Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 9 de 9
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Food Chem ; 362: 130224, 2021 Nov 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34098439

RESUMO

This study evaluated the feasibility of curcumin based photodynamic sterilization technology (PDT) applied to fresh-cut potato slices. Potato samples with 30 µmol L-1 curcumin solution were exposed to 420 nm light emitting diodes (LED) at a total dose of 0.7 kJ cm-2. Results showed that PDT inactivated 2.43 log CFU mL-1 of Escherichia coli (BL 21) and 3.18 log CFU mL-1 of Staphylococcus aureus and maintained the color, texture, weight as well as total solid content of treated potatoes. Additionally, loss of phenols and flavonoids was significantly prevented, increasing the total antioxidant capacity. This was attributed to changes in enzyme activity that PDT decreased the activity of polyphenol oxidase (PPO) and peroxidase (POD) by 59.7% and 47.8% and increased the activity of phenylalanine ammonia-lyase (PAL). Therefore, curcumin-based PDT has the potential to maintain the commercial quality of producing and achieving microbiological safety.


Assuntos
Antioxidantes/química , Curcumina/farmacologia , Conservação de Alimentos/métodos , Solanum tuberosum/química , Solanum tuberosum/microbiologia , Antibacterianos/farmacologia , Catecol Oxidase/metabolismo , Cor , Escherichia coli , Flavonoides/análise , Flavonoides/química , Conservação de Alimentos/instrumentação , Qualidade dos Alimentos , Peroxidase/química , Peroxidase/metabolismo , Fenóis/análise , Fenóis/química , Fenilalanina Amônia-Liase/metabolismo , Solanum tuberosum/efeitos dos fármacos , Solanum tuberosum/metabolismo , Staphylococcus aureus
2.
J Neurosci ; 2021 Jun 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34088795

RESUMO

Two forms of information - frequency (content) and ordinal position (structure) - have to be stored when retaining a sequence of auditory tones in working memory (WM). However, the neural representations and coding characteristics of content and structure, particularly during WM maintenance, remain elusive. Here, in two electroencephalography (EEG) studies in human participants (both sexes), by transiently perturbing the 'activity-silent' WM retention state and decoding the reactivated WM information, we demonstrate that content and structure are stored in a dissociative manner with distinct characteristics throughout WM process. First, each tone in the sequence is associated with two codes in parallel, characterizing its frequency and ordinal position, respectively. Second, during retention, a structural retrocue successfully reactivates structure but not content, whereas a following white noise triggers content but not structure. Third, structure representation remains stable whereas content code undergoes a dynamic transformation through memory progress. Finally, the noise-triggered content reactivations during retention correlate with subsequent WM behavior. Overall, our results support distinct content and structure representations in auditory WM and provide an efficient approach to access the silently stored WM information in the human brain. The dissociation of content and structure could facilitate efficient memory formation via generalizing stable structure to new auditory contents.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTIn memory experiences, contents do not exist independently but are linked with each other via ordinal structure. For instance, recalling a piece of favorite music relies on correct ordering (sequence structure) of musical tones (content). How are the structure and content for an auditory temporally structured experience maintained in working memory? Here, by employing impulse-response approach and time-resolved representational dissimilarity analysis on human EEG recordings in an auditory working memory task, we reveal that content and structure are stored in a dissociated way, which would facilitate efficient and rapid memory formation through generalizing stable structure knowledge to new auditory inputs.

3.
Prog Neurobiol ; 201: 102023, 2021 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33617918

RESUMO

Temporarily storing a list of items in working memory (WM), a fundamental ability in cognition, has been posited to rely on the temporal dynamics of multi-item neural representations during retention. However, the causal evidence, particularly in human subjects, is still lacking, let alone WM manipulation. Here, we develop a novel "dynamic perturbation" approach to manipulate the relative memory strength of WM items held in human brain, by presenting temporally correlated luminance sequences during retention to interfere with the multi-item neural dynamics. Six experiments on more than 150 subjects confirm the effectiveness of this WM manipulation approach. A computational model combining continuous attractor neural network (CANN) and short-term synaptic plasticity (STP) principles further reproduces all the empirical findings. The model reveals that the "dynamic perturbation" modifies the synaptic efficacies of WM items through STP principles, eventually leading to changes in their relative memory strengths. Our results support the causal role of temporal dynamics of neural network in mediating multi-item WM, and offer a promising, purely bottom-up approach to manipulate WM.


Assuntos
Cognição , Memória de Curto Prazo , Encéfalo , Humanos , Plasticidade Neuronal
4.
Carbohydr Polym ; 230: 115635, 2020 Feb 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31887858

RESUMO

In this study, 6-deoxy-6-arginine modified chitosan (DAC), was synthesized and characterized by Fourier Transform Infrared Spectroscopy (FTIR), 1H and 13C nuclear magnetic resonance (NMR), differential scanning calorimetry (DSC) and elemental analysis. The arginine was grafted onto C6 groups of chitosan. Antibacterial activity of DAC against gram-negative bacteria Escherichia coli (E. coli) and gram-positive bacteria Staphylococcus aureus (S. aureus) were investigated at concentration between 0.02 mg/mL and 10 mg/mL. Cell viability assessment was estimated in vitro with Caco-2 and L929 cells. Water solubility of DAC at different pH was also evaluated. The results showed that the minimum inhibitory concentration (MICs) of DAC against S. aureus and E. coli were 0.078 mg/mL and 0.312 mg/mL, respectively. The minimum bactericidal concentration (MBC) against S. aureus and E. coli was 0.625 mg/mL. The cytotoxicity of chitosan and DAC was not significantly different. It demonstrated that DAC might be a potential safe antibacterial agent.


Assuntos
Antibacterianos , Quitosana , Antibacterianos/química , Antibacterianos/farmacologia , Arginina/química , Células CACO-2 , Quitosana/análogos & derivados , Quitosana/química , Quitosana/farmacologia , Escherichia coli/efeitos dos fármacos , Humanos , Testes de Sensibilidade Microbiana/métodos , Solubilidade , Staphylococcus aureus/efeitos dos fármacos
5.
Eur J Neurosci ; 50(6): 2931-2941, 2019 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30864167

RESUMO

Crowding, the disrupted recognition of a peripheral target in the presence of nearby flankers, sets a fundamental limit on peripheral vision perception. Debates persist on whether the limit occurs at early visual cortices or is induced by top-down modulation, leaving the neural mechanism for visual crowding largely unclear. To resolve the debate, it is crucial to extract the neural signals elicited by the target from that by the target-flanker clutter, with high temporal resolution. To achieve this purpose, here we employed a temporal response function (TRF) approach to dissociate target-specific response from the overall electroencephalograph (EEG) recordings when the target was presented with (crowded) or without flankers (uncrowded) while subjects were performing a discrimination task on the peripherally presented target. Our results demonstrated two components in the target-specific contrast-tracking TRF response-an early component (100-170 ms) in occipital channels and a late component (210-450 ms) in frontoparietal channels. The late frontoparietal component, which was delayed in time under the crowded condition, was correlated with target discrimination performance, suggesting its involvement in visual crowding. Granger causality analysis further revealed stronger top-down modulation on the target stimulus under the crowded condition. Taken together, our findings support that crowding is associated with a top-down process which modulates the low-level sensory processing and delays the behavioral-relevant response in the high-level region.


Assuntos
Lobo Frontal/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
6.
Elife ; 72018 10 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30334735

RESUMO

Storing temporal sequences of events (i.e., sequence memory) is fundamental to many cognitive functions. However, it is unknown how the sequence order information is maintained and represented in working memory and its behavioral significance, particularly in human subjects. We recorded electroencephalography (EEG) in combination with a temporal response function (TRF) method to dissociate item-specific neuronal reactivations. We demonstrate that serially remembered items are successively reactivated during memory retention. The sequential replay displays two interesting properties compared to the actual sequence. First, the item-by-item reactivation is compressed within a 200 - 400 ms window, suggesting that external events are associated within a plasticity-relevant window to facilitate memory consolidation. Second, the replay is in a temporally reversed order and is strongly related to the recency effect in behavior. This fast-backward replay, previously revealed in rat hippocampus and demonstrated here in human cortical activities, might constitute a general neural mechanism for sequence memory and learning.


Have you ever played the 'Memory Maze Challenge' game, or its predecessor from the 1980s, 'Simon'? Players must memorize a sequence of colored lights, and then reproduce the sequence by tapping the colors on a pad. The sequence becomes longer with each trial, making the task more and more difficult. One wrong response and the game is over. Storing and retrieving sequences is key to many cognitive processes, from following speech to hitting a tennis ball to recalling what you did last week. Such tasks require memorizing the order in which items occur as well as the items themselves. But how do we hold this information in memory? Huang et al. reveal the answer by using scalp electrodes to record the brain activity of healthy volunteers as they memorize and then recall a sequence. Memorizing, or encoding, each of the items in the sequence triggered a distinct pattern of brain activity. As the volunteers held the sequence in memory, their brains replayed these activity patterns one after the other. But this replay showed two non-intuitive features. First, it was speeded up relative to the original encoding. In fact, the brain compressed the entire sequence into about 200 to 400 milliseconds. Second, the brain replayed the sequence backwards. The activity pattern corresponding to the last item was replayed first, while that corresponding to the first item was replayed last. This 'fast-backward' replay may explain why we tend to recall items at the end of a list better than those in the middle, a phenomenon known as the recency effect. The results of Huang et al. suggest that when we hold a list of items in memory, the brain does not replay the list in its original form, like an echo. Instead, the brain restructures and reorganizes the list, compressing and reversing it. This process, which is also seen in rodents, helps the brain to incorporate the list of items into existing neuronal networks for memory storage.


Assuntos
Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Ritmo alfa/fisiologia , Comportamento , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Humanos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Fatores de Tempo
7.
Optom Vis Sci ; 94(6): 714-724, 2017 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28538337

RESUMO

PURPOSE: An individual's reading ability cannot be reliably predicted from his/her letter acuity, contrast sensitivity, and visual field extent. We developed a set of Chinese reading acuity charts (C-READ) to assess the reading ability of Chinese readers, based on the collective wisdom of previously published reading acuity charts, especially the MNRead and the Radner Reading Charts. METHODS: The C-READ consists of three charts. Each consists sixteen 12-character simplified Chinese sentences crafted from first- to third-grade textbooks. One hundred eighteen native Chinese-speaking college students (aged 22.1 ± 2.1 years) with normal or corrected to normal near vision (-0.26 ± 0.05 logMAR) were included in the study to develop the C-READ charts, to test the homogeneity of the three charts, and to validate the C-READ against the text paragraphs from the International Reading Speed Texts (IReST) with corrected and uncorrected near vision. RESULTS: The reading acuity, critical print size, and maximum reading speed for young normal native Chinese-speaking readers were 0.16 ± 0.05 logMAR, 0.24 ± 0.06 logMAR, and 273.44 ± 34.37 characters per minute (mean ± SD), respectively. The reliability test revealed no significant differences among the three C-READ charts and no significant test order effect in the three reading parameters. Regression analyses showed that the IReST reading speed could be reliably predicted by the C-READ maximum reading speed under the corrected near-vision condition (adjusted R = 0.72) and by C-READ maximum reading speed and critical print size under the uncorrected near-vision condition (adjusted R = 0.69). CONCLUSIONS: The three C-READ charts are very comparable to each other, and there is no significant order effect. Reading test results can accurately predict continuous text reading performance quantified by the IReST reading speed over a wide range of refractive errors. The C-READ is a reliable and valid clinical instrument for quantifying reading performance in simplified Chinese readers.


Assuntos
Leitura , Transtornos da Visão/diagnóstico , Testes Visuais/métodos , Acuidade Visual/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Traduções , Transtornos da Visão/fisiopatologia , Adulto Jovem
8.
Mater Sci Eng C Mater Biol Appl ; 67: 599-610, 2016 Oct 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27287159

RESUMO

To engineer bone tissue, a scaffold with good biological properties should be provided to approximate the hierarchical structure of collagen fibrils in natural bone. In this study, we fabricated a novel scaffold consisting of multilayer nanofiber fabrics (MLNFFs) by weaving nanofiber yarns of polylactic acid (PLA) and Tussah silk fibroin (TSF). The yarns were fabricated by electrospinning, and we found that spinnability, as well as the mechanical properties of the resulting scaffold, was determined by the ratio between polylactic acid and Tussah silk fibroin. In particular, a 9:1 mixture can be spun continuously into nanofiber yarns with narrow diameter distribution and good mechanical properties. Accordingly, woven scaffolds based on this mixture had excellent mechanical properties, with Young's modulus 417.65MPa and tensile strength 180.36MPa. For nonwoven scaffolds fabricated from the same materials, the Young's modulus and tensile strength were 2- and 4-fold lower, respectively. Woven scaffolds also supported adhesion and proliferation of mouse mesenchymal stem cells, and promoted biomineralization via alkaline phosphatase and mineral deposition. Finally, the scaffolds significantly enhanced the formation of new bone in damaged femoral condyle in rabbits. Thus, the scaffolds are potentially suitable for bone tissue engineering because of biomimetic architecture, excellent mechanical properties, and good biocompatibility.


Assuntos
Substitutos Ósseos , Calcificação Fisiológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Fêmur , Fibroínas/química , Nanofibras/química , Poliésteres/química , Engenharia Tecidual , Alicerces Teciduais/química , Animais , Substitutos Ósseos/química , Substitutos Ósseos/farmacologia , Células Cultivadas , Módulo de Elasticidade , Fêmur/lesões , Fêmur/metabolismo , Fêmur/patologia , Células-Tronco Mesenquimais/citologia , Células-Tronco Mesenquimais/metabolismo , Camundongos , Coelhos
9.
Mater Sci Eng C Mater Biol Appl ; 58: 342-51, 2016 Jan 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26478319

RESUMO

The bone is a composite of inorganic and organic materials and possesses a complex hierarchical architecture consisting of mineralized fibrils formed by collagen molecules and coated with oriented hydroxyapatite. To regenerate bone tissue, it is necessary to provide a scaffold that mimics the architecture of the extracellular matrix in native bone. Here, we describe one such scaffold, a nanostructured composite with a core made of a composite of hydroxyapatite and tussah silk fibroin. The core is encased in a shell of tussah silk fibroin. The composite fibers were fabricated by coaxial electrospinning using green water solvent and were characterized using different techniques. In comparison to nanofibers of pure tussah silk, composite notably improved mechanical properties, with 90-fold and 2-fold higher initial modulus and breaking stress, respectively, obtained. Osteoblast-like MG-63 cells were cultivated on the composite to assess its suitability as a scaffold for bone tissue engineering. We found that the fiber scaffold supported cell adhesion and proliferation and functionally promoted alkaline phosphatase and mineral deposition relevant for biomineralization. In addition, the composite were more biocompatible than pure tussah silk fibroin or cover slip. Thus, the nanostructured composite has excellent biomimetic and mechanical properties and is a potential biocompatible scaffold for bone tissue engineering.


Assuntos
Materiais Biocompatíveis/química , Durapatita/química , Fibroínas/química , Nanopartículas/química , Engenharia Tecidual , Animais , Materiais Biocompatíveis/farmacologia , Bombyx/metabolismo , Osso e Ossos/fisiologia , Linhagem Celular , Proliferação de Células/efeitos dos fármacos , Humanos , Microscopia Confocal , Nanofibras/química , Nanofibras/ultraestrutura , Nanopartículas/toxicidade , Nanopartículas/ultraestrutura , Espectroscopia de Infravermelho com Transformada de Fourier , Resistência à Tração , Alicerces Teciduais
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA