Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 54
Filter
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 198: 108880, 2024 06 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38555063

ABSTRACT

Social perception and communication differ between those with and without autism, even when verbal fluency and intellectual ability are equated. Previous work found that observers responded more quickly to an actor's points if the actor had chosen by themselves where to point instead of being directed where to point. Notably, this 'choice-advantage' effect decreased across non-autistic participants as the number of autistic-like traits and tendencies increased (Pesquita et al., 2016). Here, we build on that work using the same task to study individuals over a broader range of the spectrum, from autistic to non-autistic, measuring both response initiation and mouse movement times, and considering the response to each actor separately. Autistic and non-autistic observers viewed videos of three different actors pointing to one of two locations, without knowing that the actors were sometimes freely choosing to point to one target and other times being directed where to point. All observers exhibited a choice-advantage overall, meaning they responded more rapidly when actors were freely choosing versus when they were directed, indicating a sensitivity to the actors' postural cues and movements. Our fine-grained analyses found a more robust choice-advantage to some actors than others, with autistic observers showing a choice-advantage only in response to one of the actors, suggesting that both actor and observer characteristics influence the overall effect. We briefly explore existing actor characteristics that may have contributed to this effect, finding that both duration of exposure to pre-movement cues and kinematic cues of the actors likely influence the choice advantage to different degrees across the groups. Altogether, the evidence suggested that both autistic and non-autistic individuals could detect the choice-advantage signal, but that for autistic observers the choice-advantage was actor specific. Notably, we found that the influence of the signal, when present, was detected early for all actors by the non-autistic observers, but detected later and only for one actor by the autistic observers. Altogether, we have more accurately characterized the ability of social-perception in autistic individuals as intact, but highlighted that detection of signal is likely delayed/distributed compared to non-autistic observers and that it is important to investigate actor characteristics that may influence detection and use of their social-perception signals.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder , Cues , Social Perception , Humans , Male , Female , Autistic Disorder/psychology , Autistic Disorder/physiopathology , Adult , Young Adult , Visual Perception/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Communication , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Adolescent , Photic Stimulation
2.
Neuropsychologia ; 191: 108726, 2023 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37931746

ABSTRACT

Investigations of grasping real, 3D objects subjected to illusory effects from a pictorial background often choose in-flight grasp aperture as the primary variable to test the hypothesis that the visuomotor system resists the illusion. Here we test an equally important feature of grasps that has received less attention: in-flight grasp orientation. The current study tested a variant of the simultaneous tilt illusion using a mirror-apparatus to manipulate the availability of haptic feedback. Participants performed grasps with haptic feedback (real grasps) and without it (pantomime grasps), reaching for the reflection of a real, 3D bar atop a background grating that induced a 1.1° bias in the perceived orientation of the bar in a separate sample of participants. Analysis of the hand's in-flight grasp orientation at early, late, and end stages of the reach showed that at no point were the real grasps biased by the illusion. In contrast, pantomimed grasps were affected by the illusion at the late and end stages of the reach. At each stage, the effect on the real grasps was significantly weaker than the effect of the illusion as measured by the mean point of subjective equality (PSE) in a two-alternative forced-choice task. In contrast, the effect on the pantomime grasps was statistically indistinguishable from the mean PSE at all three stages of the reach. These findings reinforce the idea that in-flight grasp orientation, like grasp aperture to pictorial illusions of target size, is refractory to pictorial backgrounds that bias perceived orientation.


Subject(s)
Illusions , Humans , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Perception , Attention , Hand Strength
3.
IEEE Trans Biomed Eng ; 68(10): 3048-3058, 2021 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33630734

ABSTRACT

Electrooculography (EOG) can be used to measure eye movements while the eyelids are open or closed and to assist in the diagnosis of certain eye diseases. However, challenges in biosignal acquisition and processing lead to limited accuracy, limited resolution (both temporal and spatial), as well as difficulties in reducing noise and detecting artifacts. Methods such as finite impulse response, wavelet transforms, and averaging filters have been used to denoise and enhance EOG measurements. However, these filters are not specifically designed to detect saccades, and so key features (e.g., saccade amplitude) can be over-filtered and distorted as a consequence of the filtering process. Here we present a model-based fusion technique to enhance saccade features within noisy and raw EOG signals. Specifically, we focus on Westheimer (WH) and linear reciprocal (LR) eye models with a Kalman filter. EOG signals were measured using OpenBCI's Cyton Board (at 250 Hz), and these measurements were compared with a state-of-the-art EyeLink 1000 (EL; 250 Hz) eye tracker. On average, the LR model-based KF produced a 47% improvement of measurement accuracy over the bandpass filters. Thus, we conclude that our LR model-based KF outperforms standard bandpass filtering techniques in reducing noise, eliminating artifacts, and restoring missing features of saccade signatures present within EOG signals.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements , Saccades , Algorithms , Artifacts , Electrooculography , Humans
4.
Int J Popul Data Sci ; 4(2): 1131, 2020 Feb 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32935035

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To profile the Manitoba Centre for Health Policy (MCHP), a population health data centre located at the University of Manitoba in Winnipeg, Canada. APPROACH: We describe how MCHP was established and funded, and how it continues to operate based on a foundation of trust and respect between researchers at the University of Manitoba and stakeholders in the Manitoba Government's Department of Health. MCHP's research priorities are jointly determined by its scientists' own research interests and by questions put forward from Manitoba government ministries. Data governance, data privacy, data linkage processes and data access are discussed in detail. We also provide three illustrative examples of the MCHP Data Repository in action, demonstrating how studies using a variety of Repository datasets have had an impact on health and social policies and programs in Manitoba. DISCUSSION: MCHP has experienced tremendous growth over the last three decades. We discuss emerging research directions as the capacity for innovation at MCHP continues to expand, including a focus on natural language processing and other applications of artificial intelligence techniques, a leadership role in the new SPOR Canadian Data Platform, and a foray into social policy evaluation and analysis. With these and other exciting opportunities on the horizon, the future at MCHP looks exceptionally bright.

5.
Cardiovasc Hematol Disord Drug Targets ; 13(1): 59-72, 2013 Mar 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22882074

ABSTRACT

Cardiovascular disease (CVD) and associated risk factors such as obesity remain at the forefront of health concerns. Adipose tissue has been well established as an endocrine organ that becomes dysfunctional with increased adipose tissue mass. The secretion of several adipokines is altered in subjects with abdominal adiposity and these changes to the endocrine balance may contribute to increased CVD risk. The identification and characterization of disease-specific proteins within the adipose tissue offers a novel therapeutic target for prevention or treatment of cardiovascular complications. This review will discuss the latest developments on therapeutic targets within the context of adipokines, such as adiponectin, C1q/ tumor necrosis factor (TNF) related proteins (CTRPs), visfatin, vaspin, chemerin and omentin, and their involvement in obesity-related cardiovascular complications.


Subject(s)
Adipokines/metabolism , Adipose Tissue/metabolism , Cardiovascular Diseases/metabolism , Obesity/metabolism , Adipose Tissue/physiopathology , Animals , Cardiovascular Diseases/physiopathology , Humans , Obesity/physiopathology , Risk Factors
6.
J Neuroendocrinol ; 24(4): 701-11, 2012 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22309318

ABSTRACT

The cycle of chronic cocaine (CC) use and withdrawal results in increased anxiety, depression and disrupted stress-responsiveness. Oxytocin and corticosterone (CORT) interact to mediate hormonal stress responses and can be altered by cocaine use. These neuroendocrine signals play important regulatory roles in a variety of social behaviours, specifically during the postpartum period, and are sensitive to disruption by CC exposure in both clinical settings and preclinical models. To determine whether CC exposure during pregnancy affected behavioural and hormonal stress response in the early postpartum period in a rodent model, Sprague-Dawley rats were administered cocaine daily (30 mg/kg) throughout gestation (days 1-20). Open field test (OFT) and forced swim test (FST) behaviours were measured on postpartum day 5. Plasma CORT concentrations were measured before and after testing throughout the test day, whereas plasma and brain oxytocin concentrations were measured post-testing only. The results obtained indicated increased CORT response after the OFT in CC-treated dams (P ≤ 0.05). CC-treated dams also exhibited altered FST behaviour (P ≤ 0.05), suggesting abnormal stress responsiveness. Peripheral, but not central, oxytocin levels were increased by cocaine treatment (P ≤ 0.05). Peripheral oxytocin and CORT increased after the FST, regardless of treatment condition (P ≤ 0.05). Changes in stress-responsiveness, both behaviourally and hormonally, may underlie some deficits in maternal behaviour; thus, a clearer understanding of the effect of CC on the stress response system may potentially lead to treatment interventions that could be relevant to clinical populations. Additionally, these results indicate that CC treatment can have long-lasting effects on peripheral oxytocin regulation in rats, similar to changes observed in persistent social behaviour and stress-response deficits in clinical populations.


Subject(s)
Cocaine/pharmacology , Corticosterone/blood , Oxytocin/metabolism , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects/metabolism , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects/psychology , Stress, Psychological/metabolism , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Animals , Body Weight/drug effects , Brain/drug effects , Brain/metabolism , Disease Models, Animal , Female , Immobility Response, Tonic/drug effects , Male , Motor Activity/drug effects , Oxytocin/blood , Pregnancy , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Stress, Psychological/blood
7.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 15(4): 802-8, 2008 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18792507

ABSTRACT

Everyday tasks often require us to keep track of multiple objects in dynamic scenes. Past studies show that tracking becomes more difficult as objects move faster. In the present study, we show that this trade-off may not be due to increased speed itself but may, instead, be due to the increased crowding that usually accompanies increases in speed. Here, we isolate changes in speed from variations in crowding, by projecting a tracking display either onto a small area at the center of a hemispheric projection dome or onto the entire dome. Use of the larger display increased retinal image size and object speed by a factor of 4 but did not increase interobject crowding. Results showed that tracking accuracy was equally good in the large-display condition, even when the objects traveled far into the visual periphery. Accuracy was also not reduced when we tested object speeds that limited performance in the small-display condition. These results, along with a reinterpretation of past studies, suggest that we might be able to track multiple moving objects as fast as we can a single moving object, once the effect of object crowding is eliminated.


Subject(s)
Attention , Field Dependence-Independence , Motion Perception , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reaction Time , Size Perception , Acceleration , Crowding , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Orientation
8.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 27(4): 969-84, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11518157

ABSTRACT

Perception of the 2nd of 2 targets (T1 and T2) is impaired if the lag between them is short (0-500 ms). The authors used this attentional blink (AB) to index attentional requirements in detection and identification tasks, with or without backward masking of T2, in 2 stimulus domains (line orientation, coherent motion). With masking, the AB occurred because T2 was masked during the attentional dwell time created by T1 processing (Experiments 1, 2, and 3). Without masking, an AB occurred only in identification because during the attentional dwell time, T2 decayed to a level that could support simple detection but not complex identification. However, an AB occurred also in detection if T2 was sufficiently degraded (Experiment 4). The authors drew 2 major conclusions: (a) Attention is required in both identification and detection, and (b) 2 factors contribute to the AB, masking of T2 while attention is focused on T1 and decay of the T2 trace while unattended.


Subject(s)
Attention , Blinking , Signal Detection, Psychological , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Random Allocation , Reaction Time
9.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 55(2): 104-10, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11433781

ABSTRACT

An influential theory of visually guided action proposes that (a) conscious perception of target displacement disrupts on-line action and (b) small target perturbations are inconsequential, provided the participant is unaware of them. This study examined these claims in a study of rapid aiming movements to targets. Novel features included on-line verbal reports of target displacement, and the factorial combination of small versus large displacements occurring near peak saccadic velocity or 100 ms later. Although awareness of target displacement had no effect on movement kinematics, even small target displacements near peak saccadic velocity affected kinematic measures. These results support both a strong view of visual stream separation in the on-line control of action and richer spatial coding by unconscious processes than has previously been acknowledged.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Electrooculography , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Visual Acuity/physiology
10.
Perception ; 30(2): 135-45, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11296495

ABSTRACT

When two visual patterns are presented in rapid succession, their contours may be combined into a single unified percept. This temporal integration is known to be influenced by such low-level visual factors as stimulus intensity, contour proximity, and stimulus duration. In this study we asked whether temporal integration is modulated by an attentional-blink procedure. The results from a localisation task in experiment 1 and a detection task in experiment 2 pointed to two separate effects. First, greater attentional availability increased the accuracy of spatial localisation. Second, it increased the duration over which successive stimuli could be integrated. These results imply that theories of visible persistence and visual masking must account for attentional influences in addition to lower-level effects. They also have practical implications for use of the temporal-integration task in the assessment of group and individual differences.


Subject(s)
Afterimage/physiology , Attention/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Humans , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Psychophysics
11.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 27(6): 1287-302, 2001 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11766925

ABSTRACT

Both the sudden appearance of an object and sudden changes in existing object features influence priority in visual search. However, direct comparisons of these influences have not been made under controlled conditions. In 5 visual search experiments, new object onsets were compared directly with changes in the luminance of old objects. Factors included the luminance contrast of items against the background, the magnitude of luminance change, and the probability that these changes were associated with the target item. New objects were consistently more effective in guiding search, such that a new item with very low luminance contrast was equivalent to an old item undergoing a large change in luminance. An important exception was an old item changing in contrast and polarity, which was as effective as the appearance of a new object. This indicates that search priority is biased toward object rather than situational changes.


Subject(s)
Attention , Light , Humans , Reaction Time , Visual Perception
12.
Dev Psychol ; 36(6): 731-40, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11081697

ABSTRACT

Visual search for compound patterns was examined in observers aged 6, 8, 10, and 22 years. The main question was whether age-related improvement in search rate (response time slope over number of items) was different for patterns defined by short- versus long-range spatial relations. Perceptual access to each type of relation was varied by using elements of same contrast (easy to access) or mixed contrast (hard to access). The results showed large improvements with age in search rate for long-range targets; search rate for short-range targets was fairly constant across age. This pattern held regardless of whether perceptual access to a target was easy or hard, supporting the hypothesis that different processes are involved in perceptual grouping at these two levels. The results also point to important links between ontogenic and microgenic change in perception (H. Werner, 1948, 1957).


Subject(s)
Child Development , Contrast Sensitivity , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Age Factors , Attention , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Space Perception
13.
Percept Psychophys ; 62(1): 218-25, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10703268

ABSTRACT

Simple response times (RTs) are known to be slower to the offset of a visual stimulus than to its onset. This is called the onset advantage. In the first of four experiments, we discovered that a spurious onset advantage can be produced by the long persistence of P31 phosphor. In the remaining three experiments, we found that offset RTs were slower only when they were made in a context in which responses to the abrupt onset of some stimuli had to be suppressed. We tested this hypothesis of response suppression in two ways: (1) by mixing regular onset trials with other trials on which a response to an onset had to be suppressed, and (2) by ramping the emergence of "offset" stimuli over time, so that offsets were the only abrupt events in the display. In both cases, we found that the onset advantage depended critically on whether the responses were made in a context of response suppression. We conclude that the onset advantage is mediated not by sensory factors such as visible persistence, but by response programming factors that are strongly affected by contextual events.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Reaction Time
14.
Psychol Sci ; 11(5): 386-93, 2000 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11228909

ABSTRACT

Do visual field effects point to differences in cortical representation, or do they reflect differences in the way these representations are used by other brain regions? This study explored three attributes of visual search that provide strong evidence in favor of differences in use. Competition refers to the finding that visual field differences in search efficiency are larger in whole- than in half-field displays (both left-right and upper-lower half-fields). Task specialization refers to the finding that some tasks favor one hemisphere whereas other tasks favor the other hemisphere, even though the same stimulus displays are used in both tasks. Anatomical alignment refers to the finding that competition effects are altered if the quadrants of the visual display are not aligned with the cortical quadrants of the observer. We propose that visual field specialization in search is the result of a competition involving limited access to cortical visual representations by the extended neural networks of attention.


Subject(s)
Nerve Net/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Attention , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Task Performance and Analysis
15.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 129(4): 481-507, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11142864

ABSTRACT

Advances in neuroscience implicate reentrant signaling as the predominant form of communication between brain areas. This principle was used in a series of masking experiments that defy explanation by feed-forward theories. The masking occurs when a brief display of target plus mask is continued with the mask alone. Two masking processes were found: an early process affected by physical factors such as adapting luminance and a later process affected by attentional factors such as set size. This later process is called masking by object substitution, because it occurs whenever there is a mismatch between the reentrant visual representation and the ongoing lower level activity. Iterative reentrant processing was formalized in a computational model that provides an excellent fit to the data. The model provides a more comprehensive account of all forms of visual masking than do the long-held feed-forward views based on inhibitory contour interactions.


Subject(s)
Consciousness , Models, Psychological , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Processes , Neurons , Visual Perception
16.
J Gen Psychol ; 126(4): 355-72, 1999 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10555865

ABSTRACT

A new illusion of perceived duration associated with focused spatial attention is reported. Brief flashes in attended locations were perceived to last longer than the same flashes in unattended locations. That illusion was shown to be completely independent of another illusion concerning the perceived onset of a flash, ruling out the possibility that the effect on perceived duration is derivative of a comparison between perceived onset and offset. The illusion also occurred when the event duration was composed of a temporal gap rather than a brief flash, ruling out low-level visible persistence as a basis for the illusion. Taken together, the results point to cortical connections from higher brain centers' both speeding and prolonging the visual signals occurring in lower sensory regions. Those temporal consequences could easily subserve many of the perceptual benefits ascribed to attention for spatial and intensive properties.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Time Factors
17.
Percept Psychophys ; 61(7): 1436-48, 1999 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10572470

ABSTRACT

When two targets are displayed in rapid visual sequence and masked by trailing patterns, identification accuracy is nearly perfect for the first target but follows a U-shaped pattern over temporal lag for the second target. Three experiments examined the role of visual masking in this attentional blink. Experiment 1 compared integration and interruption masks for both targets. Although either mask was effective in producing the blink when applied to the first target, only the interruption mask was effective when applied to the second target. Experiment 2 showed that integration masking of the second target was ineffective over a wide range of accuracy levels. Combining the two forms of masking in Experiment 3 confirmed the dissociation: A combined mask and only a main effect on accuracy for the first target, whereas it produced a qualitatively different pattern over temporal lag for the second target. These results suggest that representations of the target are substituted in consciousness by that of the interruption mask when visual attention is preoccupied.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Blinking/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
18.
Vision Res ; 38(15-16): 2489-505, 1998 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9798011

ABSTRACT

We show that early vision can use monocular cues to rapidly complete partially-occluded objects. Visual search for easily-detected fragments becomes difficult when the completed shape is similar to others in the display; conversely, search for fragments that are difficult to detect becomes easy when the completed shape is distinctive. Results indicate that completion occurs via the occlusion-triggered removal of occlusion edges and linking of associated regions. We fail to find evidence for a visible filling-in of contours or surfaces, but do find evidence for a 'functional' filling-in that prevents the constituent fragments from being rapidly accessed. As such, it is only the completed structures--and not the fragments themselves--that serve as the basis for rapid recognition.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Humans , Psychophysics , Reaction Time , Visual Fields
19.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 23(4): 980-98, 1997 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9269724

ABSTRACT

Observers made speeded discriminations of whole, occluded, and mosaic shapes. Shape matching times increased with the amount of occluded shape (Experiment 1), as did the time to merely discriminate 2 shapes (Experiments 2-4). By contrast, the time to judge the shape of the visible portion decreased with larger occluded regions (Experiments 5-7). Experiments 3 and 6 used motion parallax to show that different perceptual operations are involved in discriminating occluded versus mosaic shapes. Experiments 4 and 7 showed that shape completion was unaffected by spatial attention. Results suggest that shape completion is a rapid and obligatory aspect of perception. However, they also show that the time course of completion varies with the size of the hidden region.


Subject(s)
Form Perception , Size Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Motion , Reaction Time
20.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 51(1): 20-35, 1997 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9206322

ABSTRACT

Covert visual orienting was examined over a span of human life ranging from six to 73 years. The observer's task was the speeded discrimination of "X" from "O1" but of primary interest was the effect of a location cue that appeared prior to the target. Both an abrupt stimulus cue and a voluntary information cue were studied using response time (RT) measures. Eye movements were monitored to control for differences in the ability to maintain fixation. Experiment 1 showed that there were very few age differences in stimulus-cued orienting. In contrast, there were important differences when orienting was intentional. In comparison with young adults, children were less able to sustain orienting over time, and senior adults required more time to use the cue. Experiment 2 tested the relation between stimulus and information cues when they both occurred prior to a given target. All age groups were able to use information cues in the presence of conflicting stimulus cues, but young adults were better able to do so than either children or senior adults. These results are interpreted as support for the view that separate mechanisms underlie stimulus-based versus information-based spatial orienting.


Subject(s)
Psychomotor Performance , Adolescent , Attention , Child , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Spatial Behavior
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL