Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 2.329
Filtrar
1.
J Integr Neurosci ; 23(6): 108, 2024 May 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38940093

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: In this study, we used electroencephalogram (EEG) to investigate the activity pattern of the cerebral cortex related to visual pursuit and saccade strategies to predict the arrival position of a visual target. In addition, we clarified the differences in the EEG of those who could predict the arrival position well using the saccade strategy compared to those who were not proficient. METHODS: Sixteen participants performed two tasks: the "Pursuit Strategy Task (PST)" and the "Saccade Strategy Task (SST)" while undergoing EEG. For the PST, the participants were instructed to follow the target with their eyes throughout its trajectory and indicate when it reached the final point. For the SST, the participants were instructed to shift their gaze to the end point of arrival once they had predicted it. RESULTS: Low beta EEG activity at the Oz, Cz, and CP2 electrodes was significantly higher during the SST than during the PST. In addition, low beta EEG activity at P7 electrode was significantly higher in the group showing a small position error (PE) than in the group showing a large PE at response. CONCLUSIONS: EEG activity at the Oz, Cz, and CP2 electrodes during the SST may reflect visuospatial attention to the moving target, the tracking of moving targets, and the focus on the final destination position. In addition, EEG activity at P7 electrode may more accurately detect the speed and direction of the moving target by the small PE group at response.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología
2.
PLoS One ; 19(6): e0303596, 2024.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38905269

RESUMEN

Eye-tracking techniques have gained widespread application in various fields including research on the visual system, neurosciences, psychology, and human-computer interaction, with emerging clinical implications. In this preliminary phase of our study, we introduce a pilot test of innovative virtual reality technology designed for tracking head and eye movements among healthy individuals. This tool was developed to assess the presence of mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), given the frequent association of oculomotor function deficits with such injuries. Alongside eye-tracking, we also integrated fMRI due to the complementary nature of these techniques, offering insights into both neural activation patterns and behavioural responses, thereby providing a comprehensive understanding of oculomotor function. We used fMRI with tasks evaluating oculomotor functions: Smooth Pursuit (SP), Saccades, Anti-Saccades, and Optokinetic Nystagmus (OKN). Prior to the scanning, the testing with a system of VR goggles with integrated eye and head tracking was used where subjects performed the same tasks as those used in fMRI. 31 healthy adult controls (HCs) were tested with the purpose of identifying brain regions associated with these tasks and collecting preliminary norms for later comparison with concussed subjects. HCs' fMRI results showed following peak activation regions: SP-cuneus, superior parietal lobule, paracentral lobule, inferior parietal lobule (IPL), cerebellartonsil (CT); Saccades-middle frontal gyrus (MFG), postcentral gyrus, medial frontal gyrus; Anti-saccades-precuneus, IPL, MFG; OKN-middle temporal gyrus, ACC, postcentral gyrus, MFG, CT. These results demonstrated brain regions associated with the performance on oculomotor tasks in healthy controls and most of the highlighted areas are corresponding with those affected in concussion. This suggests that the involvement of brain areas susceptible to mTBI in implementing oculomotor evaluation, taken together with commonly reported oculomotor difficulties post-concussion, may lead to finding objective biomarkers using eye-tracking tasks.


Asunto(s)
Tecnología de Seguimiento Ocular , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Proyectos Piloto , Adulto , Masculino , Femenino , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Nistagmo Optoquinético/fisiología
3.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 13859, 2024 06 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38879556

RESUMEN

Smooth pursuit eye movements are considered a well-established and quantifiable biomarker of sensorimotor function in psychosis research. Identifying psychotic syndromes on an individual level based on neurobiological markers is limited by heterogeneity and requires comprehensive external validation to avoid overestimation of prediction models. Here, we studied quantifiable sensorimotor measures derived from smooth pursuit eye movements in a large sample of psychosis probands (N = 674) and healthy controls (N = 305) using multivariate pattern analysis. Balanced accuracies of 64% for the prediction of psychosis status are in line with recent results from other large heterogenous psychiatric samples. They are confirmed by external validation in independent large samples including probands with (1) psychosis (N = 727) versus healthy controls (N = 292), (2) psychotic (N = 49) and non-psychotic bipolar disorder (N = 36), and (3) non-psychotic affective disorders (N = 119) and psychosis (N = 51) yielding accuracies of 65%, 66% and 58%, respectively, albeit slightly different psychosis syndromes. Our findings make a significant contribution to the identification of biologically defined profiles of heterogeneous psychosis syndromes on an individual level underlining the impact of sensorimotor dysfunction in psychosis.


Asunto(s)
Biomarcadores , Trastornos Psicóticos , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Trastornos Psicóticos/diagnóstico , Trastornos Psicóticos/fisiopatología , Adulto , Adulto Joven , Trastorno Bipolar/diagnóstico , Trastorno Bipolar/fisiopatología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Adolescente
4.
eNeuro ; 11(6)2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38821872

RESUMEN

Animals use a combination of eye movements to track moving objects. These different eye movements need to be coordinated for successful tracking, requiring interactions between the systems involved. Here, we study the interaction between the saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movement systems in marmosets. Using a single-target pursuit task, we show that saccades cause an enhancement in pursuit following a saccade. Using a two-target pursuit task, we show that this enhancement in pursuit is selective toward the motion of the target selected by the saccade, irrespective of any biases in pursuit prior to the saccade. These experiments highlight the similarities in the functioning of saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movement systems across primates.


Asunto(s)
Callithrix , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Movimientos Sacádicos , Animales , Callithrix/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología
5.
Brain Behav ; 14(5): e3510, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38715394

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Multiple system atrophy (MSA) is a neurodegenerative disease that progresses rapidly and has a poor prognosis. This study aimed to assess the value of video oculomotor evaluation (VOE) in the differential diagnosis of MSA and Parkinson's disease (PD). METHODS: In total, 28 patients with MSA, 31 patients with PD, and 30 age- and sex-matched healthy controls (HC) were screened and included in this study. The evaluation consisted of a gaze-holding test, smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM), random saccade, and optokinetic nystagmus (OKN). RESULTS: The MSA and PD groups had more abnormalities and decreased SPEM gain than the HC group (64.29%, 35.48%, 10%, p < .001). The SPEM gain in the MSA group was significantly lower than that in the PD group at specific frequencies. Patients with MSA and PD showed prolonged latencies in all saccade directions compared with those with HC. However, the two diseases had no significant differences in the saccade parameters. The OKN gain gradually decreased from the HC to the PD and the MSA groups (p < .05). Compared with the PD group, the gain in the MSA group was further decreased in the OKN test at 30°/s (Left, p = .010; Right p = .016). Receiver operating characteristic curves showed that the combination of oculomotor parameters with age and course of disease could aid in the differential diagnosis of patients with MSA and PD, with a sensitivity of 89.29% and a specificity of 70.97%. CONCLUSIONS: The combination of oculomotor parameters and clinical data may aid in the differential diagnosis of MSA and PD. Furthermore, VOE is vital in the identification of neurodegenerative diseases.


Asunto(s)
Atrofia de Múltiples Sistemas , Enfermedad de Parkinson , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Atrofia de Múltiples Sistemas/diagnóstico , Atrofia de Múltiples Sistemas/fisiopatología , Enfermedad de Parkinson/diagnóstico , Enfermedad de Parkinson/fisiopatología , Masculino , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Femenino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Anciano , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Grabación en Video , Nistagmo Optoquinético/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 199: 108883, 2024 07 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38599567

RESUMEN

Left smooth pursuit eye movement training in response to large-field visual motion (optokinetic stimulation) has become a promising rehabilitation method in left spatial inattention or neglect. The mechanisms underlying the therapeutic effect, however, remain unknown. During optokinetic stimulation, there is an error in visual localisation ahead of the line of sight. This could indicate a change in the brain's estimate of one's own direction of gaze. We hypothesized that optokinetic stimulation changes the brain's estimate of gaze. Because this estimate is critical for coding the locus of attention in the visual space relative to the body and across sensory modalities, its change might underlie the change in spatial attention. Here, we report that in healthy participants optokinetic stimulation causes not only a directional bias in the proprioceptive signal from the extraocular muscles, but also a corresponding shift of the locus of attention. Both changes outlasted the period of stimulation. This result forms a step in investigating a causal link between the adaptation in the sensorimotor gaze signals and the recovery in spatial neglect.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Fijación Ocular , Trastornos de la Percepción , Humanos , Atención/fisiología , Masculino , Trastornos de la Percepción/rehabilitación , Trastornos de la Percepción/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Percepción/etiología , Femenino , Adulto , Fijación Ocular/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adulto Joven , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología
7.
J Vis ; 24(4): 3, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38558158

RESUMEN

The sudden onset of a visual object or event elicits an inhibition of eye movements at latencies approaching the minimum delay of visuomotor conductance in the brain. Typically, information presented via multiple sensory modalities, such as sound and vision, evokes stronger and more robust responses than unisensory information. Whether and how multisensory information affects ultra-short latency oculomotor inhibition is unknown. In two experiments, we investigate smooth pursuit and saccadic inhibition in response to multisensory distractors. Observers tracked a horizontally moving dot and were interrupted by an unpredictable visual, auditory, or audiovisual distractor. Distractors elicited a transient inhibition of pursuit eye velocity and catch-up saccade rate within ∼100 ms of their onset. Audiovisual distractors evoked stronger oculomotor inhibition than visual- or auditory-only distractors, indicating multisensory response enhancement. Multisensory response enhancement magnitudes were equal to the linear sum of responses to component stimuli. These results demonstrate that multisensory information affects eye movements even at ultra-short latencies, establishing a lower time boundary for multisensory-guided behavior. We conclude that oculomotor circuits must have privileged access to sensory information from multiple modalities, presumably via a fast, subcortical pathway.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Memoria , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
8.
J Vis ; 24(3): 9, 2024 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38546586

RESUMEN

The ability to accurately perceive and track moving objects is crucial for many everyday activities. In this study, we use a "double-drift stimulus" to explore the processing of visual motion signals that underlie perception, pursuit, and saccade responses to a moving object. Participants were presented with peripheral moving apertures filled with noise that either drifted orthogonally to the aperture's direction or had no net motion. Participants were asked to saccade to and track these targets with their gaze as soon as they appeared and then to report their direction. In the trials with internal motion, the target disappeared at saccade onset so that the first 100 ms of the postsaccadic pursuit response was driven uniquely by peripheral information gathered before saccade onset. This provided independent measures of perceptual, pursuit, and saccadic responses to the double-drift stimulus on a trial-by-trial basis. Our analysis revealed systematic differences between saccadic responses, on one hand, and perceptual and pursuit responses, on the other. These differences are unlikely to be caused by differences in the processing of motion signals because both saccades and pursuits seem to rely on shared target position and velocity information. We conclude that our results are instead due to a difference in how the processing mechanisms underlying perception, pursuit, and saccades combine motor signals with target position. These findings advance our understanding of the mechanisms underlying dissociation in visual processing between perception and eye movements.


Asunto(s)
Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimientos Oculares , Mano , Percepción Visual
9.
J Vis ; 24(3): 2, 2024 Mar 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38436983

RESUMEN

Humans can accurately estimate and track object motion, even if it accelerates. Research shows that humans exhibit superior estimation and tracking performance for descending (falling) than ascending (rising) objects. Previous studies presented ascending and descending targets along the gravitational and body axes in an upright posture. Thus, it is unclear whether humans rely on congruent information between the direction of the target motion and gravity or the direction of the target motion and longitudinal body axes. Two experiments were conducted to explore these possibilities. In Experiment 1, participants estimated the arrival time at a goal for both upward and downward motion of targets along the longitudinal body axis in the upright (both axes of target motion and gravity congruent) and supine (both axes incongruent) postures. In Experiment 2, smooth pursuit eye movements were assessed while tracking both targets in the same postures. Arrival time estimation and smooth pursuit eye movement performance were consistently more accurate for downward target motion than for upward motion, irrespective of posture. These findings suggest that the visual experience of seeing an object moving along an observer's leg side in everyday life may influence the ability to accurately estimate and track the descending object's motion.


Asunto(s)
Gravitación , Postura , Humanos , Movimiento (Física) , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 131(4): 652-667, 2024 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38381528

RESUMEN

The differentiation between continuous and discrete actions is key for behavioral neuroscience. Although many studies have characterized eye-hand coordination during discrete (e.g., reaching) and continuous (e.g., pursuit tracking) actions, all these studies were conducted separately, using different setups and participants. In addition, how eye-hand coordination might operate at the frontier between discrete and continuous movements remains unexplored. Here we filled these gaps by means of a task that could elicit different movement dynamics. Twenty-eight participants were asked to simultaneously track with their eyes and a joystick a visual target that followed an unpredictable trajectory and whose position was updated at different rates (from 1.5 to 240 Hz). This procedure allowed us to examine actions ranging from discrete point-to-point movements (low refresh rate) to continuous pursuit (high refresh rate). For comparison, we also tested a manual tracking condition with the eyes fixed and a pure eye tracking condition (hand fixed). The results showed an abrupt transition between discrete and continuous hand movements around 3 Hz contrasting with a smooth trade-off between fixations and smooth pursuit. Nevertheless, hand and eye tracking accuracy remained strongly correlated, with each of these depending on whether the other effector was recruited. Moreover, gaze-cursor distance and lag were smaller when eye and hand performed the task conjointly than separately. Altogether, despite some dissimilarities in eye and hand dynamics when transitioning between discrete and continuous movements, our results emphasize that eye-hand coordination continues to smoothly operate and support the notion of synergies across eye movement types.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The differentiation between continuous and discrete actions is key for behavioral neuroscience. By using a visuomotor task in which we manipulate the target refresh rate to trigger different movement dynamics, we explored eye-hand coordination all the way from discrete to continuous actions. Despite abrupt changes in hand dynamics, eye-hand coordination continues to operate via a gradual trade-off between fixations and smooth pursuit, an observation confirming the notion of synergies across eye movement types.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Desempeño Psicomotor , Humanos , Mano , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Movimiento , Movimientos Sacádicos
11.
Behav Res Methods ; 56(1): 433-446, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36698001

RESUMEN

In daily life, sensorimotor integration processes are fundamental for many cognitive operations. The pursuit-tracking paradigm is an ecological and valid paradigm to examine sensorimotor integration processes in a more complex environment than many established tasks that assess simple motor responses. However, the analysis of pursuit-tracking performance is complicated, and parameters quantified to examine performance are sometimes ambiguous regarding their interpretation. We introduce an open-source algorithm (TRACK) to calculate a new tracking error metric, the spatial error, based on the identification of the intended target position for the respective cursor position. The identification is based on assigning cursor and target direction changes to each other as key events, based on the assumptions of similarity and proximity. By applying our algorithm to pursuit-tracking data, beyond replication of known effects such as learning or practice effects, we show a higher precision of the spatial tracking error, i.e., it fits our behavioral data better than the temporal tracking error and thus provides new insights and parameters for the investigation of pursuit-tracking behavior. Our work provides an important step towards fully utilizing the potential of pursuit-tracking tasks for research on sensorimotor integration processes.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Desempeño Psicomotor , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme
12.
Trends Neurosci ; 47(1): 71-83, 2024 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38042680

RESUMEN

Movement disorders such as Parkinson's disease (PD) impact oculomotor function - the ability to move the eyes accurately and purposefully to serve a multitude of sensory, cognitive, and secondary motor tasks. Decades of neurophysiological research in monkeys and behavioral studies in humans have characterized the neural basis of healthy oculomotor control. This review links eye movement abnormalities in persons living with PD to the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms and pathways. Building on this foundation, we highlight recent progress in using eye movements to gauge symptom severity, assess treatment effects, and serve as potential precision biomarkers. We conclude that whereas eye movements provide insights into PD mechanisms, based on current evidence they appear to lack sufficient sensitivity and specificity to serve as a standalone diagnostic tool. Their full potential may be realized when combined with other disease indicators in big datasets.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Enfermedad de Parkinson , Humanos , Enfermedad de Parkinson/diagnóstico , Enfermedad de Parkinson/terapia , Movimientos Sacádicos , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme
13.
J Neurol ; 271(1): 325-339, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37713127

RESUMEN

Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) can result into an incomplete locked in state (iLIS), in which communication depends on eye tracking computer devices. Oculomotor function impairments in ALS have been reported, but there is little research, particularly with respect to patients in iLIS. In the present study, we compared reflexive and executive oculomotor function by means of an eye tracking test battery between three groups: advanced ALS patients in iLIS (n = 22), patients in early to middle ALS stages (n = 44) and healthy subjects (n = 32). Patients with ALS showed significant deteriorations in oculomotor functions, with stronger impairments in iLIS. More specifically, ALS patients produced visually guided prosaccades with longer latencies and more frequent hypometria compared to healthy subjects. Longest latencies were obtained in iLIS patients, with a stronger prolongation for vertical than for horizontal prosaccades. ALS patients made more antisaccade errors and generated antisaccades with longer latencies. Smooth pursuit was also impaired in ALS. In the earlier ALS stages, bulbar onset patients presented stronger antisaccade and smooth pursuit deficits than spinal onset patients. Our findings reveal a relevant deterioration of important oculomotor functions in ALS, which increases in iLIS. It includes impairments of reflexive eye movements to loss of executive inhibitory control, indicating a progressing pathological involvement of prefrontal, midbrain and brainstem areas. The assessment of oculomotor functions may therefore provide clinically relevant bio- and progression marker, particularly in advanced ALS.


Asunto(s)
Esclerosis Amiotrófica Lateral , Movimientos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimientos Oculares , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme
14.
J Neurophysiol ; 131(2): 394-416, 2024 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38149327

RESUMEN

Smooth pursuit eye movements respond on the basis of both immediate and anticipated target motion, where anticipations may be derived from either memory or perceptual cues. To study the combined influence of both immediate sensory motion and anticipation, subjects pursued clear or noisy random dot kinematograms (RDKs) whose mean directions were chosen from Gaussian distributions with SDs = 10° (narrow prior) or 45° (wide prior). Pursuit directions were consistent with Bayesian theory in that transitions over time from dependence on the prior to near total dependence on immediate sensory motion (likelihood) took longer with the noisier RDKs and with the narrower, more reliable, prior. Results were fit to Bayesian models in which parameters representing the variability of the likelihood either were or were not constrained to be the same for both priors. The unconstrained model provided a statistically better fit, with the influence of the prior in the constrained model smaller than predicted from strict reliability-based weighting of prior and likelihood. Factors that may have contributed to this outcome include prior variability different from nominal values, low-level sensorimotor learning with the narrow prior, or departures of pursuit from strict adherence to reliability-based weighting. Although modifications of, or alternatives to, the normative Bayesian model will be required, these results, along with previous studies, suggest that Bayesian approaches are a promising framework to understand how pursuit combines immediate sensory motion, past history, and informative perceptual cues to accurately track the target motion that is most likely to occur in the immediate future.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Smooth pursuit eye movements respond on the basis of anticipated, as well as immediate, target motions. Bayesian models using reliability-based weighting of previous (prior) and immediate target motions (likelihood) accounted for many, but not all, aspects of pursuit of clear and noisy random dot kinematograms with different levels of predictability. Bayesian approaches may solve the long-standing problem of how pursuit combines immediate sensory motion and anticipation of future motion to configure an effective response.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Humanos , Teorema de Bayes , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Estimulación Luminosa
15.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 21380, 2023 12 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38049419

RESUMEN

The neural networks subserving smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) provide an ideal model for investigating the interaction of sensory processing and motor control during ongoing movements. To better understand core plasticity aspects of sensorimotor processing for SPEM, normative sham, anodal or cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) was applied over visual area V5 and frontal eye fields (FEF) in sixty healthy participants. The identical within-subject paradigm was used to assess SPEM modulations by practice. While no specific tDCS effects were revealed, within- and between-session practice effects indicate plasticity of top-down extraretinal mechanisms that mainly affect SPEM in the absence of visual input and during SPEM initiation. To explore the potential of tDCS effects, individual electric field simulations were computed based on calibrated finite element head models and individual functional localization of V5 and FEF location (using functional MRI) and orientation (using combined EEG/MEG) was conducted. Simulations revealed only limited electric field target intensities induced by the applied normative tDCS montages but indicate the potential efficacy of personalized tDCS for the modulation of SPEM. In sum, results indicate the potential susceptibility of extraretinal SPEM control to targeted external neuromodulation (e.g., personalized tDCS) and intrinsic learning protocols.


Asunto(s)
Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa , Humanos , Estimulación Transcraneal de Corriente Directa/métodos , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Lóbulo Frontal , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos
16.
J Neurophysiol ; 130(6): 1425-1443, 2023 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37910548

RESUMEN

Following previous studies documenting the ability to generate anticipatory responses, we tested whether the repeated motion of a visual target along the same path affected its oculomotor tracking. In six rhesus monkeys, we evaluated how the frequency of a target path influenced the onset, accuracy, and velocity of eye movements. Three hundred milliseconds after its extinction, a central target reappeared and immediately moved toward the periphery in four possible (oblique) directions and at a constant speed (20°/s or 40°/s). During each daily session, the frequency of one motion direction was either uncertain (25% of trials) or certain (100% of trials). Our results show no reduction of saccade latency between the two sessions. No express saccades were observed in either session. A slow eye movement started after target onset (presaccadic glissade) and its velocity was larger during the "certain" sessions only with the 40°/s target. No anticipatory eye movement was observed. Longer intersaccadic intervals were found during the "certain" sessions but the postsaccadic pursuit velocity exhibited no change. No correlation was found between the accuracy and precision of saccades (interceptive or catch-up) and the postsaccadic pursuit velocity. Repeatedly tracking a target that moves always along the same path does not favor the generation of anticipatory eye movements, saccadic or slow. Their occurrence is not spontaneous but seems to require preliminary training. Finally, for both sessions, the lack of correlation between the saccade-related and pursuit-related kinematic parameters is consistent with separate control of saccadic and slow eye movements.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Following previous studies documenting anticipatory movements, we investigated how the frequency of occurrence of a target path influenced the generation of tracking eye movements. When present, the effects were small. The limited performance that we found suggests that anticipatory responses require preliminary training, in which case, they should not be considered as a behavioral marker of the primates' ability to extrapolate but the outcome of learning and remembering past experience.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Movimientos Oculares , Movimientos Sacádicos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología
17.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 19(10): e1011512, 2023 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37883331

RESUMEN

The complexity of natural scenes makes it challenging to experimentally study the mechanisms behind human gaze behavior when viewing dynamic environments. Historically, eye movements were believed to be driven primarily by space-based attention towards locations with salient features. Increasing evidence suggests, however, that visual attention does not select locations with high saliency but operates on attentional units given by the objects in the scene. We present a new computational framework to investigate the importance of objects for attentional guidance. This framework is designed to simulate realistic scanpaths for dynamic real-world scenes, including saccade timing and smooth pursuit behavior. Individual model components are based on psychophysically uncovered mechanisms of visual attention and saccadic decision-making. All mechanisms are implemented in a modular fashion with a small number of well-interpretable parameters. To systematically analyze the importance of objects in guiding gaze behavior, we implemented five different models within this framework: two purely spatial models, where one is based on low-level saliency and one on high-level saliency, two object-based models, with one incorporating low-level saliency for each object and the other one not using any saliency information, and a mixed model with object-based attention and selection but space-based inhibition of return. We optimized each model's parameters to reproduce the saccade amplitude and fixation duration distributions of human scanpaths using evolutionary algorithms. We compared model performance with respect to spatial and temporal fixation behavior, including the proportion of fixations exploring the background, as well as detecting, inspecting, and returning to objects. A model with object-based attention and inhibition, which uses saliency information to prioritize between objects for saccadic selection, leads to scanpath statistics with the highest similarity to the human data. This demonstrates that scanpath models benefit from object-based attention and selection, suggesting that object-level attentional units play an important role in guiding attentional processing.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Movimientos Sacádicos , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Percepción Visual/fisiología
18.
J Neurophysiol ; 130(3): 652-670, 2023 09 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37584096

RESUMEN

Visual motion drives smooth pursuit eye movements through a sensory-motor decoder that uses multiple parallel neural pathways to transform the population response in extrastriate area MT into movement. We evaluated the decoder by challenging pursuit in monkeys with reduced motion reliability created by reducing coherence of motion in patches of dots. Our strategy was to determine how reduced dot coherence changes the population response in MT. We then predicted the properties of a decoder that transforms the MT population response into dot coherence-induced deficits in the initiation of pursuit and steady-state tracking. During pursuit initiation, decreased dot coherence reduces MT population response amplitude without changing the preferred speed at its peak. The successful decoder reproduces the measured eye movements by multiplication of 1) the estimate of target speed from the peak of the population response with 2) visual-motor gain based on the amplitude of the population response. During steady-state tracking, the decoder that worked for pursuit initiation failed to reproduce the paradox that steady-state eye speeds do not accelerate to the target speed despite persistent image motion. It predicted eye acceleration to target speed even when monkeys' eye speeds were steady at well below the target speed. To account for the effect of dot coherence on steady-state eye speed, we postulate that the decoder uses sensory-motor gain to modulate the eye velocity positive feedback that normally sustains perfect steady-state tracking. Then, poor steady-state tracking persists because of balance between eye deceleration caused by low positive feedback gain and acceleration driven by MT.NEW & NOTEWORTHY By challenging a sensory-motor system with degraded sensory stimuli, we reveal how the sensory-motor decoder transforms the population response in extrastriate area MT into commands for the initiation and steady-state behavior of smooth pursuit eye movements. Conclusions are based on measuring population responses in MT for multiple target speeds and different levels of motion reliability and evaluating a decoder with a biologically motivated architecture to determine the decoder properties that create the measured eye movements.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Animales , Movimientos Oculares , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Macaca mulatta , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
19.
Neuron ; 111(15): 2448-2460.e6, 2023 08 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37536289

RESUMEN

Information transmission between neural populations could occur through either coordinated changes in firing rates or the precise transmission of spike timing. We investigate the code for information transmission from a part of the cerebellar cortex that is crucial for the accurate execution of a quantifiable motor behavior. Simultaneous recordings from Purkinje cell pairs in the cerebellum of rhesus macaques reveal how these cells coordinate their activity to drive smooth pursuit eye movements. Purkinje cells show millisecond-scale coordination of spikes (synchrony), but the level of synchrony is small and insufficient to impact the firing of downstream vestibular nucleus neurons. Analysis of previous metrics that purported to reveal Purkinje cell synchrony demonstrates that these metrics conflate changes in firing rate and neuron-neuron covariance. We conclude that the output of the cerebellar cortex uses primarily a rate rather than a synchrony code to drive the activity of downstream neurons and thus control motor behavior.


Asunto(s)
Cerebelo , Células de Purkinje , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Cerebelo/fisiología , Células de Purkinje/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Potenciales de Acción/fisiología
20.
Psychophysiology ; 60(12): e14384, 2023 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37431573

RESUMEN

In the current study, we used abrupt-onset distractors similar and dissimilar in luminance to the target of a smooth pursuit eye-movement to test if abrupt-onset distractors capture attention in a top-down or bottom-up fashion while the eyes track a moving object. Abrupt onset distractors were presented at different positions relative to the current position of a pursuit target during the closed-loop phase of smooth pursuit. Across experiments, we varied the duration of the distractors, their motion direction, and task-relevance. We found that abrupt-onset distractors decreased the gain of horizontally directed smooth-pursuit eye-movements. This effect, however, was independent of the similarity in luminance between distractor and target. In addition, distracting effects on horizontal gain were the same, regardless of the exact duration and position of the distractors, suggesting that capture was relatively unspecific and short-lived (Experiments 1 and 2). This was different with distractors moving in a vertical direction, perpendicular to the horizontally moving target. In line with past findings, these distractors caused suppression of vertical gain (Experiment 3). Finally, making distractors task-relevant by asking observers to report distractor positions increased the pursuit gain effect of the distractors. This effect was also independent of target-distractor similarity (Experiment 4). In conclusion, the results suggest that a strong location signal exerted by the pursuit targets led to very brief and largely location-unspecific interference through the abrupt onsets and that this interference was bottom-up, implying that the control of smooth pursuit was independent of other target features besides its motion signal.


Asunto(s)
Movimientos Oculares , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme , Humanos , Atención , Tiempo de Reacción
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA
...