Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 17.256
Filtrar
1.
Transl Vis Sci Technol ; 13(9): 8, 2024 Sep 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39235398

RESUMO

Purpose: Crowding is the inability to distinguish objects in the periphery in the presence of clutter. Previous studies showed that crowding is elevated in patients with glaucoma. This could serve as an indicator of the functional visual performance of patients with glaucoma but at present appears too time-consuming and attentionally demanding. We examined visual crowding in individuals with preperimetric glaucoma to compare the potential effectiveness of eye movement-based and manual response paradigms. Methods: We assessed crowding magnitude in 10 participants with preperimetric glaucoma and 10 age-matched controls. Crowding magnitudes were assessed using four different paradigms: a conventional two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) manual, a 2AFC and a six-alternative forced choice (6AFC) eye movement, and a serial search paradigm. All paradigms measured crowding magnitude by comparing participants' orientation discrimination thresholds in isolated and flanked stimulus conditions. Moreover, assessment times and participant preferences were compared across paradigms. Results: Patients with preperimetric glaucoma exhibited elevated crowding, which was most evident in the manual-response paradigm. The serial search paradigm emerged as the fastest method for assessing thresholds, yet it could not effectively distinguish between glaucoma and control groups. The 6AFC paradigm proved challenging for both groups. Conclusions: We conclude that patients with preperimetric glaucoma demonstrate heightened binocular visual crowding. This is most effectively demonstrated via the 2AFC manual response paradigm. The additional attentional demand in eye movement paradigms rendered them less effective in the elderly population of the present study. Translational Relevance: Our findings underscore both the value and the complexity of efficiently evaluating crowding in elderly participants, including those with preperimetric glaucoma.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares , Glaucoma , Campos Visuais , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Idoso , Glaucoma/fisiopatologia , Glaucoma/diagnóstico , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Testes de Campo Visual/métodos , Acuidade Visual/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
2.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 65(11): 34, 2024 09 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39312223

RESUMO

Purpose: People with albinism (PwA) are known to have visual impairments; however, little is known about whether these functions are disrupted across earlier and later stages of the visual pathway. We investigated distinct perceptual functions and fixation stability within each observer and compared the data with age- (±5 years) and sex-matched controls. Methods: Twenty-one self-reported PwA and twenty-one controls were recruited. Angular-indication measurement (AIM) and foraging-interactive-D-prime (FInD) psychophysical methods were deployed to measure OS, OD, and OU near visual acuity, spatial contrast sensitivity function (CSF), temporal contrast sensitivity (tCS; 0.5 c/°; horizontal grating: 0, 1, 2, 4, and 8 Hz), OU glare acuity, threshold-versus-contrast (2c/° vertical grating), long, medium, and short wavelength cone-isolated color detection, color discrimination, stereoacuity across spatial frequencies (1c/°, 2c/°, 4c/°, 8c/°), horizontal, circular, radial pattern and motion coherence, and equivalent-noise motion detection. Thresholds were determined by AIM and FInD and compared using N-ANOVAs, t-tests, planned multi-comparisons, correlations, and unsupervised, agglomerative hierarchical cluster analysis for each group. Results: We found significant differences between groups for most visual functions except for simple and complex form-coherence (two way-ANOVAs, P > 0.05) and complex motion coherence. Correlations between outcomes revealed more significant correlations for PwA and differences in the specific correlates between groups. Unsupervised hierarchical clustering revealed different functional clusters between groups. Conclusions: AIM and FInD successfully interrogated visual deficits in PwA. Overall, PwA showed impaired performance in achromatic, chromatic, temporal, and binocular functions, and had higher intrinsic noise levels. Midlevel vision was comparable between groups. Unsupervised cluster analysis and correlation between outcomes revealed a difference in functional outcome clusters between groups. The results may help to increase the efficiency of screening and identify target deficits for rehabilitation.


Assuntos
Sensibilidades de Contraste , Acuidade Visual , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Acuidade Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Albinismo/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Visão/fisiopatologia , Transtornos da Visão/diagnóstico , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia
3.
PLoS One ; 19(9): e0306817, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39321177

RESUMO

Detection canines are deployed to detect explosives in a wide range of environmental conditions. These environmental conditions may have negative impacts on canine capabilities as a sensor. This study leveraged an air dilution olfactometer to present controlled odor concentrations of four different energetic materials (double base smokeless powder, Composition C4, ammonium nitrate, and flake Trinitrotoluene) to dogs working in a range of high temperature, standard, and low temperature conditions with high and low humidity conditions. The air dilution olfactometer controlled concentrations independent of environmental condition. Dogs' detection threshold limits were measured using a descending staircase procedure. We measured dogs' threshold twice for each energetic under each environmental condition. Results indicated heterogeneity in effects based on energetic, but all odors were detected at their lowest concentrations under standard conditions. Smokeless powder detection was reduced under all environmental conditions compared to standard and was least detectable under high temperature and humidity conditions. AN detection was poorest under high temperature high and low humidity conditions. C4 in contrast, was least detectable at low temperatures with high humidity. TNT detection was difficult under all conditions, so decrements due to environmental conditions were not statistically detectable. Additional measures were also found to be associated with detection limits. Under high temperature conditions, correlations were observed between canine mean subcutaneous temperature and detection limits, such that dogs experiencing greater temperature increases showed poorer detection limits. In addition, dog's latency to sample the odor port from the onset of a trial was longest in the high temperature conditions. Longer latencies were also predictive of poorer detection performance. Overall, dogs showed deficits in detection sensitivity limits under all environmental conditions for at least one energetic material when the concentration of that energetic material was not directly impacted by the environmental conditions. These results suggest that behavioral factors related to environmental exposure can have important impacts on canine detection sensitivity and should be considered in operational environments.


Assuntos
Substâncias Explosivas , Animais , Cães , Substâncias Explosivas/análise , Umidade , Masculino , Odorantes/análise , Temperatura , Feminino , Limiar Sensorial , Olfato/fisiologia , Meio Ambiente
4.
Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol ; 281(10): 5541-5547, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39179916

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This pilot study aimed to determine whether the Henkin olfactory test discriminates between the olfactory function of patients with olfactory loss and healthy individuals, and whether the Henkin test is correlated with a validated psychophysical olfactory test. METHODS: Participants underwent olfactory testing using the modified Henkin test (including detection [DT] and recognition [RT] thresholds, magnitude estimation [ME], and hedonic ratings [H], for four different odors) and the extended "Sniffin' Sticks" test battery (odor detection, discrimination, identification). RESULTS: Forty-four individuals (28 women, aged 19-81 years, mean: 39 years) were included. Twenty-three were healthy (mean age: 38 years) and 21 had olfactory dysfunction (OD) (mean age: 40 years). OD patients had worse mean DT, lower composite RT, and lower ME ratings. Mean and individual odor H ratings were not significantly different between the groups. Most individuals were classified as hyposmic when using the prescribed classification by Henkin, with only very few satisfying the stringent criteria of anosmia and normosmia. Modified Henkin subtests were all positively correlated with each other and with the "Sniffin' Sticks", except for Unpleasant Mean H which was only correlated with Pleasant Mean H; and Pleasant mean H which was not correlated with mean DT scores. CONCLUSION: The Henkin test is able to separate between OD patients and controls. However, modifications to the conduct of this test may be required. Studies that used this test should be carefully interpreted.


Assuntos
Odorantes , Transtornos do Olfato , Limiar Sensorial , Olfato , Humanos , Feminino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto , Idoso , Masculino , Transtornos do Olfato/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Olfato/fisiopatologia , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Projetos Piloto , Olfato/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Psicofísica/métodos , Estudos de Casos e Controles
5.
Chem Senses ; 492024 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39215656

RESUMO

A common tool to measure olfactory function is the Sniffin' Sticks Test extended version (SSET). The SSET evaluates olfactory ability by summing the scores of three subtests: Threshold, Discrimination, and Identification. Recent meta-scientific literature revealed that many psychometric instruments currently in use have not been adequately validated, leading to a measurement crisis that raises concerns about the validity of the conclusions drawn with these instruments. Two examples of the measurement crisis are (i) the use of sum scores without testing their assumptions (e.g. unidimensionality and tau-equivalence), which indicate that all subtests have the same, stable relationship with their underlying construct, and (ii) the lack of assessment of measurement invariance across groups. Here, we aim to investigate the unidimensionality and tau-equivalence assumptions, internal consistency, and measurement invariance of sex and age groups of the SSET. We tested 988 (555 females, mean ±â€…SD: 39.75 ±â€…18.60 years) participants with the Italian version of the SSET. The tau-equivalent model demonstrated excellent fit indices (CFI robust = 1, TLI robust = 1, RMSEA robust = 0, SRMR = 0.013), which best explain the data, indicating that all subtests are equally important in measuring olfactory function, but not necessarily equally precise. The results also revealed full measurement invariance across age groups and configural, partial metric, and scalar invariance across sexes, indicating that the use of latent means to compare sex groups should be chosen over raw scores. However, the SSET demonstrated moderate internal consistency. Future studies should clarify whether the reliability of the SSET can be increased.


Assuntos
Psicometria , Olfato , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Olfato/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Idoso , Adolescente , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Odorantes/análise , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(10): 2443-2455, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39162730

RESUMO

Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation (GVS) has been proposed as an alternative display modality to relay information without increasing demands on the visual or auditory sensory modalities of the wearer or in environments where those modalities cannot be used (e.g., covert night operations). We further investigated this concept with four experiments designed to test: (1) thresholds at which subjects could distinguish between different GVS current amplitudes and polarities, (2) thresholds at which different bipolar (i.e., sinusoidal waveform with current oscillating between left and right directions) current frequencies were distinguishable among room temperature, hot, cold, and windy environments, (3) effects of unipolar (i.e., sinusoidal waveform with current occurring in only the left or right direction) currents on balance performance, and (4) dual-task performance among frequency and polarity modulated GVS conditions during a concordant visual search task. Subjects reliably distinguished between current amplitudes that varied from a pedestal of ± 0.6 mA by a median of 0.03 mA (range of 0.02-0.32 mA) and between unipolar currents at a median amplitude of 0.55 mA (range of 0.32-0.83 mA). GVS frequency thresholds were robust to the environment conditions tested, with no statistical differences found. Sway and balance errors were increased with unipolar currents. GVS thresholds were not impacted by the dual-task paradigm, but the visual search scores were slightly elevated when congruently performing a polarity thresholding task. Overall findings continue to support GVS use as a display modality, but some limitations are noted, such as the use of unipolar currents under scenarios where postural control is important.


Assuntos
Equilíbrio Postural , Postura , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Postura/fisiologia , Equilíbrio Postural/fisiologia , Estimulação Elétrica , Meio Ambiente , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Vestíbulo do Labirinto/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
7.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 17836, 2024 08 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39090148

RESUMO

The capacity to perceive tactile input at the fingertips, referred to as tactile sensitivity, is known to diminish with age due to regressive changes to mechanoreceptor density and morphology. Sensitivity is measured as perceptual responses to stimuli of varying intensity. Contrary to traditional sensitivity monitoring instruments, smartphones are uniquely suited for remote assessment and have shown to deliver highly calibrated stimuli along a broad spectrum of intensity, which may improve test reliability. The aim of this study was to evaluate a vibration-emitting smartphone application, the Vibratus App, as a mode of estimating tactile sensory thresholds in the aging adult. The peripheral nerve function of 40 neurologically healthy volunteers (ages 18-71) was measured using monofilaments, a 128-Hz tuning fork, the Vibratus App, and nerve conduction studies (NCS). Between group differences were analyzed to determine each measurement's sensitivity to age. Spearman correlation coefficients depicted the associative strength between hand-held measurements and sensory nerve action potential (SNAP) amplitude. Inter-rater reliability of traditional instruments and the software-operated smartphone were assessed by intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC2,k). Measurements taken with Vibratus App were significantly different between age groups (p < 0.001). The inter-rater reliability of monofilament, smartphone vibration, and tuning fork testing was moderate to good (ICC2,k = 0.65, 0.69, and 0.79, respectively). The findings of this study support further investigation of smartphones as sensitivity monitoring devices for at home monitoring of skin sensitivity.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento , Limiar Sensorial , Smartphone , Vibração , Humanos , Adulto , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Idoso , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto Jovem , Adolescente , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Pele , Aplicativos Móveis , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
8.
PLoS One ; 19(8): e0304874, 2024.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39106272

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: The olfactory and trigeminal system are closely interlinked. Existing literature has primarily focused on characterizing trigeminal stimulation through mechanical and chemical stimulation, neglecting thermal stimulation thus far. The present study aimed to characterize the intranasal sensitivity to heat and the expression of trigeminal receptors (transient receptor potential channels, TRP). METHODS: A total of 20 healthy participants (aged 21-27 years, 11 women) were screened for olfactory function and trigeminal sensitivity using several tests. Under endoscopic control, a thermal stimulator was placed in 7 intranasal locations: anterior septum, lateral vestibulum, interior nose tip, lower turbinate, middle septum, middle turbinate, and olfactory cleft to determine the thermal threshold. Nasal swabs were obtained in 3 different locations (anterior septum, middle turbinate, olfactory cleft) to analyze the expression of trigeminal receptors TRP: TRPV1, TRPV3, TRPA1, TRPM8. RESULTS: The thermal threshold differed between locations (p = 0.018), with a trend for a higher threshold at the anterior septum (p = 0.092). There were no differences in quantitative receptor expression (p = 0.46) at the different sites. The highest overall receptor RNA expression was detected for TRPV1 over all sites (p<0.001). The expression of TRPV3 was highest at the anterior septum compared to the middle turbinate or the olfactory cleft. The thermal sensitivity correlated with olfactory sensitivity and results from tests were related to trigeminal function like intensity ratings of ammonium, a questionnaire regarding trigeminal function, nasal patency, and CO2 thresholds. However, no correlation was found between receptor expression and psychophysical measures of trigeminal function. DISCUSSION: This study provided the first insights about intranasal thermal sensitivity and suggested the presence of topographical differences in thermal thresholds. There was no correlation between thermal sensitivity and trigeminal mRNA receptor expression. However, thermal sensitivity was found to be associated with psychophysical measures of trigeminal and olfactory function.


Assuntos
Mucosa Nasal , Canais de Cátion TRPV , Humanos , Feminino , Adulto , Masculino , Mucosa Nasal/metabolismo , Adulto Jovem , Canais de Cátion TRPV/metabolismo , Canais de Cátion TRPV/genética , Temperatura Alta , Nervo Trigêmeo/fisiologia , Nervo Trigêmeo/metabolismo , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Canais de Potencial de Receptor Transitório/metabolismo , Canais de Potencial de Receptor Transitório/genética , Canais de Cátion TRPM/metabolismo , Canais de Cátion TRPM/genética , Sensação Térmica/fisiologia , Canal de Cátion TRPA1/metabolismo , Canal de Cátion TRPA1/genética
9.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 86(6): 2187-2209, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39107652

RESUMO

The perception of temporal order or simultaneity of stimuli is almost always explained in terms of independent-channels models, such as perceptual-moment, triggered-moment, and attention-switching models. Independent-channels models generally posit that stimuli are processed in separate peripheral channels and that their arrival-time difference at a central location is translated into an internal state of order (simultaneity) if it reaches (misses) a certain threshold. Non-monotonic and non-parallel psychometric functions in a ternary-response task provided critical evidence against a wide range of independent-channels models. However, two independent-channels models have been introduced in the last decades that can account for such shapes by considering misreports of internal states (response-error model) or by assuming that simultaneity and order judgments rely on distinct sensory and decisional processes (two-stage model). Based on previous ideas, we also consider a two-threshold model, according to which the same arrival-time difference may need to reach a higher threshold for order detection than for successiveness detection. All three models were fitted to various data sets collected over a period of more than a century. The two-threshold model provided the best balance between goodness of fit and parsimony. This preference for the two-threshold model over the two-stage model and the response-error model aligns well with several lines of evidence from cognitive modeling, psychophysics, mental chronometry, and psychophysiology. We conclude that the seemingly deviant shapes of psychometric functions can be explained within the framework of independent-channels models in a simpler way than previously assumed.


Assuntos
Julgamento , Percepção do Tempo , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos , Psicometria , Atenção/fisiologia , Psicofísica , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
10.
Vision Res ; 223: 108474, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39178751

RESUMO

The aim of this study was to assess the perceptibility and acceptability thresholds of maxillofacial silicones for light and dark skin colors and to evaluate the effect of gender and professional experience on these thresholds. Two different sets of specimens (as light and dark) each, consisting of 14 (25 × 25 × 6-mm3) silicone skin replications, were produced. Four specimens of each set were produced from the same silicon mixture of the relevant set and polymerized simultaneously to provide standardized fabrication conditions. These 4 light/dark specimens were assigned as "baseline color specimens (BCs)" in each set, while the other 10 specimens were produced with a color difference level that increased gradually from BC. These stepped levels were obtained by controlled increasing of the pigment concentration in the relevant baseline silicon mixture. Color difference levels of specimens were calculated by using the CIELAB and CIEDE2000 formulas. Observers comprised of 3 different professional experiences as first-year dental students, interns, and dentists (n = 30/group, gender-balanced) were included. Combinations consisting of 5 specimens with 4 BCs and 1 different color were shown to the observers to assess whether the color difference was perceptible or acceptable. Perceptibility and acceptability percentages were regressed with color difference levels to estimate the best fit curve and confidence intervals were calculated (α = 0.05). The highest estimation of the coefficient of determination (R2) was found in the cubic curve for all parameters. A significant difference was found between the light and dark colors. The perceptibility thresholds (ΔE*ab/ΔE00) were 0.8/0.59 and 2.63/1.75 for light and dark colors, respectively. The acceptability thresholds (ΔE*ab/ΔE00) were 3.35/2.25, 10.07/7.04 for light and dark colors, respectively. No significant differences were found between gender and among experience groups concerning visual thresholds. Regardless of gender and experience, observers could perceive color differences more easily in light skin colors.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Limiar Sensorial , Pigmentação da Pele , Humanos , Feminino , Masculino , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Pigmentação da Pele/fisiologia , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Silicones , Pigmentação em Prótese/normas , Odontólogos
11.
Vision Res ; 223: 108460, 2024 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39094263

RESUMO

Neon color spreading (NCS) is an illusory color phenomenon that provides a dramatic example of surface completion and filling-in. Numerous studies have varied both spatial and temporal aspects of the neon-generating stimulus to explore variations in the strength of the effect. Here, we take a novel, parametric, low-level psychophysical approach to studying NCS in two experiments. In Experiment 1, we test the ability of both cone-isolating and equiluminant stimuli to generate neon color spreading for both increments and decrements in cone modulations. As expected, sensitivity was low to S(hort-wavelength) cone stimuli due to their poor spatial resolution, but sensitivity was similar for the other color directions. We show that when these differences in detection sensitivity are accounted for, the particular cone type, and the polarity (increment or decrement), make little difference in generating neon color spreading, with NCS visible at about twice detection threshold level in all cases. In Experiment 2, we use L-cone flicker modulations (reddish and greenish excursions around grey) to study sensitivity to NCS as a function of temporal frequency from 0.5 to 8 Hz. After accounting for detectability, the temporal contrast sensitivity functions for NCS are approximately constant or even increase over the studied frequency range. Therefore there is no evidence in this study that the processes underlying NCS are slower than the low-level processes of simple flicker detection. These results point to relatively fast mechanisms, not slow diffusion processes, as the substrate for NCS.


Assuntos
Percepção de Cores , Estimulação Luminosa , Psicofísica , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones , Limiar Sensorial , Humanos , Percepção de Cores/fisiologia , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Cones/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Adulto , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Feminino , Masculino , Ilusões Ópticas/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
12.
Vestn Otorinolaringol ; 89(3): 36-40, 2024.
Artigo em Russo | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39104271

RESUMO

The psychophysical Sniffin' Sticks test, which includes an odor identification test, is the gold standard for assessing the sense of smell in clinical and scientific practice. A necessary requirement for the odor identification test is a close familiarity with the odors used by the inhabitants of the region in which it is used. We studied 77 healthy volunteers and 51 patients with olfactory dysfunction and we found that Russians are not familiar with the three smells from the test (licorice, turpentine and anise) and are completely unfamiliar with the one proposed alternative answer (chives). Moreover, four odors demonstrated very low recognition (less than 75%). The test has been adapted for the use In Russia. In the booklet, licorice is replaced by cough syrup, turpentine by paint thinner, and chives by bay leaf. For odors with low recognition (lemon, apple, pineapple), the alternative fruity odors in the booklet were replaced with more contrasting ones. Based on the data obtained, we are going to develop a domestic version of the odor identification test.


Assuntos
Odorantes , Transtornos do Olfato , Olfato , Humanos , Federação Russa , Odorantes/análise , Masculino , Feminino , Transtornos do Olfato/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Olfato/fisiopatologia , Transtornos do Olfato/etiologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Adulto , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
13.
Vestn Otorinolaringol ; 89(3): 41-47, 2024.
Artigo em Russo | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39104272

RESUMO

Olfactory disorders is one of the first symptoms of diseases from various departments of medicine (otorhinolaryngology, psychology, neurology, etc.). Based on international clinical recommendations, olfactory tests are the gold standard for the diagnosis of olfactory disorders. There are many different psychophysical tests: UPSIT (USA, Pennsylvania), Sniffin' Sticks test (Germany), BAST-24 (Spain), etc. Currently, there is an acute shortage of olfactory tests available for clinical practice In Russia. This problem is related to the fact that there are no olfactory tests registered as medical devices on the territory of the Russian Federation. Also, a significant limitation is the unrecognizability of odors by the population of our country, which include foreign analogues (licorice, anise, turpentine, etc.). OBJECTIVE: To develop and validate the national olfactory test on healthy volunteers. MATERIAL AND METHODS: The development and validation of the olfactory test included several stages. First, the development of an olfactory test was carried out, the selection of aromas to assess the threshold and identification ability of olfaction. 25 dilutions of n-butanol were used for the assessment of the threshold olfactory ability. For the stage of assessing the identification ability of the sense of smell, in our previous study, an assessment of the recognition of odor names in the territory of the Russian Federation was carried out. A total of 3.000 people from 8 federal districts of the Russian Federation were interviewed. During the development of the test, 20 names of flavors with the highest rating were used. By the 8th, the selection of monocomponent substances was carried out. Commercially available certified food and perfume flavorings have been used for fragrances whose equivalent in the test cannot be a monocomponent substance. A group of 25 healthy volunteers selected a flavor or a monocomponent for each of the 20 positions. To carry out the identification stage of testing, a booklet was developed with answer options for each fragrance, including 80 images associated with the smell. A methodology for conducting diagnostics has been created. Next, the validation of the developed olfactory test was carried out on 150 healthy volunteers. The study included an assessment of the threshold and identification ability of the sense of smell using the developed test and conducting a comparative analysis with a set of flavors and descriptors corresponding to the Sniffin' Sticks test. RESULTS: The developed test includes: 2 panels - panel 1 to assess the threshold ability of smell, panel 2 to assess the identification ability of smell, a booklet with 80 images and captions to them. The norms of threshold and identification olfactory abilities were also determined in the developed test. The domestic test was validated against the relative foreign Sniffin' Sticks test. Spearman's correlation between the accuracy values of the domestic test (17-20; 85.00-100.00%) and the values of the foreign test (11-16; 68.75-100.00%) did not reveal statistically significant differences (rs=0.065, p=0.432), which confirms the equally effective assessment of olfactory ability by the domestic olfactory test in comparison with its foreign counterpart. CONCLUSION: In this work, a methodology for the use of Russian olfactory test was developed and validated on healthy volunteers. The features of the developed test are an assessment of the threshold and identification ability of smell, an adapted set of odors for the Russian population, the use of paper blotters when applying flavor and visual images of descriptors. Despite the wide variety of psychophysical tests, this problem requires further study and comparative analysis of olfactory tests available In Russia and foreign analogues in order to obtain a universal and effective diagnostic method for the populations of our country.This work was supported by the Russian Foundation for Basic Research (Project No. 24-25-00415).


Assuntos
Transtornos do Olfato , Olfato , Humanos , Federação Russa , Transtornos do Olfato/diagnóstico , Adulto , Feminino , Masculino , Olfato/fisiologia , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Odorantes/análise , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
14.
Vision Res ; 222: 108453, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38991467

RESUMO

Visual processing differs between the foveal and peripheral visual field. These differences can lead to different appearances of objects in the periphery and the fovea, posing a challenge to perception across saccades. Differences in the appearance of visual features between the peripheral and foveal visual field may bias change discrimination across saccades. Previously it has been reported that spatial frequency (SF) appears higher in the periphery compared to the fovea (Davis et al., 1987). In this study, we investigated the visual appearance of SF before and after a saccade and the discrimination of SF changes during saccades. In addition, we tested the contributions of pre- and postsaccadic information to change discrimination performance. In the first experiment, we found no differences in the appearance of SF before and after a saccade. However, participants showed a clear bias to report SF increases. Interestingly, a 200-ms postsaccadic blank improved the precision of the responses but did not affect the bias. In the second experiment, participants showed lower thresholds for SF increases than for decreases, suggesting that the bias in the first experiment was not just a response bias. Finally, we asked participants to discriminate the SF of stimuli presented before a saccade. Thresholds in the presaccadic discrimination task were lower than in the change discrimination task, suggesting that transsaccadic change discrimination is not merely limited by presaccadic discrimination in the periphery. The change direction bias might stem from more effective masking or overwriting of the presaccadic stimulus by the postsaccadic low SF stimulus.


Assuntos
Estimulação Luminosa , Movimentos Sacádicos , Humanos , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Masculino , Feminino , Adulto , Adulto Jovem , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Fóvea Central/fisiologia
15.
Vision Res ; 222: 108457, 2024 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39029384

RESUMO

Strong monochromatic point light sources such as Light Emitting Diodes (LED) or Lasers have been increasingly used in recent decades. This also raises the risk of misuse resulting in glare phenomena and associated visual impairment. The objective of this prospective and partially blinded study was the visualization and characterization of glare-induced scotomas in visual field by dazzling with monochromatic point light sources in terms of disability and discomfort glare. Automated threshold perimetry under dazzling by LED exposure at three different wavelengths (470, 530 and 625 nm) and four different intensities (25, 50, 75, and 100%) was performed in 31 healthy subjects resulting in 434 visual field examinations. Visual disability was measured by sensitivity loss in the central 30°as compared to unexposed controls and visualized by reconstruction of mean visual fields for each group via backward-calculation. Psychological glare was assessed by subsequent questionnaire and evaluated based on the de Boer rating scale of discomfort. Increasing glare intensities resulted in a significant decrease in mean sensitivity for all wavelengths tested, paralleled by an increase of discomfort glare. The loss of sensitivity was scattered over all quadrants with accentuation of the corresponding mean exposure area. Reconstructed visual fields confirmed visual impairment in all quadrants at an extent of at least 30°. We conclude that even off-axis light exposure may affect central visual field perception. Our results extend previous research on directed light interaction and contribute in explaining its incapacitating impact on human performance.


Assuntos
Ofuscação , Escotoma , Testes de Campo Visual , Campos Visuais , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Adulto , Escotoma/fisiopatologia , Estudos Prospectivos , Testes de Campo Visual/métodos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
16.
Behav Brain Res ; 472: 115153, 2024 08 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39025432

RESUMO

Rodents use their whisker system to discriminate surface texture. Whisker-based texture discrimination tasks are often used to investigate the mechanisms encoding tactile sensation. One such task is the textured Novel Object Recognition Test (tNORT). It takes advantage of a tendency of rodents to explore novel objects more than familiar ones and assesses the sensitivity of whiskers in discriminating different textures of objects. It requires little training of the animals and the equipment involved is a simple arena with typically two objects placed inside. The success of the test relies on rodents spending sufficient time exploring these objects. Animals may lose interests in such tasks when performed repetitively within a limited time frame. However, such repeated tests may be crucial when establishing a sensitivity threshold of the whisker system. Here we present an adapted rodent tNORT protocol designed to maintain sustained interest in the objects even with repeated testing. We constructed complex objects from three simple-shaped objects. Different textures were provided by sandpapers of varying grit sizes. To minimise olfactory clues, we used the sandy and the laminar side of the same sandpaper as the familiar and novel textures assigned at random. We subsequently conducted repeated tNORTs on eight rats in order to identify a critical threshold of the sandpaper grit size below which rats would be unable to discriminate the sandy from the laminar side. With an inter-test-interval of seven days and after five tNORTs, the protocol enabled us to successfully identify the threshold. We suggest that the proposed tNORT is a useful tool for investigating the sensitivity threshold of the whisker system of rodent, and for testing the effectiveness of an intervention by comparing sensitivity threshold pre- and post-intervention.


Assuntos
Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção do Tato , Vibrissas , Animais , Vibrissas/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Ratos , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Masculino , Comportamento Exploratório/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
17.
Physiol Behav ; 284: 114648, 2024 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39059598

RESUMO

Olfaction acuity, which includes detection thresholds, discrimination and identification, appears to decline with age, obesity, and various neurological disorders. Knowing that smell influences energy intake, there is a growing interest in protecting this sense. Physical activity could be a key intervention to counteract the loss of olfaction. This systematic review aims to explore the literature on the effect of physical activity on olfaction acuity. The search strategy consisted of using index terms and keywords in MEDLINE, EMBASE, EBM Reviews - Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, CINAHL, SPORTDiscus, and Web of Science search engine. Data from 17 trials involving 10,861 participants showed that physical activity improved olfaction thresholds, discrimination, identification and perceived intensity. Regular practice of physical activity seemed to have better effects on olfaction components than acute exercise. Although this review has clarified the evidence on the effects of physical activity on olfaction, better methodological consistency is needed.


Assuntos
Exercício Físico , Olfato , Humanos , Olfato/fisiologia , Exercício Físico/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia
18.
J Texture Stud ; 55(4): e12849, 2024 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38961563

RESUMO

While taste and smell perception have been thoroughly investigated, our understanding of oral somatosensory perception remains limited. Further, assessing and measuring individual differences in oral somatosensory perception pose notable challenges. This review aimed to evaluate the existing methods to assess oral somatosensory perception by examining and comparing the strengths and limitations of each method. The review highlighted the lack of standardized assessment methods and the various procedures within each method. Tactile sensitivity can be assessed using several methods, but each method measures different tactile dimensions. Further investigations are needed to confirm its correlation with texture sensitivity. In addition, measuring a single textural attribute may not provide an overall representation of texture sensitivity. Thermal sensitivity can be evaluated using thermal-change detection or temperature discrimination tests. The chemesthetic sensitivity tests involve either localized or whole-mouth stimulation tests. The choice of an appropriate method for assessing oral somatosensory sensitivity depends on several factors, including the specific research objectives and the target population. Each method has its unique intended purpose, strengths, and limitations, so no universally superior approach exists. To overcome some of the limitations associated with certain methods, the review offers alternative or complementary approaches that could be considered. Researchers can enhance the comprehensive assessment of oral somatosensory sensitivity by carefully selecting and potentially combining methods. In addition, a standardized protocol remains necessary for each method.


Assuntos
Boca , Percepção do Tato , Humanos , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Boca/fisiologia , Individualidade , Percepção Gustatória/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Paladar/fisiologia , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Olfato/fisiologia , Sensação Térmica/fisiologia
19.
J Vis ; 24(7): 2, 2024 Jul 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38953860

RESUMO

Bayesian adaptive methods for sensory threshold determination were conceived originally to track a single threshold. When applied to the testing of vision, they do not exploit the spatial patterns that underlie thresholds at different locations in the visual field. Exploiting these patterns has been recognized as key to further improving visual field test efficiency. We present a new approach (TORONTO) that outperforms other existing methods in terms of speed and accuracy. TORONTO generalizes the QUEST/ZEST algorithm to estimate simultaneously multiple thresholds. After each trial, without waiting for a fully determined threshold, the trial-oriented approach updates not only the location currently tested but also all other locations based on patterns in a reference data set. Since the availability of reference data can be limited, techniques are developed to overcome this limitation. TORONTO was evaluated using computer-simulated visual field tests: In the reliable condition (false positive [FP] = false negative [FN] = 3%), the median termination and root mean square error (RMSE) of TORONTO was 153 trials and 2.0 dB, twice as fast with equal accuracy as ZEST. In the FP = FN = 15% condition, TORONTO terminated in 151 trials and was 2.2 times faster than ZEST with better RMSE (2.6 vs. 3.7 dB). In the FP = FN = 30% condition, TORONTO achieved 4.2 dB RMSE in 148 trials, while all other techniques had > 6.5 dB RMSE and terminated much slower. In conclusion, TORONTO is a fast and accurate algorithm for determining multiple thresholds under a wide range of reliability and subject conditions.


Assuntos
Algoritmos , Psicometria , Limiar Sensorial , Humanos , Psicometria/métodos , Psicometria/normas , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Testes de Campo Visual/métodos , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Teorema de Bayes , Simulação por Computador , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
20.
Neurol India ; 72(3): 540-545, 2024 May 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39041970

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND AIM: The threshold values of two-point discrimination (TPD) provide a numerical measure of tactile acuity. Normal reference values are needed to decide whether sensory variability is within normal sensorial limits. The study aimed to determine the upper extremity and face threshold values in healthy young adults. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Static TPD thresholds of 67 healthy young adults aged 18-35 years were assessed. Eight skin areas in the face and upper extremity on the dominant side were assessed using a "method of limits" approach with an aesthesiometer. Differences between genders were examined with the Mann-Whitney U test. The Spearman correlation analysis investigated the relationship between age and TPD measurements. RESULTS: TPD values ranged between 4.66 and 19.16 mm and 1.33-68.66 mm in the face and upper extremity, respectively, in the participants with a mean age of 23.83 ± 4.66 years. Fingertips and the area over the lateral mandibula showed the greatest sensitivity. The threshold values of TPD showed both interindividual and intraindividual variability. There was no statistical difference in the TPD values according to gender in any of the measured areas, and there was no relationship between age and TPD test values. CONCLUSIONS: The threshold values of TPD have clinical applicability in various diseases affecting the sensation of the upper extremity and/or face. These data may help the detection of early sensory loss.


Assuntos
Face , Limiar Sensorial , Extremidade Superior , Humanos , Adulto Jovem , Adulto , Estudos Transversais , Masculino , Feminino , Adolescente , Limiar Sensorial/fisiologia , Percepção do Tato/fisiologia , Tato/fisiologia , Voluntários Saudáveis
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...