Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 25
Filtrar
1.
Child Care Health Dev ; 44(6): 901-907, 2018 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30125386

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: There is growing evidence that specific styles of parent-child interaction benefit child development, particularly child language development. Direct observational techniques help clarify the behaviours and styles within parent-child interactions that may influence child language outcomes; however, these techniques tend to be labour-intensive and costly. We report on the development of a replicable, low-burden mechanism for observing and coding specific maternal linguistic behaviours in a population-based cohort of 2-year-olds. METHODS: The coding scheme was developed as part of a prospective, longitudinal study examining the associations between maternal responsive behaviours and child language outcomes in slow-to-talk toddlers. In the first phase of the study, three coding systems were tested by coding five sample parent-toddler interactions and then comparing them based on (a) the ease of method and thus likely intrarater and interrater reliability and (b) the number of data points. The second phase was to demonstrate how the chosen method could be used in practice with a large at-risk group of toddlers. RESULTS: Of the three coding systems explored, the Observer® XT software was selected for ease of use and because detailed coding of free-play videos could be achieved in close to real time. Intrarater and interrater reliability were established in 251 mother-child free-play videos, producing high intraclass correlation coefficients of 0.95 to 0.99 for the six behaviours. CONCLUSIONS: The study provides evidence that numerous parent-child interactions can be rigourously yet efficiently coded without substantial information loss. The observational mechanism in the current study has been fully developed and is shown to be feasible for research purposes focusing on parent-toddler interactions. However, further testing of the observational mechanism is required to examine whether the same results could be produced if coding was conducted "live" and for shorter duration thereby making it readily useable for clinicians.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/etiologia , Comportamento Materno/psicologia , Relações Mãe-Filho/psicologia , Comportamento Verbal/fisiologia , Técnicas de Observação do Comportamento , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Estudos Prospectivos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Gravação de Videoteipe
2.
Child Care Health Dev ; 44(5): 776-783, 2018 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30043426

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: In a community sample of slow-to-talk toddlers, we aimed to (a) quantify how well maternal responsive behaviors at age 2 years predict language ability at age 4 and (b) examine whether maternal responsive behaviors more accurately predict low language status at age 4 than does expressive vocabulary measured at age 2 years. DESIGN OR METHODS: Prospective community-based longitudinal study. At child age 18 months, 1,138 parents completed a 100-word expressive vocabulary checklist within a population survey; 251 (22.1%) children scored ≤20th percentile and were eligible for the current study. Potential predictors at 2 years were (a) responsive language behaviors derived from videotaped parent-child free-play samples and (b) late-talker status. Outcomes were (a) Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool Second Edition receptive and expressive language standard score at 4 years and (b) low language status (standard score > 1.25 standard deviations below the mean on expressive or receptive language). RESULTS: Two hundred eight (82.9% of 251) participants were retained to age 4. In adjusted linear regression analyses, maternal expansions predicted higher receptive (p < 0.001, partial R2  = 6.5%) and expressive (p < 0.001, partial R2  = 7.7%), whereas labels predicted lower receptive (p = 0.01, partial R2  = 2.8%) and expressive (p = 0.007, partial R2  = 3.5%) language scores at 4. The logistic regression model containing only responsive behaviors achieved "fair" predictive ability of low language status at age 4 (area under curve [AUC] = 0.79), slightly better than the model containing only late-talker status (AUC = 0.74). This improved to "good" predictive ability with inclusion of other known risk factors (AUC = 0.82). CONCLUSION: A combination of short measures of different dimensions, such as parent responsive behaviors, in addition to a child's earlier language skills increases the ability to predict language outcomes at age 4 to a precision that is approaching clinical value. Research to further enhance predictive values should be a priority, enabling health professionals to identify which slow-to-talk toddlers most likely will or will not experience later poorer language.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Comportamento Materno/psicologia , Relações Mãe-Filho/psicologia , Mães/psicologia , Linguagem Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Mães/educação , Estudos Prospectivos , Psicometria
3.
Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch ; 48(3): 153-167, 2017 07 26.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28679000

RESUMO

Purpose: Knowledge of verb development in typically developing bilingual preschoolers may inform clinicians about verb accuracy rates during the 1st 2 years of English instruction. This study aimed to investigate tensed verb accuracy in 2 assessment contexts in 4- and 5-year-old Cantonese-English bilingual preschoolers. Method: The sample included 47 Cantonese-English bilinguals enrolled in English preschools. Half of the children were in their 1st 4 months of English language exposure, and half had completed 1 year and 4 months of exposure to English. Data were obtained from the Test of Early Grammatical Impairment (Rice & Wexler, 2001) and from a narrative generated in English. Results: By the 2nd year of formal exposure to English, children in the present study approximated 33% accuracy of tensed verbs in a formal testing context versus 61% in a narrative context. The use of the English verb BE approximated mastery. Predictors of English third-person singular verb accuracy were task, grade, English expressive vocabulary, and lemma frequency. Conclusions: Verb tense accuracy was low across both groups, but a precocious mastery of BE was observed. The results of the present study suggest that speech-language pathologists may consider, in addition to an elicitation task, evaluating the use of verbs during narratives in bilingual Cantonese-English bilingual children.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Multilinguismo , Pré-Escolar , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Vocabulário
4.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 59(3): 521-32, 2016 06 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27253078

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The aim of this study was to compare narratives generated by 4-year-old and 5-year-old children who were bilingual in English and Cantonese. METHOD: The sample included 47 children (23 who were 4 years old and 24 who were 5 years old) living in Toronto, Ontario, Canada, who spoke both Cantonese and English. The participants spoke and heard predominantly Cantonese in the home. Participants generated a story in English and Cantonese by using a wordless picture book; language order was counterbalanced. Data were transcribed and coded for story grammar, morphosyntactic quality, mean length of utterance in words, and the number of different words. RESULTS: Repeated measures analysis of variance revealed higher story grammar scores in English than in Cantonese, but no other significant main effects of language were observed. Analyses also revealed that older children had higher story grammar, mean length of utterance in words, and morphosyntactic quality scores than younger children in both languages. Hierarchical regressions indicated that Cantonese story grammar predicted English story grammar and Cantonese microstructure predicted English microstructure. However, no correlation was observed between Cantonese and English morphosyntactic quality. CONCLUSIONS: The results of this study have implications for speech-language pathologists who collect narratives in Cantonese and English from bilingual preschoolers. The results suggest that there is a possible transfer in narrative abilities between the two languages.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Multilinguismo , Narração , Análise de Variância , Pré-Escolar , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Humanos , Linguística , Masculino , Fonética , Análise de Regressão
5.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 24(4): 717-32, 2015 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26363186

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The objective of this study was to examine the effects of coaching by speech-language pathologists on educators' interactive shared book reading, children's participation in shared reading, and children's language development. METHOD: Thirty-two educators and small groups of preschoolers were randomly assigned to experimental and comparison groups. The experimental group (n = 15) received 4 in-service workshops plus 5 individualized coaching sessions. The comparison group received only the 4 workshops. Participants were video-recorded during a shared book reading activity with a small group of children at pretest and posttest. The video recordings were transcribed and coded to yield measures of conversations, educators' questions, and children's responses. The mean length of utterances of the children's responses was also calculated. RESULTS: There were no significant Time × Group interaction effects for the number and length of shared reading conversations or for the number of participants in these conversations. However, significant Time × Group interactions were observed for the use of educators' experiential reasoning questions, children's experiential reasoning responses, and the mean length of utterances of children's responses. CONCLUSION: These results suggest that coaching increases educators' use of inferential questions, enhancing an interactive shared-reading strategy that had a direct impact on the children's quality and complexity of language.


Assuntos
Comportamento Cooperativo , Capacitação em Serviço , Comunicação Interdisciplinar , Relações Interpessoais , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Leitura , Escolas Maternais , Patologia da Fala e Linguagem , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Ontário
6.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 50(6): 830-41, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26215148

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The body of literature on narratives of bilingual children with and without specific language impairment (SLI) is growing. However, little is known about the narrative abilities of bilingual preschool children with SLI and their patterns of growth. AIMS: To determine the similarities and differences in narrative abilities between preschoolers with and without SLI who are either monolingual or bilingual at two time points. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Forty children completed a narrative retell task in English at two test points. The mean ages were 52 and 58 months at Times 1 and 2, respectively. We examined performance on measures of narrative macrostructure (narrative information) and microstructure (sentence length, number of different words, verb accuracy, first mentions) in monolingual and bilingual children with and without SLI. The bilingual children were from diverse first-language backgrounds and all spoke English most of the time. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: A series of repeated-measures analyses of variance was used with language ability (typical development or SLI) and bilingual status (monolingual versus bilingual) as the between-subjects factors and time (Times 1 or 2) as the within-subjects factor. Results indicated a significant main effect of time for four measures (i.e., Information Score, lexical diversity, sentence length and verb accuracy). The between-subjects analyses indicated a significant difference between the typically developing children and the children with SLI in all measures and a significant difference between monolingual and bilingual children for verb accuracy only. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: This study showed that all four groups of children showed growth over a 6-month period and that bilingual children exposed predominantly to English in the home performed similarly to their monolingual peers in measures of narrative information, sentence length, number of different words and first mentions.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Multilinguismo , Narração , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/terapia , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Psicometria
7.
Int J Speech Lang Pathol ; 17(6): 565-576, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25882445

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This study investigates the effects of peer-mediated intervention on the responses and initiations of pre-schoolers (aged 4;2-5;1) with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). METHOD: A speech-language pathologist and three early childhood educators trained typically-developing peers to facilitate responses and initiations from three pre-schoolers with ASD during playtime. A multiple baseline design across subjects was used to determine the effects of the intervention. Play sessions between the children with ASD and their typically-developing peers were videotaped. Videotapes were analysed using an interval coding system in order to collect information regarding responses and initiations. RESULT: All three target children demonstrated improvements in their responses and initiations to their peers during intervention. Moreover, they maintained these gains 4 weeks later and generalized their response and initiation skills to an untrained peer. Social validity data, obtained using unbiased, independent, lay, observers to rate the children's performance provided external validation of an observable treatment effect (i.e. increased responses and initiations) following intervention. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that training typically-developing peers to implement intervention strategies using the collaboration between a speech-language pathologist and early childhood educators may be an effective model of service delivery to enhance peer interaction skills of pre-schoolers with ASD.

8.
Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch ; 46(2): 94-111, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25615430

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The current study investigated the effects of coaching as part of an emergent literacy professional development program to increase early childhood educators' use of verbal references to print and phonological awareness during interactions with children. METHOD: Thirty-one educators and 4 children from each of their classrooms (N = 121) were randomly assigned to an experimental group (21 hr of in-service workshops plus 5 coaching sessions) and a comparison group (workshops alone). The in-service workshops included instruction on how to talk about print and phonological awareness during a post-story craft/writing activity. All educators were video-recorded during a 15-min craft/writing activity with a small group of preschoolers at pretest and posttest. All videotapes were transcribed and coded for verbal references to print and phonological awareness by the educators and children. RESULTS: Although at posttest, there were no significant group differences in the educators' or the children's references to print as measured by rate per minute, both the educators and the children in the experimental group used a significantly higher rate per minute of references to phonological awareness relative to the comparison group. CONCLUSION: Professional development that included coaching with a speech-language pathologist enabled educators and children to engage in more phonological awareness talk during this activity.


Assuntos
Conscientização , Linguagem Infantil , Educação Continuada/métodos , Fonética , Leitura , Ensino/métodos , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Ontário , Gravação em Vídeo
9.
Int J Speech Lang Pathol ; 17(4): 346-56, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25482999

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The purpose of this study was to investigate whether an emergent literacy professional development program enhanced educators' use of vocabulary-teaching strategies during shared reading with small groups of pre-schoolers. METHOD: Thirty-two pre-school educators and small groups of pre-schoolers from their classrooms were randomly assigned to experimental or comparison groups. The 15 educators in the experimental group received four in-service workshops as well as five individualized classroom coaching sessions. The comparison group received only the workshops. Each educator was video-recorded reading a storybook to a small group of pre-schoolers at pre-test and post-test. The videos were transcribed and coded to yield measures of the vocabulary-teaching strategies and children's vocabulary-related talk. RESULT: The findings revealed that the children in the experimental group engaged in significantly more vocabulary-related talk relative to the comparison group. A non-significant trend in the data indicated that educators in the experimental group used more vocabulary-teaching strategies at post-test. The educators' familiarity with children's authors and book titles at pre-test was a significant predictor of their outcomes. CONCLUSION: These findings suggest that an emergent literacy professional development program that includes coaching can enhance children's participation in vocabulary-related conversations with their educators.


Assuntos
Educação Continuada/métodos , Educação/métodos , Docentes , Leitura , Ensino/métodos , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Vocabulário
10.
J Dev Behav Pediatr ; 35(4): 274-81, 2014 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24748010

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine, in a community-based sample of slow-to-talk toddlers, the extent to which specific maternal responsive behaviors at 24 months predict child language at 24 and 36 months. METHODS: Mother-child dyads were recruited for this prospective longitudinal study from 3 local government areas spanning low, middle, and high socioeconomic status in Melbourne, Australia. At child age 18 months, 1138 parents completed a 100-word expressive vocabulary checklist; the 251 (22.1%) children scoring ≤20th percentile were then followed up to comprise this study. PREDICTORS: Six maternal responsive behaviors (imitations, interpretations, labels, expansions, supportive directives and responsive questions) were derived from mother-child free-play videos collected at 24 months of age and coded using the Observer XT system. OUTCOMES: Expressive and receptive language measured at 24 and 36 months of age (Preschool Language Scale-4), blind to maternal responsiveness ratings. RESULTS: Two hundred and twenty-six of the 251 (90.0%) mother-child dyads were followed up at 36 months. In confounder-adjusted linear regression analyses, expansions, imitations, and responsive questions were strongly associated with better receptive and expressive language at 24 and 36 months. Labels unexpectedly predicted poorer expressive language at 36 months. Expansions were the only maternal behavior that predicted improvement in language between 24 and 36 months. CONCLUSIONS: Maternal responsive behaviors, particularly expansions, offer promise in enhancing early language learning in slow-to-talk toddlers. Parent-child interactions characterized by frequent use of maternal labels at 24 months could also be a predictive marker of those slow-to-talk toddlers at greater risk of persistent language problems.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Comportamento Materno/psicologia , Relações Mãe-Filho/psicologia , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Prognóstico , Ensaios Clínicos Controlados Aleatórios como Assunto
11.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 21(1): 47-63, 2012 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22230181

RESUMO

PURPOSE: This study examined the efficacy of a professional development program for early childhood educators that facilitated emergent literacy skills in preschoolers. The program, led by a speech-language pathologist, focused on teaching alphabet knowledge, print concepts, sound awareness, and decontextualized oral language within naturally occurring classroom interactions. METHOD: Twenty educators were randomly assigned to experimental and control groups. Educators each recruited 3 to 4 children from their classrooms to participate. The experimental group participated in 18 hr of group training and 3 individual coaching sessions with a speech-language pathologist. The effects of intervention were examined in 30 min of videotaped interaction, including storybook reading and a post-story writing activity. RESULTS: At posttest, educators in the experimental group used a higher rate of utterances that included print/sound references and decontextualized language than the control group. Similarly, the children in the experimental group used a significantly higher rate of utterances that included print/sound references and decontextualized language compared to the control group. CONCLUSION: These findings suggest that professional development provided by a speech-language pathologist can yield short-term changes in the facilitation of emergent literacy skills in early childhood settings. Future research is needed to determine the impact of this program on the children's long-term development of conventional literacy skills.


Assuntos
Intervenção Educacional Precoce/organização & administração , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/reabilitação , Escolas Maternais/organização & administração , Distúrbios da Fala/reabilitação , Patologia da Fala e Linguagem/organização & administração , Desenvolvimento de Pessoal/métodos , Adulto , Linguagem Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Intervenção Educacional Precoce/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Avaliação de Programas e Projetos de Saúde , Leitura , Desenvolvimento de Pessoal/organização & administração , Adulto Jovem
12.
BMJ ; 343: d4741, 2011 Aug 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21852344

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: To determine the benefits of a low intensity parent-toddler language promotion programme delivered to toddlers identified as slow to talk on screening in universal services. DESIGN: Cluster randomised trial nested in a population based survey. SETTING: Three local government areas in Melbourne, Australia. PARTICIPANTS: Parents attending 12 month well child checks over a six month period completed a baseline questionnaire. At 18 months, children at or below the 20th centile on an expressive vocabulary checklist entered the trial. INTERVENTION: Maternal and child health centres (clusters) were randomly allocated to intervention (modified "You Make the Difference" programme over six weekly sessions) or control ("usual care") arms. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: The primary outcome was expressive language (Preschool Language Scale-4) at 2 and 3 years; secondary outcomes were receptive language at 2 and 3 years, vocabulary checklist raw score at 2 and 3 years, Expressive Vocabulary Test at 3 years, and Child Behavior Checklist/1.5-5 raw score at 2 and 3 years. RESULTS: 1217 parents completed the baseline survey; 1138 (93.5%) completed the 18 month checklist, when 301 (26.4%) children had vocabulary scores at or below the 20th centile and were randomised (158 intervention, 143 control). 115 (73%) intervention parents attended at least one session (mean 4.5 sessions), and most reported high satisfaction with the programme. Interim outcomes at age 2 years were similar in the two groups. Similarly, at age 3 years, adjusted mean differences (intervention-control) were -2.4 (95% confidence interval -6.2 to 1.4; P=0.21) for expressive language; -0.3 (-4.2 to 3.7; P=0.90) for receptive language; 4.1 (-2.3 to 10.6; P=0.21) for vocabulary checklist; -0.5 (-4.4 to 3.4; P=0.80) for Expressive Vocabulary Test; -0.1 (-1.6 to 1.4; P=0.86) for externalising behaviour problems; and -0.1 (-1.3 to 1.2; P=0. 92) for internalising behaviour problems. CONCLUSION: This community based programme targeting slow to talk toddlers was feasible and acceptable, but little evidence was found that it improved language or behaviour either immediately or at age 3 years. TRIAL REGISTRATION: Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN20953675.


Assuntos
Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/terapia , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Terapia da Linguagem/normas , Vocabulário , Serviços de Saúde da Criança , Pré-Escolar , Análise por Conglomerados , Estudos de Viabilidade , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Serviços de Saúde Materna , Inquéritos e Questionários , Resultado do Tratamento , Vitória
13.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 54(4): 1197-210, 2011 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21173389

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Little is known about the social interaction skills of children with severe to profound hearing loss (SPHL) in terms of how they manage conversational exchanges with peers. This study compared the initiation and response skills of children with SPHL with those of children with typical hearing during group play in integrated preschool programs. METHOD: Two groups of 12 children were matched on a number of variables and assessed for intelligence, language, speech, and social development. All initiations, responses, and resulting interactions during 20 min of group play were transcribed and coded. Outcome measures included number and type of initiation strategies, number of responses, and length of interactions. RESULTS: Despite poorer speech, language, and social development, there were no significant differences in initiation and response skills measured between children with SPHL and their matched peers. The small sample size may have made differences difficult to detect; however, playmates initiated interactions less often with the children with SPHL and ignored their initiations more often than those of other children. CONCLUSIONS: Preschool children with SPHL were excluded from interactions by their playmates. Having age-appropriate language skills did not ensure successful peer interactions. Inclusive preschool programs may consider offering classroom-wide social skills training to enhance interaction opportunities.


Assuntos
Implantes Cocleares/psicologia , Perda Auditiva/psicologia , Relações Interpessoais , Jogos e Brinquedos/psicologia , Comportamento Social , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Análise por Pareamento , Grupo Associado , Distância Psicológica , Valores de Referência
15.
J Commun Disord ; 43(6): 511-22, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20579660

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The aim of this study was to compare narrative abilities in children with specific language impairment (SLI) who are monolingual and those who are dual language learners. METHOD: The participants were 26 children with SLI, 14 monolingual English speakers and 12 dual language learners. The dual language learners were English dominant and spoke a variety of other languages in the home. The two SLI groups were compared using standardized tests and measures from two narrative samples. RESULTS: Compared to the monolingual children, the dual language learners achieved lower scores on standardized tests of morphosyntax but not on measures of language form derived from the narrative samples. Both groups achieved below average scores on productivity, narrative structure, literate language, and language form measures from the narrative samples. CONCLUSION: The data suggest that narrative samples can be a sensitive way to assess the language skills of dual language learners with specific language impairment. Furthermore, the findings are consistent with the position that English standardized tests may be a biased assessment measure when used with dual language learners, particularly for the assessment of expressive morphosyntactic skills. LEARNING OUTCOMES: Readers will be able to (1) describe the narrative abilities of typically developing dual language learners; (2) describe similarities between the narrative abilities of children with SLI who are monolingual and dual language learners; (3) identify ways to analyses narratives at a variety of levels.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Multilinguismo , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/etiologia , Testes de Linguagem , Masculino , Narração , Pais
16.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 42(5): 607-23, 2007.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17729148

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Previous research on the pragmatic abilities of late talkers at 24 months of age indicates that they have difficulties initiating conversational interactions, but possess intact responsiveness skills. This study uses a parent-administered questionnaire for evaluating the conversational skills of late talkers and suggesting pragmatic intervention goals. AIM: To examine the conversational assertiveness and responsiveness of late talkers at 2 years of age. METHODS & PROCEDURES: A parent report measure, The Social Conversational Skills Rating Scale-Italian version, was administered to 30 parents of late talkers, 30 parents of typically developing children matched for age, and 30 parents of younger, typically developing children matched for vocabulary size. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: The late talkers received significantly lower ratings for both assertiveness and responsiveness in comparison with their age-matched peers. They did not differ significantly from the younger, vocabulary-matched group. Assertiveness and responsiveness mean ratings were positively correlated with vocabulary size for the age-matched group, but not for either of the other two groups. CONCLUSIONS: This study confirms a delay in the development of late talkers' social-conversational skills. An investigation of individual profiles suggests that some late talkers may require goals for vocabulary development as well as independent goals for developing pragmatic skills.


Assuntos
Assertividade , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Comportamento Verbal , Pré-Escolar , Escolaridade , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Psicometria
17.
Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch ; 38(1): 72-83, 2007 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17218537

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The purpose of this study was to determine the feasibility of a 2-day in-service education program for (a) promoting the use of two emergent literacy strategies by early childhood educators and (b) increasing children's responses to these strategies. METHOD: Sixteen early childhood educators were randomly assigned to an experimental and a control group. The experimental in-service program sought to increase educators' use of abstract utterances and print references. Educators were videotaped with small groups of preschoolers during storybook reading and a post-story craft activity. Pretest and posttest videotapes were coded to yield rates of abstract language, verbal print references, and children's responses. RESULTS: In comparison to the control group, educators in the experimental program used more abstract utterances that elicited talk about emotions and children's past experiences during storybook reading. They also used significantly more print references during a post-story craft activity. In addition, children in the experimental group responded more often with appropriate responses to abstract utterances and print references in comparison to children in the control group. CONCLUSION: A 2-day in-service education program resulted in short-term behavioral changes in educators' use of abstract language and print references. Suggestions for improving instruction include providing opportunities for classroom practice with feedback, modeling the use of strategies in classroom routines, and long-term mentoring of educators to promote retention of gains.


Assuntos
Creches , Escolaridade , Gestão de Recursos Humanos , Competência Profissional , Ensino/métodos , Adulto , Criança , Educação , Estudos de Viabilidade , Humanos
18.
J Commun Disord ; 40(6): 470-92, 2007.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17169368

RESUMO

PURPOSE: The purpose of this multiple case study was to describe outcomes for three parents and their children following participation in a social interactive model of language intervention. More specifically, this study sought to clarify if changes in children's participation, engagement, and initiation of social interaction could be observed following an 11-week intervention. METHOD: Three preschool children with autism spectrum disorders and their mothers participated in an 11-week intervention program. The intervention taught parents to follow the children's lead, promote children's participation in routines, and model language at the children's level. Outcome measures included estimates of parents' responsive language input, and measures of children's rate of communication, number of engagements in social interaction, and initiations. RESULTS: The results indicated that all three mothers increased their responsive comments during play interactions and were rated as being more responsive on a rating scale. All three children evidenced positive outcomes in vocabulary and the number of engagements in social interaction. In addition, improvement was observed in social initiation skills for all three children. CONCLUSIONS: Increases in mothers' responsiveness and children's engagement in social interactions are consistent with the theoretical mechanisms of the social interaction model of language intervention. The results suggest that further randomized control trials of this intervention approach are warranted.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/terapia , Transtornos da Comunicação/terapia , Educação , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/terapia , Relações Mãe-Filho , Comportamento Social , Transtorno Autístico/diagnóstico , Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Terapia Comportamental , Pré-Escolar , Terapia Combinada , Transtornos da Comunicação/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Comunicação/psicologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/diagnóstico , Transtornos do Desenvolvimento da Linguagem/psicologia , Masculino , Avaliação de Resultados em Cuidados de Saúde , Comportamento Verbal , Vocabulário
19.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ ; 11(2): 214-23, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16410609

RESUMO

This exploratory study examined the attention-gaining and attention-regaining strategies used by a preschool educator who is Deaf during child-directed play. Four children (2 with typical hearing and 2 with severe-to-profound hearing loss) were videotaped interacting with the educator in two different play contexts. The educator used four different strategies to gain and to regain the children's attention: visual, visual using an American Sign Language (ASL) sign, tactile/vibratory, and observing/waiting. Overall, tactile and visual strategies were used with the same frequency and occurred more often than either waiting or using an ASL sign to establish joint attention. With the exception of waiting, all strategies were equally successful at gaining or regaining the children's attention. The knowledge and experience of educators with hearing loss potentially provide important insights into enhancing the effectiveness of the communicative environment for preschool children with hearing loss. The implications of this line of inquiry include training for educators on the effective use of strategies to establish joint attention with preschool children with hearing loss.


Assuntos
Atenção , Docentes , Pessoas com Deficiência Auditiva , Ensino , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Educação de Pessoas com Deficiência Auditiva , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoas com Deficiência Auditiva/psicologia , Língua de Sinais , Gravação de Videoteipe , Percepção Visual
20.
Am J Speech Lang Pathol ; 14(1): 14-26, 2005 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15962844

RESUMO

An exploratory study examined adults' questions to small groups of children to determine how questions influenced their response rate and complexity of response. Thirteen educators of toddlers and 13 educators of preschoolers were videotaped during free-play. Both groups of educators used an equivalent frequency of open-ended and closed questions, but the preschool educators used more topic-continuing questions. Consistent with their developmental level, preschoolers responded more frequently than toddlers. Toddlers demonstrated few effects of question type. In contrast, preschoolers used more multiword utterances following open-ended questions and topic-continuing questions. Implications for in-service education for staff of early childhood settings include increasing the use of both open-ended and topic-continuing questions.


Assuntos
Linguagem Infantil , Hospital Dia , Entrevistas como Assunto , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Jogos e Brinquedos , Ensino , Comportamento Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA