Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 6 de 6
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Vis ; 24(6): 6, 2024 Jun 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38843389

RESUMO

Infant primates see poorly, and most perceptual functions mature steadily beyond early infancy. Behavioral studies on human and macaque infants show that global form perception, as measured by the ability to integrate contour information into a coherent percept, improves dramatically throughout the first several years after birth. However, it is unknown when sensitivity to curvature and shape emerges in early life or how it develops. We studied the development of shape sensitivity in 18 macaques, aged 2 months to 10 years. Using radial frequency stimuli, circular targets whose radii are modulated sinusoidally, we tested monkeys' ability to radial frequency stimuli from circles as a function of the depth and frequency of sinusoidal modulation. We implemented a new four-choice oddity task and compared the resulting data with that from a traditional two-alternative forced choice task. We found that radial frequency pattern perception was measurable at the youngest age tested (2 months). Behavioral performance at all radial frequencies improved with age. Performance was better for higher radial frequencies, suggesting the developing visual system prioritizes processing of fine visual details that are ecologically relevant. By using two complementary methods, we were able to capture a comprehensive developmental trajectory for shape perception.


Assuntos
Percepção de Forma , Macaca mulatta , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Estimulação Luminosa , Animais , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Masculino , Feminino
2.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Apr 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38463955

RESUMO

We studied visual development in macaque monkeys using texture stimuli, matched in local spectral content but varying in "naturalistic" structure. In adult monkeys, naturalistic textures preferentially drive neurons in areas V2 and V4, but not V1. We paired behavioral measurements of naturalness sensitivity with separately-obtained neuronal population recordings from neurons in areas V1, V2, V4, and inferotemporal cortex (IT). We made behavioral measurements from 16 weeks of age and physiological measurements as early as 20 weeks, and continued through 56 weeks. Behavioral sensitivity reached half of maximum at roughly 25 weeks of age. Neural sensitivities remained stable from the earliest ages tested. As in adults, neural sensitivity to naturalistic structure increased from V1 to V2 to V4. While sensitivities in V2 and IT were similar, the dimensionality of the IT representation was more similar to V4's than to V2's.

3.
J Vis ; 23(2): 4, 2023 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36745452

RESUMO

Natural images contain information at multiple spatial scales. Though we understand how early visual mechanisms split multiscale images into distinct spatial frequency channels, we do not know how the outputs of these channels are processed further by mid-level visual mechanisms. We have recently developed a texture discrimination task that uses synthetic, multi-scale, "naturalistic" textures to isolate these mid-level mechanisms. Here, we use three experimental manipulations (image blur, image rescaling, and eccentric viewing) to show that perceptual sensitivity to naturalistic structure is strongly dependent on features at high object spatial frequencies (measured in cycles/image). As a result, sensitivity depends on a texture acuity limit, a property of the visual system that sets the highest retinal spatial frequency (measured in cycles/degree) at which observers can detect naturalistic features. Analysis of the texture images using a model observer analysis shows that naturalistic image features at high object spatial frequencies carry more task-relevant information than those at low object spatial frequencies. That is, the dependence of sensitivity on high object spatial frequencies is a property of the texture images, rather than a property of the visual system. Accordingly, we find human observers' ability to extract naturalistic information (their efficiency) is similar for all object spatial frequencies. We conclude that the mid-level mechanisms that underlie perceptual sensitivity effectively extract information from all image features below the texture acuity limit, regardless of their retinal and object spatial frequency.


Assuntos
Discriminação Psicológica , Retina , Humanos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos
4.
Front Neuroinform ; 7: 33, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24367328

RESUMO

The use of information-based measures to assess changes in conscious state is an increasingly popular topic. Though recent results have seemed to justify the merits of such methods, little has been done to investigate the applicability of such measures to children. For our work, we used the approximate entropy (ApEn), a measure previously shown to correlate with changes in conscious state when applied to the electroencephalogram (EEG), and sought to confirm whether previously reported trends in adult ApEn values across wake and sleep were present in children. Besides validating the prior findings that ApEn decreases from wake to sleep (including wake, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, and non-REM sleep) in adults, we found that previously reported ApEn decreases across vigilance states in adults were also present in children (ApEn trends for both age groups: wake > REM sleep > non-REM sleep). When comparing ApEn values between age groups, adults had significantly larger ApEn values than children during wakefulness. After the application of an 8 Hz high-pass filter to the EEG signal, ApEn values were recalculated. The number of electrodes with significant vigilance state effects dropped from all 109 electrodes with the original 1 Hz filter to 1 electrode with the 8 Hz filter. The number of electrodes with significant age effects dropped from 10 to 4. Our results support the notion that ApEn can reliably distinguish between vigilance states, with low-frequency sleep-related oscillations implicated as the driver of changes between vigilance states. We suggest that the observed differences between adult and child ApEn values during wake may reflect differences in connectivity between age groups, a factor which may be important in the use of EEG to measure consciousness.

5.
Perfusion ; 22(1): 9-14, 2007 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17633129

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The objective of this study is to investigate the hypothesis that a reduced prime extracorporeal circulation (ECC) system and ensuing reduction in patient hemodilution can affect blood product use. METHODS: We performed a prospective, randomized study from a group of 60 consecutive coronary artery bypass graft (CABG) patients, comparing blood product usage and postoperative bleeding in 30 mini bypass systems (n = 30) to 30 conventional systems (n = 30). The patient demographics in terms of patient weight, height, age, preoperative hemoglobin, preoperative hematocrit, BSA, ejection fraction, and NYHA were not statistically significant. RESULTS: Blood product use, including fresh frozen plasma (FFP) and homolgous blood transfusions was tracked through the operating theater and into the intensive care unit. In the mini bypass group, while no homologous blood transfusions were given in the OR, 27% of the patients received at least one unit of homologous blood. In the control group, 43% of the patients received at least one unit of blood in the OR or in the ICU and there was a stastistically-significant 38% reduction in homologous blood product use (p = 0.05). For the patients who received homologous blood, there was also a significant reduction in transfused volume (0.53 +/- 0.90 units blood mini bypass vs 1.3 +/- 1.93 units conventional, p < 0.05). In terms of FFP, there was also a stastistically significant difference between the two groups (0 units transfused in mini bypass group vs 3 patients receiving one unit FFP in the control group, p < 0.001). Cumulative postoperative bleeding during the ICU stay was also evaluated, yielding a significant reduction (365 +/- 495 ml mini bypass vs 825 +/- 975 ml conventional, p < 0.05). CONCLUSION: Mini bypass reduces on-pump hemodilution and, therefore, donor blood usage in routine CABG patients as compared to conventional ECC circuits and can reduce postoperative bleeding as compared to a traditional system. The mini bypass system is safe in routine clinical use and can manage easily the same number of anastomoses as a traditional system and should be considered a favorable alternative to conventional ECC in all revascularization cases.


Assuntos
Transfusão de Sangue/estatística & dados numéricos , Ponte de Artéria Coronária/instrumentação , Circulação Extracorpórea/instrumentação , Idoso , Anastomose Cirúrgica , Circulação Extracorpórea/métodos , Feminino , Hemodiluição , Hemorragia/etiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Complicações Pós-Operatórias
6.
Am J Hum Genet ; 61(2): 282-94, 1997 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9311732

RESUMO

In most patients with isolated unilateral retinoblastoma, tumor development is initiated by somatic inactivation of both alleles of the RB1 gene. However, some of these patients can transmit retinoblastoma predisposition to their offspring. To determine the frequency and nature of constitutional RB1-gene mutations in patients with isolated unilateral retinoblastoma, we analyzed DNA from peripheral blood and from tumor tissue. The analysis of tumors from 54 (71%) of 76 informative patients showed loss of constitutional heterozygosity (LOH) at intragenic loci. Three of 13 uninformative patients had constitutional deletions. For 39 randomly selected tumors, SSCP, hetero-duplex analysis, sequencing, and Southern blot analysis were used to identify mutations. Mutations were detected in 21 (91%) of 23 tumors with LOH. In 6 (38%) of 16 tumors without LOH, one mutation was detected, and in 9 (56%) of the tumors without LOH, both mutations were found. Thus, a total of 45 mutations were identified in tumors of 36 patients. Thirty-nine of the mutations-including 34 small mutations, 2 large structural alterations, and hypermethylation in 3 tumors-were not detected in the corresponding peripheral blood DNA. In 6 (17%) of the 36 patients, a mutation was detected in constitutional DNA, and 1 of these mutations is known to be associated with reduced expressivity. The presence of a constitutional mutation was not associated with an early age at treatment. In 1 patient, somatic mosaicism was demonstrated by molecular analysis of DNA and RNA from peripheral blood. In 2 patients without a detectable mutation in peripheral blood, mosaicism was suggested because 1 of the patients showed multifocal tumors and the other later developed bilateral retinoblastoma. In conclusion, our results emphasize that the manifestation and transmissibility of retinoblastoma depend on the nature of the first mutation, its time in development, and the number and types of cells that are affected.


Assuntos
Neoplasias Oculares/genética , Genes do Retinoblastoma/genética , Retinoblastoma/genética , Análise Atuarial , Fatores Etários , Southern Blotting , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Pré-Escolar , Cromossomos Humanos Par 13 , Ilhas de CpG , Metilação de DNA , Análise Mutacional de DNA , DNA de Neoplasias/sangue , DNA de Neoplasias/genética , Fosfatos de Dinucleosídeos , Neoplasias Oculares/diagnóstico , Feminino , Deleção de Genes , Mutação em Linhagem Germinativa , Heterozigoto , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Mosaicismo , Linhagem , Polimorfismo de Fragmento de Restrição , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Retinoblastoma/diagnóstico , Medição de Risco
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA