Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 1.387
Filtrar
1.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 27(9): 788-790, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37507243

RESUMEN

Does expertise mostly stem from pattern recognition or look-ahead search? van Opheusden et al. contribute to this important debate in cognitive psychology and artificial intelligence (AI) with a multi-method, multi-experiment study and a new model. Using a novel, relatively simple board game, they show that players increase depth of search when improving their skill.


Asunto(s)
Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Humanos , Inteligencia Artificial
2.
Mol Cell Neurosci ; 120: 103719, 2022 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35283305

RESUMEN

Pattern separation is a hippocampal process in which highly similar stimuli are recognized as separate representations, and deficits could lead to memory impairments in neuropsychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia. The 5-HT1A receptor (5-HT1AR) is believed to be involved in these hippocampal pattern separation processes. However, in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN), the 5-HT1AR is expressed as a somatodendritic autoreceptor, negatively regulates serotonergic signaling, and could thereby counteract the effects of hippocampal postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. Therefore, this study aims to identify how pre- and post-synaptic 5-HT1AR activity affects pattern separation. Object pattern separation (OPS) performance was measured in male Wistar rats after both acute and chronic treatment (i.p.) with 5-HT1AR biased agonists F13714 (0.0025 mg/kg acutely, 0.02 mg/kg/day chronically) or NLX-101 (0.08 mg/kg acutely, 0.32 mg/kg/day chronically), which preferentially activate autoreceptors or postsynaptic receptors respectively, for 14 days. Body temperature - a functional correlate of hypothalamic 5-HT1AR stimulation - was measured daily. Additionally, 5-HT1AR density (DRN) and plasticity markers (hippocampus) were assessed. Acute treatment with F13714 impaired OPS performance, whereas chronic treatment normalized this, and a drop in body temperature was found from day 4 onwards. NLX-101 enhanced OPS performance acutely and chronically, and caused an acute drop in body temperature. Chronic NLX-101 treatment increased doublecortin positive neurons in the dorsal hippocampus, while chronic treatment with F13714 resulted in a downregulation of 5-HT1A autoreceptors, which likely reversed the acute impairment in OPS performance. Chronic treatment with NLX-101 appears to have therapeutic potential to improve brain plasticity and OPS performance.


Asunto(s)
Aminopiridinas , Autorreceptores , Hipocampo , Plasticidad Neuronal , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Piperidinas , Pirimidinas , Receptor de Serotonina 5-HT1A , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Agonistas del Receptor de Serotonina 5-HT1 , Aminopiridinas/farmacología , Animales , Autorreceptores/fisiología , Hipocampo/efectos de los fármacos , Hipocampo/fisiología , Masculino , Plasticidad Neuronal/efectos de los fármacos , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/efectos de los fármacos , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Piperidinas/farmacología , Pirimidinas/farmacología , Ratas , Ratas Wistar , Receptor de Serotonina 5-HT1A/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/efectos de los fármacos , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Agonistas del Receptor de Serotonina 5-HT1/farmacología , Agonistas del Receptor de Serotonina 5-HT1/uso terapéutico
3.
PLoS One ; 17(2): e0264564, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35213662

RESUMEN

In this paper, we wanted to verify the hypothesis that extruding cartographic symbols on tactile maps to different heights might allow reducing the minimum (suggested in the literature) horizontal distances between them, without impacting the overall map's legibility. This approach might allow preparing tactile maps in smaller scales and thus, reducing production cost, or putting additional spatial information on the same map sheet that would not fit otherwise. To verify the hypothesis we have prepared 6 different stimuli variants with or without height differentiation applied and different horizontal distances between tactile symbols adopted (1 mm, 2 mm and 3 mm). In the controlled study sessions with 30 participants with visual impairments we have measured the times required for solving 3 different spatial tasks on 3D printed tactile stimuli. We have also performed qualitative analysis to learn participants' opinions about the proposed design and materials used. It turns out that applying height differentiation not only results in shorter times required for solving spatial tasks but is also considered by blind individuals as a convenient improvement in terms of use comfort and allows reduction of recommended minimum horizontal distances between symbols on tactile maps.


Asunto(s)
Dispositivos de Autoayuda , Tacto/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Mapas como Asunto , Persona de Mediana Edad , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Impresión Tridimensional , Personas con Daño Visual/psicología , Adulto Joven
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(5)2022 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35101921

RESUMEN

Observers with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) find it difficult to read intentions from movements. However, the computational bases of these difficulties are unknown. Do these difficulties reflect an intention readout deficit, or are they more likely rooted in kinematic (dis-)similarities between typical and ASD kinematics? We combined motion tracking, psychophysics, and computational analyses to uncover single-trial intention readout computations in typically developing (TD) children (n = 35) and children with ASD (n = 35) who observed actions performed by TD children and children with ASD. Average intention discrimination performance was above chance for TD observers but not for ASD observers. However, single-trial analysis showed that both TD and ASD observers read single-trial variations in movement kinematics. TD readers were better able to identify intention-informative kinematic features during observation of TD actions; conversely, ASD readers were better able to identify intention-informative features during observation of ASD actions. Crucially, while TD observers were generally able to extract the intention information encoded in movement kinematics, those with autism were unable to do so. These results extend existing conceptions of mind reading in ASD by suggesting that intention reading difficulties reflect both an interaction failure, rooted in kinematic dissimilarity between TD and ASD kinematics (at the level of feature identification), and an individual readout deficit (at the level of information extraction), accompanied by an overall reduced sensitivity of intention readout to single-trial variations in movement kinematics.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/fisiopatología , Fenómenos Biomecánicos/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Adolescente , Trastorno Autístico , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Cognición , Comprensión/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Humanos , Intención , Movimiento/fisiología
5.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 1829, 2022 02 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35115559

RESUMEN

Brain systems dealing with multiple meanings of ambiguous stimuli are relatively well studied, while the processing of non-selected meanings is less investigated in the neurophysiological literature and provokes controversy between existing theories. It is debated whether these meanings are actively suppressed and, if yes, whether suppression characterizes any task that involves alternative solutions or only those tasks that emphasize semantic processing or the existence of alternatives. The current functional MRI event-related study used a modified version of the word fragment completion task to reveal brain mechanisms involved in implicit processing of the non-selected solutions of ambiguous fragments. The stimuli were pairs of fragmented adjectives and nouns. Noun fragments could have one or two solutions (resulting in two words with unrelated meanings). Adjective fragments had one solution and created contexts strongly suggesting one solution for ambiguous noun fragments. All fragmented nouns were presented twice during the experiment (with two different adjectives). We revealed that ambiguity resolution was associated with a reduced BOLD signal within several regions related to language processing, including the anterior hippocampi and amygdala and posterior lateral temporal cortex. Obtained findings were interpreted as resulting from brain activity inhibition, which underlies a hypothesized mechanism of suppression of non-selected solutions.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Hipocampo/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Semántica , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/anatomía & histología , Mapeo Encefálico , Comprensión/fisiología , Femenino , Hipocampo/anatomía & histología , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Vocabulario
6.
Nat Commun ; 13(1): 79, 2022 01 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35013205

RESUMEN

Object recognition is among the basic survival skills of human beings and other animals. To date, artificial intelligence (AI) assisted high-performance object recognition is primarily visual-based, empowered by the rapid development of sensing and computational capabilities. Here, we report a tactile-olfactory sensing array, which was inspired by the natural sense-fusion system of star-nose mole, and can permit real-time acquisition of the local topography, stiffness, and odor of a variety of objects without visual input. The tactile-olfactory information is processed by a bioinspired olfactory-tactile associated machine-learning algorithm, essentially mimicking the biological fusion procedures in the neural system of the star-nose mole. Aiming to achieve human identification during rescue missions in challenging environments such as dark or buried scenarios, our tactile-olfactory intelligent sensing system could classify 11 typical objects with an accuracy of 96.9% in a simulated rescue scenario at a fire department test site. The tactile-olfactory bionic sensing system required no visual input and showed superior tolerance to environmental interference, highlighting its great potential for robust object recognition in difficult environments where other methods fall short.


Asunto(s)
Nariz Electrónica , Aprendizaje Automático , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas/métodos , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Animales , Incendios , Humanos , Topos/anatomía & histología , Topos/fisiología , Odorantes/análisis , Entrenamiento Simulado
7.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 69, 2022 01 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34996965

RESUMEN

Working memory and pattern separation are fundamental cognitive abilities which, when impaired, significantly diminish quality of life. Discovering genetic mechanisms underlying innate and disease-induced variation in these cognitive abilities is a critical step towards treatments for common and devastating neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer's disease. In this regard, the trial-unique nonmatching-to-location assay (TUNL) is a touchscreen operant conditioning procedure allowing simultaneous quantification of working memory and pattern separation in mice and rats. In the present study, we used the TUNL assay to quantify these cognitive abilities in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice. These strains are the founders of the BXD recombinant inbred mouse panel which enables discovery of genetic mechanisms underlying phenotypic variation. TUNL testing revealed that pattern separation was significantly influenced by mouse strain, whereas working memory was not. Moreover, horizontal distance and vertical distance between choice-phase stimuli had dissociable effects on TUNL performance. These findings provide novel data on mouse strain differences in pattern separation and support previous findings of equivalent working memory performance in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice. Although working memory of the BXD founder strains was equivalent in this study, working memory of BXD strains may be divergent because of transgressive segregation. Collectively, data presented here indicate that pattern separation is heritable in the mouse and that the BXD panel can be used to identify mechanisms underlying variation in pattern separation.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Cognición , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Animales , Condicionamiento Operante , Genotipo , Masculino , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Ratones Endogámicos DBA , Fenotipo , Especificidad de la Especie
8.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0260700, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34905544

RESUMEN

Working memory is a cognitive system devoted to storage and retrieval processing of information. Numerous studies on the development of working memory have investigated the processing of visuo-spatial and verbal non-spatialized information; however, little is known regarding the refinement of acoustic spatial and memory abilities across development. Here, we hypothesize that audio-spatial memory skills improve over development, due to strengthening spatial and cognitive skills such as semantic elaboration. We asked children aged 6 to 11 years old (n = 55) to pair spatialized animal calls with the corresponding animal spoken name. Spatialized sounds were emitted from an audio-haptic device, haptically explored by children with the dominant hand's index finger. Children younger than 8 anchored their exploration strategy on previously discovered sounds instead of holding this information in working memory and performed worse than older peers when asked to pair the spoken word with the corresponding animal call. In line with our hypothesis, these findings demonstrate that age-related improvements in spatial exploration and verbal coding memorization strategies affect how children learn and memorize items belonging to a complex acoustic spatial layout. Similar to vision, audio-spatial memory abilities strongly depend on cognitive development in early years of life.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Memoria Espacial/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Animales , Niño , Perros , Femenino , Interfaces Hápticas , Humanos , Masculino , Semántica , Vocalización Animal/fisiología
9.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0260587, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34905551

RESUMEN

The degree of attention individuals pay to olfactory cues (called odor awareness) influences the role of odors in everyday life. Particularly, odors produced by the human body (i.e., social odors) are able to carry a wide variety of information and to elicit a broad spectrum of emotional reactions, making them essential in interpersonal relationships. Hence, despite the assessment of awareness toward social odors is crucial, a proper tool is still lacking. Here, we designed and initially validated the Social Odor Scale (SOS), a 12-item scale designed to measure the individual differences in awareness towards different social odors. In Study 1, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA; KMO test: MSA = 0.78; Bartlett's test: χ2(78) = 631.34, p < 0.001; Chi-squared test: χ2(42) = 71.84, p = 0.003) suggests that the three factors structure was the model that best fit with the Italian version of the scale. The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) supports a second-order model with one higher-order factor representing social odor awareness in general and three lower-order factors representing familiar, romantic partner, and stranger social odors. The final version of the scale presented a good fit (RMSEA = 0.012, SRMR = 0.069, CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.997). In Study 2, CFA was performed in the German version of the scale confirming the validity of scale structure. Study 3 and 4 revealed that SOS total score and its subscales were positively correlated with other validated olfactory scales, but not with olfactory abilities. Moreover, SOS was found to be related to the gender of the participants: women reported to be more aware to social odors and, specifically, to familiar social odors than men. Overall, the results indicated that SOS is a valid and reliable instrument to assess awareness toward social odors in everyday life.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Interpersonales , Odorantes/análisis , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Olfato/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Proyectos de Investigación
10.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0218006, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34919558

RESUMEN

Music is especially valued in human societies, but music-like behavior in the form of song also occurs in a variety of other animal groups including primates. The calling of our primate ancestors may well have evolved into the music of modern humans via multiple selective scenarios. But efforts to uncover these influences have been hindered by the challenge of precisely defining musical behavior in a way that could be more generally applied across species. We propose an acoustic focused reconsideration of "musicality" that could help enable independent inquiry into potential ecological pressures on the evolutionary emergence of such behavior. Using published spectrographic images (n = 832 vocalizations) from the primate vocalization literature, we developed a quantitative formulation that could be used to help recognize signatures of human-like musicality in the acoustic displays of other species. We visually scored each spectrogram along six structural features from human music-tone, interval, transposition, repetition, rhythm, and syllabic variation-and reduced this multivariate assessment into a concise measure of musical patterning, as informed by principal components analysis. The resulting acoustic reappearance diversity index (ARDI) estimates the number of different reappearing syllables within a call type. ARDI is in concordance with traditional measures of bird song complexity yet more readily identifies shorter, more subtly melodic primate vocalizations. We demonstrate the potential utility of this index by using it to corroborate several origins scenarios. When comparing ARDI scores with ecological features, our data suggest that vocalizations with diversely reappearing elements have a pronounced association with both social and environmental factors. Musical calls were moderately associated with wooded habitats and arboreal foraging, providing partial support for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. But musical calling was most strongly associated with social monogamy, suggestive of selection for constituents of small family-sized groups by neighboring conspecifics. In sum, ARDI helps construe musical behavior along a continuum, accommodates non-human musicality, and enables gradualistic co-evolutionary paths between primate taxa-ranging from the more inhibited locational calls of archaic primates to the more exhibitional displays of modern apes.


Asunto(s)
Retroalimentación Sensorial/fisiología , Música/psicología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Primates/fisiología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Acústica , Animales , Evolución Biológica , Aves/fisiología , Humanos , Análisis de Componente Principal
11.
PLoS Biol ; 19(9): e3001119, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34491980

RESUMEN

Statistical learning (SL) is the ability to extract regularities from the environment. In the domain of language, this ability is fundamental in the learning of words and structural rules. In lack of reliable online measures, statistical word and rule learning have been primarily investigated using offline (post-familiarization) tests, which gives limited insights into the dynamics of SL and its neural basis. Here, we capitalize on a novel task that tracks the online SL of simple syntactic structures combined with computational modeling to show that online SL responds to reinforcement learning principles rooted in striatal function. Specifically, we demonstrate-on 2 different cohorts-that a temporal difference model, which relies on prediction errors, accounts for participants' online learning behavior. We then show that the trial-by-trial development of predictions through learning strongly correlates with activity in both ventral and dorsal striatum. Our results thus provide a detailed mechanistic account of language-related SL and an explanation for the oft-cited implication of the striatum in SL tasks. This work, therefore, bridges the long-standing gap between language learning and reinforcement learning phenomena.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpo Estriado/fisiología , Desarrollo del Lenguaje , Aprendizaje por Probabilidad , Refuerzo en Psicología , Cuerpo Estriado/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Adulto Joven
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 118(29)2021 07 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34272278

RESUMEN

Rhythm perception is fundamental to speech and music. Humans readily recognize a rhythmic pattern, such as that of a familiar song, independently of the tempo at which it occurs. This shows that our perception of auditory rhythms is flexible, relying on global relational patterns more than on the absolute durations of specific time intervals. Given that auditory rhythm perception in humans engages a complex auditory-motor cortical network even in the absence of movement and that the evolution of vocal learning is accompanied by strengthening of forebrain auditory-motor pathways, we hypothesize that vocal learning species share our perceptual facility for relational rhythm processing. We test this by asking whether the best-studied animal model for vocal learning, the zebra finch, can recognize a fundamental rhythmic pattern-equal timing between event onsets (isochrony)-based on temporal relations between intervals rather than on absolute durations. Prior work suggests that vocal nonlearners (pigeons and rats) are quite limited in this regard and are biased to attend to absolute durations when listening to rhythmic sequences. In contrast, using naturalistic sounds at multiple stimulus rates, we show that male zebra finches robustly recognize isochrony independent of absolute time intervals, even at rates distant from those used in training. Our findings highlight the importance of comparative studies of rhythmic processing and suggest that vocal learning species are promising animal models for key aspects of human rhythm perception. Such models are needed to understand the neural mechanisms behind the positive effect of rhythm on certain speech and movement disorders.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Pinzones/fisiología , Animales , Corteza Auditiva/fisiología , Femenino , Aprendizaje , Masculino , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Sonido , Voz
13.
Brain Res ; 1768: 147578, 2021 10 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34284021

RESUMEN

While the notion of the brain as a prediction machine has been extremely influential and productive in cognitive science, there are competing accounts of how best to model and understand the predictive capabilities of brains. One prominent framework is of a "Bayesian brain" that explicitly generates predictions and uses resultant errors to guide adaptation. We suggest that the prediction-generation component of this framework may involve little more than a pattern completion process. We first describe pattern completion in the domain of visual perception, highlighting its temporal extension, and show how this can entail a form of prediction in time. Next, we describe the forward momentum of entrained dynamical systems as a model for the emergence of predictive processing in non-predictive systems. Then, we apply this reasoning to the domain of language, where explicitly predictive models are perhaps most popular. Here, we demonstrate how a connectionist model, TRACE, exhibits hallmarks of predictive processing without any representations of predictions or errors. Finally, we present a novel neural network model, inspired by reservoir computing models, that is entirely unsupervised and memoryless, but nonetheless exhibits prediction-like behavior in its pursuit of homeostasis. These explorations demonstrate that brain-like systems can get prediction "for free," without the need to posit formal logical representations with Bayesian probabilities or an inference machine that holds them in working memory.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Teorema de Bayes , Encéfalo/fisiología , Humanos , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Probabilidad , Percepción Visual/fisiología
14.
Neuroimage ; 238: 118238, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34098064

RESUMEN

Repeating structures forming regular patterns are common in sounds. Learning such patterns may enable accurate perceptual organization. In five experiments, we investigated the behavioral and neural signatures of rapid perceptual learning of regular sound patterns. We show that recurring (compared to novel) patterns are detected more quickly and increase sensitivity to pattern deviations and to the temporal order of pattern onset relative to a visual stimulus. Sustained neural activity reflected perceptual learning in two ways. Firstly, sustained activity increased earlier for recurring than novel patterns when participants attended to sounds, but not when they ignored them; this earlier increase mirrored the rapid perceptual learning we observed behaviorally. Secondly, the magnitude of sustained activity was generally lower for recurring than novel patterns, but only for trials later in the experiment, and independent of whether participants attended to or ignored sounds. The late manifestation of sustained activity reduction suggests that it is not directly related to rapid perceptual learning, but to a mechanism that does not require attention to sound. In sum, we demonstrate that the latency of sustained activity reflects rapid perceptual learning of auditory patterns, while the magnitude may reflect a result of learning, such as better prediction of learned auditory patterns.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Auditiva/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Adulto Joven
15.
PLoS One ; 16(4): e0250214, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33861789

RESUMEN

Research has repeatedly shown that familiar and unfamiliar voices elicit different neural responses. But it has also been suggested that different neural correlates associate with the feeling of having heard a voice and knowing who the voice represents. The terminology used to designate these varying responses remains vague, creating a degree of confusion in the literature. Additionally, terms serving to designate tasks of voice discrimination, voice recognition, and speaker identification are often inconsistent creating further ambiguities. The present study used event-related potentials (ERPs) to clarify the difference between responses to 1) unknown voices, 2) trained-to-familiar voices as speech stimuli are repeatedly presented, and 3) intimately familiar voices. In an experiment, 13 participants listened to repeated utterances recorded from 12 speakers. Only one of the 12 voices was intimately familiar to a participant, whereas the remaining 11 voices were unfamiliar. The frequency of presentation of these 11 unfamiliar voices varied with only one being frequently presented (the trained-to-familiar voice). ERP analyses revealed different responses for intimately familiar and unfamiliar voices in two distinct time windows (P2 between 200-250 ms and a late positive component, LPC, between 450-850 ms post-onset) with late responses occurring only for intimately familiar voices. The LPC present sustained shifts, and short-time ERP components appear to reflect an early recognition stage. The trained voice equally elicited distinct responses, compared to rarely heard voices, but these occurred in a third time window (N250 between 300-350 ms post-onset). Overall, the timing of responses suggests that the processing of intimately familiar voices operates in two distinct steps of voice recognition, marked by a P2 on right centro-frontal sites, and speaker identification marked by an LPC component. The recognition of frequently heard voices entails an independent recognition process marked by a differential N250. Based on the present results and previous observations, it is proposed that there is a need to distinguish between processes of voice "recognition" and "identification". The present study also specifies test conditions serving to reveal this distinction in neural responses, one of which bears on the length of speech stimuli given the late responses associated with voice identification.


Asunto(s)
Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Reconocimiento de Voz/fisiología , Adulto , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Quebec , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Voz/fisiología
16.
PLoS Biol ; 19(4): e3000751, 2021 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33848299

RESUMEN

Across many species, scream calls signal the affective significance of events to other agents. Scream calls were often thought to be of generic alarming and fearful nature, to signal potential threats, with instantaneous, involuntary, and accurate recognition by perceivers. However, scream calls are more diverse in their affective signaling nature than being limited to fearfully alarming a threat, and thus the broader sociobiological relevance of various scream types is unclear. Here we used 4 different psychoacoustic, perceptual decision-making, and neuroimaging experiments in humans to demonstrate the existence of at least 6 psychoacoustically distinctive types of scream calls of both alarming and non-alarming nature, rather than there being only screams caused by fear or aggression. Second, based on perceptual and processing sensitivity measures for decision-making during scream recognition, we found that alarm screams (with some exceptions) were overall discriminated the worst, were responded to the slowest, and were associated with a lower perceptual sensitivity for their recognition compared with non-alarm screams. Third, the neural processing of alarm compared with non-alarm screams during an implicit processing task elicited only minimal neural signal and connectivity in perceivers, contrary to the frequent assumption of a threat processing bias of the primate neural system. These findings show that scream calls are more diverse in their signaling and communicative nature in humans than previously assumed, and, in contrast to a commonly observed threat processing bias in perceptual discriminations and neural processes, we found that especially non-alarm screams, and positive screams in particular, seem to have higher efficiency in speeded discriminations and the implicit neural processing of various scream types in humans.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Miedo/psicología , Reconocimiento de Voz/fisiología , Adulto , Vías Auditivas/diagnóstico por imagen , Vías Auditivas/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Caracteres Sexuales , Adulto Joven
17.
Cell Rep ; 35(3): 109003, 2021 04 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33882311

RESUMEN

It has been proposed that sound information is separately streamed into onset and offset pathways for parallel processing. However, how offset responses contribute to auditory perception remains unclear. Here, loose-patch and whole-cell recordings in awake mouse primary auditory cortex (A1) reveal that a subset of pyramidal neurons exhibit a transient "Off" response, with its onset tightly time-locked to the sound termination and its frequency tuning similar to that of the transient "On" response. Both responses are characterized by excitation briefly followed by inhibition, with the latter mediated by parvalbumin (PV) inhibitory neurons. Optogenetically manipulating sound-evoked A1 responses at different temporal phases or artificially creating phantom sounds in A1 further reveals that the A1 phasic On and Off responses are critical for perceptual discrimination of sound duration. Our results suggest that perception of sound duration is dependent on precisely encoding its onset and offset timings by phasic On and Off responses.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Auditivos/fisiología , Optogenética/métodos , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico/fisiología , Células Piramidales/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Animales , Corteza Auditiva/anatomía & histología , Corteza Auditiva/fisiología , Proteínas Bacterianas/genética , Proteínas Bacterianas/metabolismo , Electrodos Implantados , Femenino , Expresión Génica , Genes Reporteros , Proteínas Luminiscentes/genética , Proteínas Luminiscentes/metabolismo , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Ratones Transgénicos , Técnicas de Placa-Clamp , Sonido , Vigilia/fisiología , Proteína Fluorescente Roja
18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33835199

RESUMEN

To perform adaptive behaviours, animals have to establish a representation of the physical "outside" world. How these representations are created by sensory systems is a central issue in sensory physiology. This review addresses the history of experimental approaches toward ideas about sensory coding, using the relatively simple auditory system of acoustic insects. I will discuss the empirical evidence in support of Barlow's "efficient coding hypothesis", which argues that the coding properties of neurons undergo specific adaptations that allow insects to detect biologically important acoustic stimuli. This hypothesis opposes the view that the sensory systems of receivers are biased as a result of their phylogeny, which finally determine whether a sound stimulus elicits a behavioural response. Acoustic signals are often transmitted over considerable distances in complex physical environments with high noise levels, resulting in degradation of the temporal pattern of stimuli, unpredictable attenuation, reduced signal-to-noise levels, and degradation of cues used for sound localisation. Thus, a more naturalistic view of sensory coding must be taken, since the signals as broadcast by signallers are rarely equivalent to the effective stimuli encoded by the sensory system of receivers. The consequences of the environmental conditions for sensory coding are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Acústica , Vías Auditivas/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva , Conducta Animal , Ambiente , Arquitectura y Construcción de Instituciones de Salud , Insectos/fisiología , Sonido , Estimulación Acústica , Adaptación Psicológica , Animales , Quirópteros/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Conducta Predatoria , Localización de Sonidos
19.
Commun Biol ; 4(1): 287, 2021 03 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33674781

RESUMEN

Understanding what maintains the broad spectrum of variation in animal phenotypes and how this influences survival is a key question in biology. Frequency dependent selection - where predators temporarily focus on one morph at the expense of others by forming a "search image" - can help explain this phenomenon. However, past work has never tested real prey colour patterns, and rarely considered the role of different types of camouflage. Using a novel citizen science computer experiment that presented crab "prey" to humans against natural backgrounds in specific sequences, we were able to test a range of key hypotheses concerning the interactions between predator learning, camouflage and morph. As predicted, switching between morphs did hinder detection, and this effect was most pronounced when crabs had "disruptive" markings that were more effective at destroying the body outline. To our knowledge, this is the first evidence for variability in natural colour patterns hindering search image formation in predators, and as such presents a mechanism that facilitates phenotypic diversity in nature.


Asunto(s)
Braquiuros/fisiología , Percepción de Color , Ecosistema , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico , Pigmentación , Conducta Predatoria , Adaptación Fisiológica , Animales , Color , Humanos , Fenotipo , Factores de Tiempo , Juegos de Video
20.
IEEE Pulse ; 11(6): 31-33, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33315552

RESUMEN

In very old days past, I used to play piano, but not anymore. Age wore out the fingers, but they are not rusty.


Asunto(s)
Música , Percepción/fisiología , Periodicidad , Humanos , Patrones de Reconocimiento Fisiológico
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA