Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 51
Filter
1.
Asian Pac Isl Nurs J ; 8: e60104, 2024 Sep 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39298259

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Music has frequently been used in movement exercises to enhance health benefits. However, scientific evidence regarding the application of music to Tai Chi practice is limited. OBJECTIVE: This scoping review aims to understand how music has been used in Tai Chi practice and whether music could be applied to Tai Chi practice to help optimize its benefits. METHODS: PubMed, CINAHL, CNKI, and Weipu databases were searched. We included studies that compare Tai Chi practice experience or health outcomes between individuals practicing Tai Chi with music and those practicing Tai Chi without music. Studies published through September 2022 were identified. Two researchers (YD and YH) independently performed study selection and data extraction. Thematic analysis was used to summarize and categorize the findings of the included studies. RESULTS: Seven studies were included in this review. All 7 included studies are experimental studies. Practicing Tai Chi with music might lead to positive perceptions of Tai Chi practice (eg, motivation, concentration, enjoyment, compliance, and performance) and higher evaluations of Tai Chi instructional quality, especially for Tai Chi beginners. The effects of incorporating music into Tai Chi practice on health outcomes are inconclusive due to the heterogeneities of the sample size, and the intervention components, lengths, and frequencies of the included studies. CONCLUSIONS: Applying music to Tai Chi practice may result in positive Tai Chi practice experience and adherence, particularly for beginners, which could help improve the dissemination and implementation of Tai Chi interventions for public health. However, whether applying music to Tai Chi practice leads to synergetic effects on health outcomes needs further investigation.

2.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 67(9): 3217-3231, 2024 Sep 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39116317

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Listening effort is a broad construct, and there is no consensus on how to subdivide listening effort into dimensions. This project focuses on the subjective experience of effortful listening and tests if cognitive workload, mental fatigue, and mood are interrelated dimensions. METHOD: Two online studies tested young adults (n = 74 and n = 195) and measured subjective workload, fatigue (subscales of fatigue and energy), and mood (subscales of positive and negative mood) before and after a challenging listening task. In the listening effort task, participants responded to intermittent 1-kHz target tones in continuous white noise for approximately 12 min. RESULTS: Correlations and principal component analysis showed that fatigue and mood were distinct but interrelated constructs that weakly correlated with workload. Effortful listening provoked increased fatigue and decreased energy and positive mood yet did not influence negative mood or workload. CONCLUSIONS: The findings suggest that self-reported listening effort has multiple dimensions that can have different responses to the same effortful listening episode. The results can help guide evidence-based development of clinical listening effort tests and may reveal mechanisms for how listening effort relates to quality of life in those with hearing impairment. SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL: https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.26418976.


Subject(s)
Affect , Psychomotor Performance , Self Report , Humans , Female , Male , Young Adult , Affect/physiology , Adult , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Mental Fatigue/psychology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Fatigue/psychology , Fatigue/physiopathology , Adolescent
4.
Mil Med ; 189(1-2): e306-e312, 2024 Jan 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37715688

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Hearing protection devices (HPDs) are standard personal protective equipment in military settings, but many service members may choose to not use HPDs because they impair spatial hearing and situation awareness. In an effort to reduce barriers to compliance by improving situation awareness while wearing HPDs, this study investigated whether brief training could counteract spatial hearing deficits when wearing HPDs. Participant's ability to correctly apply the HPDs across days was also examined. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Young adults were randomly assigned to one of two groups: training or control (n = 25/group). Participants in each group performed a spatial hearing task while wearing HPDs and in an open ear condition without HPDs. Individual targets were battlefield sounds or white noise presented from a speaker array that surrounded the participant in the horizontal plane. After presentation of each target sound, the participant then controlled a white noise "auditory pointer," which they moved to the perceived location of the target. The two primary measures were the percent of trials with very large errors (> 45°), which were usually due to confusing front and back locations, and absolute localization, which is the difference between the pointer location and the true sound location. Both groups were tested on Days 1 (baseline) and 5 (post-test). On Days 2 to 4, the training group wore HPDs while receiving auditory and visual feedback after each trial. RESULTS: Across all participants on Day 1, wearing HPDs increased the frequency of very large errors by about 3× and impaired localization by about 40%, relative to the open ear condition. When comparing performance at baseline (Day 1) and post-training Day 5, the training group with HPDs had significant reductions in very large errors and improved absolute localization (P values < .001). The training group also had significant improvements from Days 1 to 5 in the open ear condition. When the control group wore HPDs, there were also significant improvements from Days 1 to 5 (fewer very large errors and better localization), with smaller effect sizes vs. the training group. Controls did not have significant improvement in the open ear condition, but had similar trends. Most participants consistently applied the HPDs, but a subset of ∼20% frequently failed to achieve the criterion attenuation of 15 dB (over 0.25-4.0 kHz) in both ears. CONCLUSIONS: These findings show that simple, relatively brief practice and training can substantially reduce HPD impairments on spatial hearing and situation awareness. The gains from training and practice can inform the development of relatively simple, brief methods to reduce HPD spatial hearing impairments, potentially leading to increased HPD compliance. Longitudinal data show that a subset of participants would not have received the full benefit of hearing protection because of improper application of the HPDs.


Subject(s)
Awareness , Hearing Loss, Noise-Induced , Young Adult , Humans , Ear Protective Devices , Hearing , Hearing Tests , Hearing Loss, Noise-Induced/prevention & control
5.
J Commun Disord ; 105: 106353, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37331327

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The variable and intermittent nature of stuttering makes it difficult to consistently elicit a sufficient number of stuttered trials for longitudinal experimental research. This study tests the efficacy of using non-word pairs that phonetically mimic English words with no associated meaning, to reliably elicit balanced numbers of stuttering and fluent trials over multiple sessions. The study also evaluated the effect of non-word length on stuttering frequency, the consistency of stuttering frequency across sessions, and potential carry-over effects of increased stuttering frequency in the experimental task to conversational and reading speech after the task. METHODS: Twelve adults who stutter completed multiple sessions (mean of 4.8 sessions) where they were video-recorded during pre-task reading and conversation, followed by an experimental task where they read 400 non-word pairs randomized for each session, and then a post-task reading and conversation sample. RESULTS: On average, across sessions and participants, non-word pairs consistently yielded a balanced distribution of fluent (60.7%) and stuttered (39.3%) trials over five sessions. Non-word length had a positive effect on stuttering frequency. No carryover effects from experimental to post-task conversation and reading were found. CONCLUSIONS: Non-word pairs effectively and consistently elicited balanced proportions of stuttered and fluent trials. This approach can be used to gather longitudinal data to better understand the neurophysiological and behavioral correlates of stuttering.


Subject(s)
Speech , Stuttering , Adult , Humans , Speech/physiology , Stuttering/therapy , Language , Communication , Reading , Speech Production Measurement
6.
Front Neurosci ; 16: 912798, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35979337

ABSTRACT

A key goal of cognitive neuroscience is to better understand how dynamic brain activity relates to behavior. Such dynamics, in terms of spatial and temporal patterns of brain activity, are directly measured with neurophysiological methods such as EEG, but can also be indirectly expressed by the body. Autonomic nervous system activity is the best-known example, but, muscles in the eyes and face can also index brain activity. Mostly parallel lines of artificial intelligence research show that EEG and facial muscles both encode information about emotion, pain, attention, and social interactions, among other topics. In this study, we examined adults who stutter (AWS) to understand the relations between dynamic brain and facial muscle activity and predictions about future behavior (fluent or stuttered speech). AWS can provide insight into brain-behavior dynamics because they naturally fluctuate between episodes of fluent and stuttered speech behavior. We focused on the period when speech preparation occurs, and used EEG and facial muscle activity measured from video to predict whether the upcoming speech would be fluent or stuttered. An explainable self-supervised multimodal architecture learned the temporal dynamics of both EEG and facial muscle movements during speech preparation in AWS, and predicted fluent or stuttered speech at 80.8% accuracy (chance=50%). Specific EEG and facial muscle signals distinguished fluent and stuttered trials, and systematically varied from early to late speech preparation time periods. The self-supervised architecture successfully identified multimodal activity that predicted upcoming behavior on a trial-by-trial basis. This approach could be applied to understanding the neural mechanisms driving variable behavior and symptoms in a wide range of neurological and psychiatric disorders. The combination of direct measures of neural activity and simple video data may be applied to developing technologies that estimate brain state from subtle bodily signals.

7.
Neuropsychologia ; 160: 107979, 2021 09 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34339719

ABSTRACT

Cognitive control allows humans to process relevant sensory information while minimizing distractions from irrelevant stimuli. The neural basis of cognitive control is known to involve frontal regions of the brain such as the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), but the temporal dynamics of larger scale networks is unclear. Here we used EEG with source localization to identify how the neural oscillations localized to the mPFC and ACC coordinate with parietal, sensory, and motor areas during spatial cognitive control. Theta coherence (3-8 Hz) between the mPFC and ACC increased with task difficulty and predicted individual differences in reaction time. Individual differences in accuracy were predicted by earlier activation of ACC-motor coherence, highlighting the relationship between processing speed and task performance. Our results provide evidence that successful cognitive control requires dynamic coordination between a widespread network of brain regions. Long range theta coherence may be a key mechanism for efficient cognitive control across the neocortex.


Subject(s)
Gyrus Cinguli , Motor Cortex , Brain , Cognition , Electroencephalography , Humans , Reaction Time , Theta Rhythm
8.
Psychophysiology ; 58(10): e13903, 2021 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34342887

ABSTRACT

Selection and effort are central to attention, yet it is unclear whether they draw on a common pool of cognitive resources, and if so, whether there are differences for early versus later stages of cognitive processing. This study assessed effort by quantifying the vigilance decrement, and spatial processing at early and later stages as a function of time-on-task. Participants performed an auditory spatial attention task, with occasional "catch" trials requiring no response. Psychophysiological measures included bilateral cerebral blood flow (transcranial Doppler), pupil dilation, and blink rate. The shape of attention gradients using reaction time indexed early processing, and did not significantly vary over time. Later stimulus-response conflict was comparable over time, except for a reduction to left hemispace stimuli. Target and catch trial accuracy decreased with time, with a more abrupt decrease for catch versus target trials. Diffusion decision modeling found progressive decreases in information accumulation rate and non-decision time, and the adoption of more liberal response criteria. Cerebral blood flow increased from baseline and then decreased over time, particularly in the left hemisphere. Blink rate steadily increased over time, while pupil dilation increased only at the beginning and then returned towards baseline. The findings suggest dissociations between resources for selectivity and effort. Measures of high subjective effort and temporal declines in catch trial accuracy and cerebral blood flow velocity suggest a standard vigilance decrement was evident in parallel with preserved selection. Different attentional systems and classes of computations that may account for dissociations between selectivity versus effort are discussed.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Ultrasonography, Doppler, Transcranial , Young Adult
9.
Proc AAAI Conf Artif Intell ; 35(1): 818-826, 2021 May 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34221694

ABSTRACT

Human behavior is the confluence of output from voluntary and involuntary motor systems. The neural activities that mediate behavior, from individual cells to distributed networks, are in a state of constant flux. Artificial intelligence (AI) research over the past decade shows that behavior, in the form of facial muscle activity, can reveal information about fleeting voluntary and involuntary motor system activity related to emotion, pain, and deception. However, the AI algorithms often lack an explanation for their decisions, and learning meaningful representations requires large datasets labeled by a subject-matter expert. Motivated by the success of using facial muscle movements to classify brain states and the importance of learning from small amounts of data, we propose an explainable self-supervised representation-learning paradigm that learns meaningful temporal facial muscle movement patterns from limited samples. We validate our methodology by carrying out comprehensive empirical study to predict future speech behavior in a real-world dataset of adults who stutter (AWS). Our explainability study found facial muscle movements around the eyes (p <0.×001) and lips (p <0.001) differ significantly before producing fluent vs. disfluent speech. Evaluations using the AWS dataset demonstrates that the proposed self-supervised approach achieves a minimum of 2.51% accuracy improvement over fully-supervised approaches.

10.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 47(4): 616-633, 2021 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33983793

ABSTRACT

The cognitive penetration literature suggests that top-down knowledge influences perception, but whether such influences exist is controversial. We tested for top-down influences on perception by loading short-term memory with digits and then had participants make perceptual judgments to index spatial hearing. Memory of spatial number codes were predicted to bias spatial judgments to the left for small digits and rightward for larger digits. Participants encoded one or more digits and then made spatial judgments in either spatial hearing or dichotic listening tasks. Results across five experiments supported the predicted spatial biases. Digits had to be deliberately encoded, and at least two were needed to be memorized before a small number left-right bias in dichotic listening was evident. In dichotic listening, smaller numbers in memory also promoted more intrusions, and a mix of small and large numbers enhanced the right ear advantage. Results suggest that long-term knowledge about number magnitude imparts a top-down bias on auditory spatial perception. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Dichotic Listening Tests , Memory, Short-Term , Attention , Auditory Perception , Bias , Functional Laterality , Humans
11.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 39(2): 252-268, 2021 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33001487

ABSTRACT

The authors used an analysis of individual differences to examine the role of executive control in strategic encoding and retrieval in verbal recall. Participants enrolled in the Louisiana Healthy Aging Study completed measures of working memory (WM), cognitive status, vocabulary, and free recall of words. Indices of clustering in free recall were calculated to permit inferences on strategic encoding and retrieval processes. We hypothesized that WM would be more strongly associated with strategic encoding and retrieval metrics than vocabulary based on the assumption that successful remembering requires executive control in WM. Regression analyses, together with a variance portioning procedure, confirmed that WM had comparable levels of unique and shared variance with the strategic encoding and retrieval metrics, and both exceeded vocabulary. Theoretical and clinical implications of these data are considered, with the suggestion of future research in lifespan samples as opposed to exclusively young adult or older adult samples.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Mental Recall , Aged , Executive Function , Humans , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Vocabulary , Young Adult
12.
Front Psychol ; 11: 1755, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33101096

ABSTRACT

Social engineering cyberattacks are a major threat because they often prelude sophisticated and devastating cyberattacks. Social engineering cyberattacks are a kind of psychological attack that exploits weaknesses in human cognitive functions. Adequate defense against social engineering cyberattacks requires a deeper understanding of what aspects of human cognition are exploited by these cyberattacks, why humans are susceptible to these cyberattacks, and how we can minimize or at least mitigate their damage. These questions have received some amount of attention, but the state-of-the-art understanding is superficial and scattered in the literature. In this paper, we review human cognition through the lens of social engineering cyberattacks. Then, we propose an extended framework of human cognitive functions to accommodate social engineering cyberattacks. We cast existing studies on various aspects of social engineering cyberattacks into the extended framework, while drawing a number of insights that represent the current understanding and shed light on future research directions. The extended framework might inspire future research endeavor toward a new sub-field that can be called Cybersecurity Cognitive Psychology, which tailors or adapts principles of Cognitive Psychology to the cybersecurity domain while embracing new notions and concepts that are unique to the cybersecurity domain.

13.
Neurobiol Aging ; 88: 128-136, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32035848

ABSTRACT

The auditory system allows us to monitor background environmental sound patterns and recognize deviations that may indicate opportunities or threats. The mismatch negativity and P3a potentials have generators in the auditory and inferior frontal cortex and index expected sound patterns (standards) and any aberrations (deviants). The mismatch negativity and P3a waveforms show increased positivity for consecutive standards and deviants preceded by more standards. We hypothesized attenuated repetition effects in older participants, potentially because of differences in prefrontal functions. Young (23 ± 5 years) and older (75 ± 5 years) adults were tested in 2 oddball paradigms with pitch or location deviants. Significant repetition effects were observed in the young standard and deviant waveforms at multiple time windows. Except the earliest time window (30-100 ms), repetition effects were absent in the older group. Repetition effects were significant at frontal but not temporal lobe sites and did not differ among pitch and location deviants. However, P3a repetition was evident in both ages. Findings suggest age differences in the dynamic updating of sensory memory for background sound patterns.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Aging/psychology , Auditory Cortex/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Brain/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Memory/physiology , Sensation/physiology , Adult , Aged , Humans , Repetition Priming/physiology , Sound , Young Adult
14.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 1496, 2020 01 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32001755

ABSTRACT

Whenever we move, speak, or play musical instruments, our actions generate auditory sensory input. The sensory consequences of our actions are thought to be predicted via sensorimotor integration, which involves anatomical and functional links between auditory and motor brain regions. The physiological connections are relatively well established, but less is known about how sensorimotor integration affects auditory perception. The sensory attenuation hypothesis suggests that the perceived loudness of self-generated sounds is attenuated to help distinguish self-generated sounds from ambient sounds. Sensory attenuation would work for louder ambient sounds, but could lead to less accurate perception if the ambient sounds were quieter. We hypothesize that a key function of sensorimotor integration is the facilitated processing of self-generated sounds, leading to more accurate perception under most conditions. The sensory attenuation hypothesis predicts better performance for higher but not lower intensity comparisons, whereas sensory facilitation predicts improved perception regardless of comparison sound intensity. A series of experiments tested these hypotheses, with results supporting the enhancement hypothesis. Overall, people were more accurate at comparing the loudness of two sounds when making one of the sounds themselves. We propose that the brain selectively modulates the perception of self-generated sounds to enhance representations of action consequences.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Sensorimotor Cortex/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Auditory Cortex/physiology , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Female , Humans , Loudness Perception/physiology , Male , Models, Neurological , Models, Psychological , Sensory Gating/physiology , Young Adult
15.
Cognition ; 194: 104058, 2020 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31494432

ABSTRACT

Auditory spatial attention faces the conflicting demands of focusing on the current task while also rapidly shifting location to unexpected stimuli. We examined the interplay of sustained focus and intermittent shifts in an auditory spatial attention task. Most trials required a choice response from a standard location in virtual space (L-R: -90°, 0°, +90°), but occasionally the location shifted between 45°-180°. Reaction time curves for angular shifts had a quadratic shape, with slowing for small shifts but faster reaction times for larger shifts. The reaction time curves were maintained at faster stimulus rates and usually scaled to fit the range of stimulus locations. However, focus on the right had an attenuated curve, and did not scale to the range of locations. The findings suggest two mechanisms: a top-down bias centered on standard locations that decreases with distance, and a bottom-up bias that under these conditions increases with distance from the standard location.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Sound Localization/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
16.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 81(1): 270-280, 2019 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30338454

ABSTRACT

Attention control is a core element of cognitive aging, but the specific mechanisms that differ with age are unclear. Here we used a novel auditory spatial attention task to evaluate stimulus processing at the level of early attention capture, later response selection, and the lingering effects of attention capture across trials in young and older adults. We found that the shapes of spatial attention capture gradients were remarkably similar in young and older adults, but only the older group had lingering effects of attention capture on the next trial. Response selection for stimulus-response incompatibilities took longer in older subjects, but primarily when attending to the midline location. The results suggest that the likelihood and spatial tuning of attention capture is comparable among groups, but once attention is captured, older subjects take longer to disengage. Age differences in response selection were supported, but may not be a general feature of cognitive aging.


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Aging/physiology , Attention/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Spatial Processing/physiology , Adolescent , Aged , Aging/psychology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Cognitive Aging/physiology , Cognitive Aging/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Random Allocation , Young Adult
17.
Neuropsychologia ; 111: 216-228, 2018 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29410123

ABSTRACT

Cognitive control of attention in conflict situations is a basic skill that is vital for goal-oriented behavior. Behavioral evidence shows that conflict control occurs over successive trials as well as longer time scales of trial blocks, but the relation among time scales as well as their neural mechanisms are unclear. This study used measures of behavior, EEG, and a simple quantitative model to test the hypothesis that conflict control at the block level is not exclusively driven by control adjustments over successive trials. Young adults performed an auditory Simon task, and the base rate of compatible vs. incompatible trials was manipulated in separate blocks (25, 50, 75% compatible). EEG data were analyzed using independent component analysis (ICA) to define cortical mechanisms of any base rate and trial-by-trial sequence effects. Reaction time measures had both sequence and base rate effects. Two fronto-medial ICA components indexed sequence and base rate effects, with specific profiles for evoked potentials and oscillations in the theta and alpha frequency bands. Predictive modeling showed that sequence effects accounted for a minority of the variance on behavioral and ICA measures (all < 36%). The results strongly suggest that the base rate manipulation affected behavior and many neural measures beyond the influence of sequence effects.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Brain/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Theta Rhythm/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Time Factors , Young Adult
18.
J Cogn ; 2(1): 2, 2018 Jan 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31517222

ABSTRACT

Researchers have often utilized the classic Stroop task as a measure of selective attention processes. While it is largely agreed upon that semantic interference plays a role in the classic task, the role of attentional processes is less clear. The picture is further muddied when variations on the classic task are used. For example, the cross-modal Stroop task, in which one names the color of visual items while ignoring distracting auditory color words, typically leads to smaller sized interference effects and little or no facilitation when compared to the classic task. Furthermore, relationship(s) with working memory capacity have only been found in the classic version. We examined whether these differences are due to a methodological factor; namely, spatial integration versus separation of the target and distractor locations. We conducted four experiments manipulating the location of auditory distractors within the cross-modal Stroop task, and found that the location of the distractors had little to no impact on the size of the effect. This lack of an effect of location implies that the mechanisms contributing to the cross-modal Stroop effect are not the same as those of the classic Stroop effect, and that the role of spatial attention in cross-modal Stroop is limited. The implications of a unique role for auditory distractors is considered as well, and supports the conclusion that interference in the cross-modal Stroop effect is the result of a combination of semantic interference and modality-specific interference.

19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34720566

ABSTRACT

Normal human speech requires precise coordination between motor planning and sensory processing. Speech disfluencies are common when children learn to talk, but usually abate with time. About 5% of children experience stuttering. For most, this resolves within a year. However, for approximately 1% of the world population, stuttering continues into adulthood, which is termed 'persistent developmental stuttering'. Most stuttering events occur at the beginning of an utterance. So, in principle, brain activity before speaking should differ between fluent and stuttered speech. Here we present a method for classifying brain network states associated with fluent vs. stuttered speech on a single trial basis. Brain activity was recorded with EEG before people who stutter read aloud pseudo-word pairs. Offline independent component analysis (ICA) was used to identify the independent neural sources that underlie speech preparation. A time window selection algorithm extracted spectral power and coherence data from salient windows specific to each neural source. A stepwise linear discriminant analysis (sLDA) algorithm predicted fluent vs. stuttered speech for 81% of trials in two subjects. These results support the feasibility of developing a brain-computer interface (BCI) system to detect stuttering before it occurs, with potential for therapeutic application.

20.
Front Psychol ; 8: 2028, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29218024

ABSTRACT

Short-term memory load can impair attentional control, but prior work shows that the extent of the effect ranges from being very general to very specific. One factor for the mixed results may be reliance on point estimates of memory load effects on attention. Here we used auditory attention gradients as an analog measure to map-out the impact of short-term memory load over space. Verbal or spatial information was maintained during an auditory spatial attention task and compared to no-load. Stimuli were presented from five virtual locations in the frontal azimuth plane, and subjects focused on the midline. Reaction times progressively increased for lateral stimuli, indicating an attention gradient. Spatial load further slowed responses at lateral locations, particularly in the left hemispace, but had little effect at midline. Verbal memory load had no (Experiment 1), or a minimal (Experiment 2) influence on reaction times. Spatial and verbal load increased switch costs between memory encoding and attention tasks relative to the no load condition. The findings show that short-term memory influences the distribution of auditory attention over space; and that the specific pattern depends on the type of information in short-term memory.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL