggeration somewhat but still tolerated exaggeration well beyond that which would be physically possible. We contend that, in this case, exaggeration acts to bridge the so-called uncanny valley for artificial depictions of physical reality.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Ilusiones/fisiología
15.
Cogn Behav Neurol ; 37(3): 99-106, 2024 Sep 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39056214

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The rubber hand illusion (RHI) is a well-established method for studying body ownership: Given adequate concordance of visual, sensory, and proprioceptive stimuli, the individual experiences a rubber hand as his or her own. OBJECTIVE: To study the effects of a threat to the rubber hand. METHODS: We created a typical RHI paradigm but added threatening pain: Both the real hand of an individual and the rubber hand were stroked with a brush, either synchronously (RHI-inducing condition) or asynchronously (control condition), but only the rubber hand was then pricked with a needle to create a threatening pain condition. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded in a group of 23 typically-developed individuals. ERP effects were source-localized using low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. RESULTS: The individuals consistently reported experience of the RHI during the experiment when the brush strokes were applied synchronously to both the real hand and the rubber hand. ERP analysis revealed that synchronous brush stroking gave rise to higher amplitude of frontal ERPs in the 100-200 ms range than asynchronous brush stroking, which was interpreted as reflecting the RHI. In the threatening pain condition, ERPs showed a greater positivity at frontocentral electrodes, source localized in the supplementary motor area (SMA). CONCLUSION: SMA activation could reflect a control mechanism over reflexive motor activity, facilitating a possible threat-related response. Further studies should address ERP effects and the extent of the RHI to standard and threat stimuli in a correlative fashion to further elucidate the functional significance of the neurophysiological findings.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Potenciales Evocados , Mano , Ilusiones , Humanos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Mano/fisiología , Adulto , Potenciales Evocados/fisiología , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Adulto Joven , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Dolor/fisiopatología , Percepción Visual/fisiología
16.
Emotion ; 24(7): 1781-1787, 2024 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38842878

RESUMEN

The happy face advantage, the faster recognition of happy than of negative, angry or fearful, emotional expressions, has been reliably found and is modulated by social category cues, such as perceived gender, that is, is larger on female than on male faces. In this study, we tested whether this pattern of results is unique to human faces by investigating whether ambient examples of face pareidolia can also evoke a happy face advantage that is dependent on perceived gender. "Face pareidolia" describes the illusion of facial structure on inanimate objects, such as a tree trunk or a piece of burnt toast. While it has been shown that these illusory faces have expressions that can be recognized by participants, it is unknown whether they drive the same behavioral biases as real facial expressions. Thus, we measured the speed and accuracy with which the expressions of illusory faces that are perceived as female or male are recognized as happy or angry. We found a robust happy face advantage for illusory faces that were rated as more feminine in appearance. Concomitantly, we also found a robust angry face advantage for illusory faces that were rated as more masculine in appearance. Taken together, these findings demonstrate that illusory faces confer the same behavioral advantages as human faces. They also suggest that both perceived emotion and perceived gender are powerful socioevaluative dimensions that are extracted from visual stimuli that merely resemble human faces. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Expresión Facial , Reconocimiento Facial , Felicidad , Ilusiones , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Adulto Joven , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Adulto , Ilusiones/fisiología , Ira/fisiología , Percepción Social , Factores Sexuales , Adolescente
17.
Aerosp Med Hum Perform ; 95(7): 390-398, 2024 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38915170

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Spatial disorientation (SD) remains the leading contributor to Class A mishaps in the U.S. Navy, consistent with historical trends. Despite this, SD training for military aircrew is largely confined to the classroom and experiential training replicating SD illusions is limited and infrequent. Static flight simulators are most commonly used for training but offer no vestibular stimulation to the flight crew, omitting the source of vestibular-mediated SD.BACKGROUND: We first cover vestibular-mediated SD illusions which may be replicated through galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) in a static environment. GVS is a safe, reliable, low-cost avenue for providing vestibular sensory stimulation. We review the underlying mechanisms of GVS such as the excitement and inhibition of the afferent neurons innervating the vestibular system, particularly in the binaural bipolar electrode montage.APPLICATIONS: Two approaches for how GVS may be used to enhance SD training are examined. The first is a means for providing unreliable vestibular sensory perceptions to pilots, and the second details how GVS can be leveraged for replicating vestibular-mediated SD illusions.DISCUSSION: We recommend GVS be pursued as an enhancement to existing SD training. The ability to disorient aircrew in the safe training environment of a static flight simulator would allow for aircrew familiarization to SD, serving as an opportunity to practice life-saving checklist items to recover from SD. A repeatable training profile that could be worn by military aircrew in a static flight simulator may afford a low-cost training solution to the number one cause of fatalities in military aviation.Allred AR, Lippert AF, Wood SJ. Galvanic vestibular stimulation advancements for spatial disorientation training. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. 2024; 95(7):390-398.


Asunto(s)
Medicina Aeroespacial , Confusión , Personal Militar , Vestíbulo del Laberinto , Humanos , Vestíbulo del Laberinto/fisiología , Pilotos , Estimulación Eléctrica/métodos , Entrenamiento Simulado/métodos , Ilusiones/fisiología
18.
eNeuro ; 11(6)2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38844346

RESUMEN

In measurement, a reference frame is needed to compare the measured object to something already known. This raises the neuroscientific question of which reference frame is used by humans when exploring the environment. Previous studies suggested that, in touch, the body employed as measuring tool also serves as reference frame. Indeed, an artificial modification of the perceived dimensions of the body changes the tactile perception of external object dimensions. However, it is unknown if such a change in tactile perception would occur when the body schema is modified through the illusion of owning a limb altered in size. Therefore, employing a virtual hand illusion paradigm with an elongated forearm of different lengths, we systematically tested the subjective perception of distance between two points [tactile distance perception (TDP) task] on the corresponding real forearm following the illusion. Thus, the TDP task is used as a proxy to gauge changes in the body schema. Embodiment of the virtual arm was found significantly greater after the synchronous visuotactile stimulation condition compared with the asynchronous one, and the forearm elongation significantly increased the TDP. However, we did not find any link between the visuotactile-induced ownership over the elongated arm and TDP variation, suggesting that vision plays the main role in the modification of the body schema. Additionally, significant effect of elongation found on TDP but not on proprioception suggests that these are affected differently by body schema modifications. These findings confirm the body schema malleability and its role as a reference frame in touch.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia , Ilusiones , Percepción del Tacto , Realidad Virtual , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Ilusiones/fisiología , Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Imagen Corporal , Antebrazo/fisiología
19.
Behav Brain Sci ; 47: e122, 2024 Jun 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38934449

RESUMEN

Why have core knowledge? Standard answers typically emphasize the difficulty of learning core knowledge from experience, or the benefits it confers for learning about the world. Here, we suggest a complementary reason: Core knowledge is critical for learning not just about the external world, but about the mind itself.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Aprendizaje , Humanos , Ilusiones/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Conocimiento , Percepción Visual/fisiología
20.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 14813, 2024 06 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38926514

RESUMEN

To understand how the human brain distinguishes itself from external stimulation, it was examined if motor predictions enable healthy adult volunteers to infer self-location and to distinguish their body from the environment (and other agents). By uniquely combining a VR-setup with full-body motion capture, a full-body illusion paradigm (FBI) was developed with different levels of motion control: (A) a standard, passive FBI in which they had no motion control; (B) an active FBI in which they made simple, voluntary movements; and (C) an immersive game in which they real-time controlled a human-sized avatar in third person. Systematic comparisons between measures revealed a causal relationship between (i) motion control (prospective agency), (ii) self-other identification, and (iii) the ability to locate oneself. Healthy adults could recognise their movements in a third-person avatar and psychologically align with it (action observation); but did not lose a sense of place (self-location), time (temporal binding), nor who they are (self/other). Instead, motor predictions enabled them to localise their body and to distinguish self from other. In the future, embodied games could target and strengthen the brain's control networks in psychosis and neurodegeneration; real-time motion simulations could help advance neurorehabilitation techniques by fine-tuning and personalising therapeutic settings.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Humanos , Adulto , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Ilusiones/fisiología , Imagen Corporal/psicología , Movimiento/fisiología , Realidad Virtual , Autoimagen
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...