Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
Mais filtros












Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
2.
Anim Cogn ; 26(3): 885-905, 2023 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36583802

RESUMO

We sometimes perceive meaningful patterns or images in random arrangements of colors and shapes. This phenomenon is called pareidolia and has recently been studied intensively, especially face pareidolia. In contrast, there are few comparative-cognitive studies on face pareidolia with nonhuman primates. This study explored behavioral evidence for face pareidolia in chimpanzees using visual search and matching tasks. Faces are processed in a configural manner, and their perception and recognition are hampered by inversion and misalignment of top and bottom parts. We investigated whether the same effect occurs in a visual search for face-like objects. The results showed an effect of misalignment. On the other hand, consistent results were not obtained with the photographs of fruits. When only the top or bottom half of the face-like object was presented, chimpanzees showed better performance for the top-half condition, suggesting the importance of the eye area in face pareidolia. In the positive-control experiments, chimpanzees received the same experiment using human faces and human participants with face-like objects and fruits. As a result, chimpanzees showed an inefficient search for inverted and misaligned faces and humans for manipulated face-like objects. Finally, to examine the role of face awareness, we tested matching a human face to a face-like object in chimpanzees but obtained no substantial evidence that they saw the face-like object as a "face." Based on these results, we discussed the extents and limits of face pareidolia in chimpanzees.


Assuntos
Face , Pan troglodytes , Humanos , Animais , Pan troglodytes/psicologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Estimulação Luminosa
3.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 7148, 2020 04 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32345997

RESUMO

Both humans and chimpanzees have better performances when recognizing faces or bodies when the stimuli are upright compared to inverted. This is called the inversion effect. It suggests that these two species use a specific way to process faces and bodies. Previous research has suggested that humans also show the inversion effect to objects that they have expertise about, and this is called the expert effect. We investigated whether chimpanzees show the expert effect and how humans and chimpanzees differ by testing chimpanzees (human experts) with human body stimuli and testing humans (chimpanzee experts) with chimpanzee and human body stimuli in body recognition tasks. The main finding was that humans (chimpanzee experts) showed the expert effect to chimpanzee bodies, while chimpanzees partially showed it to human bodies. This suggests that compared with chimpanzees, the special processing in humans can be more flexibly tuned for other objects. We also tested humans that were not chimpanzee experts using chimpanzee body stimuli. Although they showed similar performances as the chimpanzee experts, the two groups had differences in some situations, indicating the effect of expertise. This study revealed the important role of experience in object processing in humans, and our evolutionary relatives, chimpanzees.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal , Pan troglodytes/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos
4.
Primates ; 59(2): 135-144, 2018 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29383576

RESUMO

The Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island (OUI) Foundation has been conducting behavioral and veterinary research on orangutans as an attempt at ex situ conservation. Since 2010, the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University has been collaborating with OUI to promote environmental enrichment and infant rearing by biological mothers in addition to the continuous efforts of refining the veterinary management of the endangered species. In 2011, three Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus) were released on an island, called BJ Island, adjacent to OUI. This island is approximately 5.6 ha in size, and 635 trees belonging to 102 plant species were identified prior to their release. Behavioral monitoring of the released individuals has been conducted to evaluate their behavioral adaptation to the new environment. Two of the three released orangutans were born in the wild, whereas the youngest individual was born on OUI and expected to learn forest survival strategies from the two older individuals. One of the orangutans was pregnant at the time of release and subsequently gave birth to two male infants on BJ Island. The behavioral monitoring indicated that these orangutans traveled more and spent more time on trees following their release onto BJ Island. However, resting was longer for two females both on OUI and BJ Island when compared to other populations. The orangutans consumed some natural food resources on BJ Island. The release into a more naturalistic environment may help the orangutans to develop more naturalistic behavioral patterns that resemble their wild counterparts.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Pongo pygmaeus/fisiologia , Medicina Veterinária/métodos , Animais , Doenças dos Símios Antropoides/etiologia , Doenças dos Símios Antropoides/terapia , Ecossistema , Espécies em Perigo de Extinção , Feminino , Variação Genética , Ilhas , Malásia , Masculino , Filogenia , Medicina Veterinária/organização & administração , Medicina Veterinária/normas
5.
Biol Lett ; 13(11)2017 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29142043

RESUMO

Referential signals, such as manual pointing or deictic words, allow individuals to efficiently locate a specific entity in the environment, using distance-specific linguistic and/or gestural units. To explore the evolutionary prerequisites of such deictic ability, the present study investigates the ability of chimpanzees to adjust their communicative signals to the distance of a referent. A food-request paradigm in which the chimpanzees had to request a close or distant piece of food on a table in the presence/absence of an experimenter was employed. Our main finding concerns the chimpanzees adjusting their requesting behaviours to the distance of the food such that higher manual gestures and larger mouth openings were used to request the distant piece of food. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that chimpanzees are able to use distance-specific gestures.


Assuntos
Comunicação Animal , Gestos , Pan troglodytes/psicologia , Animais , Alimentos , Humanos , Motivação
6.
Proc Biol Sci ; 284(1861)2017 Aug 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28835550

RESUMO

Humans can extract statistical information, such as the average size of a group of objects or the general emotion of faces in a crowd without paying attention to any individual object or face. To determine whether summary perception is unique to humans, we investigated the evolutional origins of this ability by assessing whether chimpanzees, which are closely related to humans, can also determine the average size of multiple visual objects. Five chimpanzees and 18 humans were able to choose the array in which the average size was larger, when presented with a pair of arrays, each containing 12 circles of different or the same sizes. Furthermore, both species were more accurate in judging the average size of arrays consisting of 12 circles of different or the same sizes than they were in judging the average size of arrays consisting of a single circle. Our findings could not be explained by the use of a strategy in which the chimpanzee detected the largest or smallest circle among those in the array. Our study provides the first evidence that chimpanzees can perceive the average size of multiple visual objects. This indicates that the ability to compute the statistical properties of a complex visual scene is not unique to humans, but is shared between both species.


Assuntos
Pan troglodytes/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho , Percepção Visual , Animais , Humanos
7.
Primates ; 58(1): 93-101, 2017 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27485748

RESUMO

Spontaneous smiles are facial movements that are characterized by lip corner raises that occur during irregular sleep or drowsiness without known external or internal causes. They are shown by human infants and infant chimpanzees. These smiles are considered to be the developmental origin of smiling and laughter. There are some case studies showing that spontaneous smiles occur in Japanese macaques. The goals of this study were to investigate whether newborn Japanese macaques show a considerable number of spontaneous smiles thus to examine the mechanism of them. Seven newborn Japanese macaques were observed in a room for an average of 44 min, and incidental sleeping situations were monitored twice. All seven participants showed spontaneous smiles at least once during the observation. They showed 8.29 spontaneous smiles in average (SD = 10.89; 58 smiles in total), all found in the state of REM sleep. Thirty-nine of the 58 smiles were produced on the left side of the mouth. These characteristics were similar to those of spontaneous smiles in human infants. This is the first evidence that macaques as well as hominoids show a considerable number of spontaneous smiles. These phenomena may facilitate the development of the zygomaticus major muscle, which is implicated in smiling-like facial expressions.


Assuntos
Macaca/fisiologia , Sorriso , Animais , Feminino , Masculino
8.
Infant Behav Dev ; 35(3): 466-71, 2012 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22721744

RESUMO

Five-hundred-sixty-eight fetuses were observed at a clinic by using four-dimensional ultrasonography, and 31 fetuses who showed smiles were selected. The range of conceptional age was from 156 days to 214 days. The participants exhibited 51 smiles in 62 min of recording. The average duration of smiles was 3.21s (SD = 1.98). This must be the first study which collected fetal smiles intensively. The age effect on the frequency and duration of fetal smiles is not examined in this study. In comparison with previous studies, the average duration of fetal smiles and that of spontaneous smiles in preterm neonates (3.28s; Kawakami et al., 2008) were not significantly different, but they were significantly longer than that of spontaneous smiles in full-term neonates (1.97s; Kawakami et al., 2006). The fact that fetuses show a lot of smiles makes us reconsider the meanings of spontaneous smiles; fetal smiles must not result from social effects.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Fetal , Feto/fisiologia , Sorriso/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Análise de Variância , Humanos , Ultrassonografia Pré-Natal
9.
Infant Behav Dev ; 34(2): 264-9, 2011 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21397951

RESUMO

Twenty-two pairs of typically developing toddlers (M=24.32 months) and their mothers were observed in a play-room solving puzzles during 30 min. The target of the observations was hand-taking gesture. Researchers have thought that this gesture is rare among typically developing children and is more frequent among autistic children. Ten in 22 children showed this gesture in only 30 min. They should know "I can not do it by myself, but my mother can do it." When we can assume that children know others' mental mechanism, it might be the origins of a theory of mind.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Gestos , Relações Mãe-Filho , Jogos e Brinquedos/psicologia , Teoria da Mente/fisiologia , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Mãos , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Pensamento/fisiologia
10.
Infant Behav Dev ; 32(4): 416-21, 2009 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19665804

RESUMO

Five infants were observed longitudinally. In over 30 h of observations, seven spontaneous smiles and one spontaneous laugh were found. All smiles were observed in infants between the ages of 10 and 15 months. These data indicate that spontaneous smiles do not disappear at 2 months of age and they still exist at over 15 months. This disproves some emotional expression theories, where spontaneous smiles are considered to be precursor to and replaced by social smiles. Our data suggest that those theories must be revised and provide new perspectives on this field of studies.


Assuntos
Comportamento do Lactente/psicologia , Sorriso/psicologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Riso/psicologia , Masculino
11.
Infant Behav Dev ; 31(3): 518-22, 2008 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18468691

RESUMO

Twenty-two preterm neonates were observed 1h per neonate in the NICU. Ninety-five spontaneous smiles were recorded. Younger and smaller neonates showed more and longer spontaneous smiles than older and larger. The youngest neonate was 200 days from conception on the observational day. She was 511 g. This infant showed spontaneous smiles. The roots of spontaneous smiles are discussed.


Assuntos
Recém-Nascido Prematuro/fisiologia , Sorriso/fisiologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Recém-Nascido Prematuro/psicologia , Unidades de Terapia Intensiva Neonatal/tendências , Masculino , Sorriso/psicologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...