Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 23
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cogn Psychol ; 91: 124-149, 2016 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27773367

RESUMO

Although learning and development reflect changes situated in an individual brain, most discussions of behavioral change are based on the evidence of group averages. Our reliance on group-averaged data creates a dilemma. On the one hand, we need to use traditional inferential statistics. On the other hand, group averages are highly ambiguous when we need to understand change in the individual; the average pattern of change may characterize all, some, or none of the individuals in the group. Here we present a new method for statistically characterizing developmental change in each individual child we study. Using false-belief tasks, fifty-two children in two cohorts were repeatedly tested for varying lengths of time between 3 and 5 years of age. Using a novel Bayesian change point analysis, we determined both the presence and-just as importantly-the absence of change in individual longitudinal cumulative records. Whenever the analysis supports a change conclusion, it identifies in that child's record the most likely point at which change occurred. Results show striking variability in patterns of change and stability across individual children. We then group the individuals by their various patterns of change or no change. The resulting patterns provide scarce support for sudden changes in competence and shed new light on the concepts of "passing" and "failing" in developmental studies.


Assuntos
Formação de Conceito , Individualidade , Teoria da Mente , Teorema de Bayes , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicologia da Criança
2.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 32(3): 320-9, 2014 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24766200

RESUMO

Creative labour has an effect on children's and adults' ownership decisions in Western cultures. We investigated whether preschoolers and adults from an Eastern culture (Japan) would show a similar bias. In a first-party task (Experiment 1), in which participants created their own objects, Japanese preschoolers but not adults assigned ownership to creators. When participants watched videos of third-party conflicts between owners of materials and creators (Experiment 2), Japanese adults, but not preschoolers, transferred ownership to creators. In a British comparison group, both preschoolers and adults showed an effect of creative labour in the third-party task. A bias to attribute ownership on the basis of creative labour is thus not specific to Western culture.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Criatividade , Comparação Transcultural , Propriedade , Trabalho/psicologia , Adulto , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Japão/etnologia , Masculino , Reino Unido/etnologia
3.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 76(1): 49-63, 2014 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24170380

RESUMO

During search, executive function enables individuals to direct attention to potential targets, remember locations visited, and inhibit distracting information. In the present study, we investigated these executive processes in large-scale search. In our tasks, participants searched a room containing an array of illuminated locations embedded in the floor. The participants' task was to press the switches at the illuminated locations on the floor so as to locate a target that changed color when pressed. The perceptual salience of the search locations was manipulated by having some locations flashing and some static. Participants were more likely to search at flashing locations, even when they were explicitly informed that the target was equally likely to be at any location. In large-scale search, attention was captured by the perceptual salience of the flashing lights, leading to a bias to explore these targets. Despite this failure of inhibition, participants were able to restrict returns to previously visited locations, a measure of spatial memory performance. Participants were more able to inhibit exploration to flashing locations when they were not required to remember which locations had previously been visited. A concurrent digit-span memory task further disrupted inhibition during search, as did a concurrent auditory attention task. These experiments extend a load theory of attention to large-scale search, which relies on egocentric representations of space. High cognitive load on working memory leads to increased distractor interference, providing evidence for distinct roles for the executive subprocesses of memory and inhibition during large-scale search.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Apresentação de Dados , Feminino , Voluntários Saudáveis , Humanos , Inibição Psicológica , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(5): 556; discussion 571-87, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24103610

RESUMO

Jeffery et al. accurately identify the importance of developing an understanding of spatial reference frames in a three-dimensional world. We examine human spatial cognition via a unique paradigm that investigates the role of saliency and adjusting reference frames. This includes work with adults, typically developing children, and children who develop non-typically (e.g., those with autism).


Assuntos
Cognição/fisiologia , Modelos Neurológicos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial , Animais , Humanos
5.
Dev Sci ; 15(3): 417-25, 2012 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22490181

RESUMO

This study examined children's ability to use mutual eye gaze as a cue to friendships in others. In Experiment 1, following a discussion about friendship, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds were shown animations in which three cartoon children looked at one another, and were told that one target character had a best friend. Although all age groups accurately detected the mutual gaze between the target and another character, only 5- and 6-year-olds used this cue to infer friendship. Experiment 2 replicated the effect with 5- and 6-year-olds when the target character was not explicitly identified. Finally, in Experiment 3, where the attribution of friendship could only be based on synchronized mutual gaze, 6-year-olds made this attribution, while 4- and 5-year-olds did not. Children occasionally referred to mutual eye gaze when asked to justify their responses in Experiments 2 and 3, but it was only by the age of 6 that reference to these cues correlated with the use of mutual gaze in judgements of affiliation. Although younger children detected mutual gaze, it was not until 6 years of age that children reliably detected and justified mutual gaze as a cue to friendship.


Assuntos
Sinais (Psicologia) , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Amigos , Comunicação não Verbal/fisiologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Comunicação não Verbal/psicologia , Percepção Social , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
6.
J Comp Psychol ; 125(4): 436-45, 2011 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21767009

RESUMO

The endowment effect describes the bias that people often value things that they possess more than things they do not possess. Thus, they are often reluctant to trade items in their possession for items of equivalent value. Some nonhuman primates appear to share this bias with humans, but it remains an open question whether they show endowment effects to the same extent as humans do. We investigated endowment effects in all four great ape species (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus) by varying whether apes were endowed with food items (Experiment 1, N = 22) or tools that were instrumental in retrieving food (Experiment 2, N = 23). We first assessed apes' preferences for items of a pair and their willingness to trade items in their possession. We then endowed apes with one item of a pair and offered them to trade for the other item. Apes showed endowment effects for food, but not for tools. In Experiment 3, we endowed bonobos (N = 4) and orangutans (N = 5) with either one or 12 food items. Endowment effects did not differ between species and were not influenced by the number of endowed food items. Our findings suggest that endowment effects in great apes are restricted to immediate food gratification and remain unaffected by the quantity of food rewards. However, endowment effects do not seem to extend to other, nonconsumable possessions even when they are instrumental in retrieving food. In general, apes do not show endowment effects across a range of different commodities as humans typically do.


Assuntos
Gorilla gorilla/psicologia , Julgamento , Pan paniscus/psicologia , Pan troglodytes/psicologia , Pongo pygmaeus/psicologia , Animais , Comportamento de Escolha , Feminino , Alimentos , Masculino , Recompensa
7.
Dev Sci ; 14(2): 270-9, 2011 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22213900

RESUMO

Core knowledge theories advocate the primacy of fundamental principles that constrain cognitive development from early infancy. However, there is concern that core knowledge of object properties does not constrain older preschoolers' reasoning during manual search. Here we address in detail both failure and success on two well-established search measures that require reasoning about solidity. We show that poor performance arises from an inability to engage the appropriate search strategy rather than a simple failure of core knowledge. Moreover, we demonstrate that successful search is positively correlated with inhibitory control. We believe that toddlers' manual search for an occluded object reflects a general capacity to deploy inhibition so that search behaviour can be guided by core knowledge.


Assuntos
Cognição , Função Executiva , Resolução de Problemas , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Formação de Conceito , Feminino , Humanos , Inibição Psicológica , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos
8.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(1): 421-6, 2011 Jan 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21173235

RESUMO

It is well established that children with autism often show outstanding visual search skills. To date, however, no study has tested whether these skills, usually assessed on a table-top or computer, translate to more true-to-life settings. One prominent account of autism, Baron-Cohen's "systemizing" theory, gives us good reason to suspect that they should. In this study, we tested whether autistic children's exceptional skills at small-scale search extend to a large-scale environment and, in so doing, tested key claims of the systemizing account. Twenty school-age children with autism and 20 age- and ability-matched typical children took part in a large-scale search task in the "foraging room": a purpose-built laboratory, with numerous possible search locations embedded into the floor. Children were instructed to search an array of 16 (green) locations to find the hidden (red) target as quickly as possible. The distribution of target locations was manipulated so that they appeared on one side of the midline for 80% of trials. Contrary to predictions of the systemizing account, autistic children's search behavior was much less efficient than that of typical children: they showed reduced sensitivity to the statistical properties of the search array, and furthermore, their search patterns were strikingly less optimal and less systematic. The nature of large-scale search behavior in autism cannot therefore be explained by a facility for systemizing. Rather, children with autism showed difficulties exploring and exploiting the large-scale space, which might instead be attributed to constraints (rather than benefits) in their cognitive repertoire.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Comportamento Exploratório/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Reino Unido
9.
Psychol Sci ; 21(9): 1236-41, 2010 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20713635

RESUMO

Recognizing property ownership is of critical importance in social interactions, but little is known about how and when this attribute emerges. We investigated whether preschool children and adults believe that ownership of one person's property is transferred to a second person following the second person's investment of creative labor in that property. In our study, an experimenter and a participant borrowed modeling-clay objects from each other to mold into new objects. Participants were more likely to transfer ownership to the second individual after he or she invested creative labor in the object than after any other manipulations (holding the object, making small changes to it). This effect was significantly stronger in preschool children than in adults. Duration of manipulation had no effect on property-ownership transfer. Changes in the object's identity acted only as a secondary cue for children. We conclude that ownership is transferred after an investment of creative labor and that determining property ownership may be an intuitive process that emerges in early childhood.


Assuntos
Propriedade , Trabalho/psicologia , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Criatividade , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
10.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 36(3): 605-18, 2010 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438260

RESUMO

Finding an object in our environment is an important human ability that also represents a critical component of human foraging behavior. One type of information that aids efficient large-scale search is the likelihood of the object being in one location over another. In this study we investigated the conditions under which individuals respond to this likelihood, and the reference frames in which this information is coded, using a novel, large-scale environmental search paradigm. Participants searched an array of locations, on the floor of a room, for a hidden target by pressing switches at each location. We manipulated the probability of the target being at a particular set of locations. Participants reliably learned target likelihoods when the possible search locations were kept constant throughout the experiment and the starting location was fixed. There was no evidence of such learning when room-based and body-based reference frames were dissociated. However, when this was combined with a more salient perceptual landmark, an allocentric cuing effect was observed. These data suggest that the encoding of this type of statistical contingency depends on the combination of spatial cues.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Comportamento Exploratório/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Comportamento Espacial/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Atenção/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientação/fisiologia , Probabilidade , Fatores Sexuais , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia
11.
Dev Psychol ; 44(6): 1655-67, 2008 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18999328

RESUMO

This study examined whether the well-documented adult tendency to perceive gaze aversion as a lying cue is also evident in children. In Experiment 1, 6-year-olds, 9-year-olds, and adults were shown video vignettes of speakers who either maintained or avoided eye contact while answering an interviewer's questions. Participants evaluated whether the speaker was telling the truth or lying on each trial. The results revealed that at both ages, children were more likely to attribute lying to speakers in the gaze aversion condition; however, the effect was significantly greater among 9-year-olds. Significant gender differences were also uncovered, with girls demonstrating strongest sensitivity to the gaze cue. Experiment 2 replicated the gender effect in 6-year-olds but found that when the speakers' verbal responses were removed, boys' use of the gaze cue increased and the gender difference disappeared. These findings indicate that at 6 years old, children interpret interpersonal gaze behavior as a socially informative cue. Furthermore, the growing appreciation of the stereotypic gaze behavior associated with lying and the reputed female advantage in gaze sensitivity may reflect differential processing of multimodal communication.


Assuntos
Cultura , Enganação , Fixação Ocular , Relações Interpessoais , Orientação , Percepção Visual , Criança , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Generalização Psicológica , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Comunicação não Verbal , Fatores Sexuais , Percepção da Fala , Adulto Jovem
12.
Cogn Process ; 9(2): 121-6, 2008 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18188627

RESUMO

It has been argued that visual search is a valid model for human foraging. However, the two tasks differ greatly in terms of the coding of space and the effort required to search. Here we describe a direct comparison between visually guided searches (as studied in visual search tasks) and foraging that is not based upon a visually distinct target, within the same context. The experiment was conducted in a novel apparatus, where search locations were indicated by an array of lights embedded in the floor. In visually guided conditions participants searched for a target defined by the presence of a feature (red target amongst green distractors) or the absence of a feature (green target amongst red and green distractors). Despite the expanded search scale and the different response requirements, these conditions followed the pattern found in conventional visual search paradigms: feature-present search latencies were not linearly related to display size, whereas feature-absent searches were longer as the number of distractors increased. In a non-visually guided foraging condition, participants searched for a target that was only visible once the switch was activated. This resulted in far longer latencies that rose markedly with display size. Compared to eye-movements in previous visual search studies, there were few revisit errors to previously inspected locations in this condition. This demonstrates the important distinction between visually guided and non-visually guided foraging processes, and shows that the visual search paradigm is an equivocal model for general search in any context. We suggest a comprehensive model of human spatial search behaviour needs to include search at a small and large scale as well as visually guided and non-visually guided search.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
13.
Cognition ; 107(3): 1102-11, 2008 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18062948

RESUMO

An influential series of studies have argued that young children are unable to use landmark information to reorient. However, these studies have used artificial experimental environments that may lead to an underestimation of the children's ability. We tested whether young children could reorient using landmarks in an ecologically valid setting. Children aged between 3 and 7 years completed a reorientation task in open parkland, and the properties of the search array (size and distinctiveness) were manipulated in a within-subjects design. Responses were recorded using Global Positioning Systems technology. All age groups performed above chance level, demonstrating that young children can reorient using natural landmarks. This behaviour was modulated by the nature of the search array: children were more accurate when the locations were spaced in a large array, and when the search locations were distinctively coloured. This suggests that the integration between landmarks and search locations, at different spatial scales, is a key factor in characterising human reorientation in the real world.


Assuntos
Cognição , Meio Ambiente , Orientação , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Tempo de Reação
14.
Cognition ; 106(1): 455-62, 2008 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17335793

RESUMO

Adults value certain unique individuals--such as artwork, sentimental possessions, and memorabilia--more than perfect duplicates. Here we explore the origins of this bias in young children, by using a conjurer's illusion where we appear to produce identical copies of real-world objects. In Study 1, young children were less likely to accept an identical replacement for an attachment object than for a favorite toy. In Study 2, children often valued a personal possession of Queen Elizabeth II more than an identical copy, but showed no such bias for another sort of valuable object. These findings suggest that young children develop attachments to individuals that are independent of any perceptible properties that the individuals possess.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Cognição , Apego ao Objeto , Psicologia da Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Ilusões Ópticas
15.
Perception ; 35(8): 1129-36, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17076070

RESUMO

Under suitable conditions, pupillary dilation is a reliable index of processing activity. Pupil size was tracked in male and female observers following the presentation of face stimuli for an age-judgment task. The eyes on the faces were either directed towards the observer or deviated to the side. Pupil dilation accompanied processing of the faces, but female observers showed significantly more sustained pupil dilation when viewing direct- than deviated-gaze faces over the period 3 to 7 s after stimulus onset, regardless of stimulus sex. In contrast, male observers did not show a consistent pattern in response to either the gaze or sex of the face stimuli. These findings indicate a sex difference in the processing of gaze direction and suggest that females, but not males, apply increased effort to processing socially relevant (direct-gaze) than irrelevant (deviated-gaze) faces. They also demonstrate that pupillary measurement can potentially provide new insights into the processing of even visual input, provided reflexes are sufficiently controlled.


Assuntos
Face , Fixação Ocular , Reflexo Pupilar/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Fatores Sexuais
16.
Dev Sci ; 9(5): 465-72, 2006 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16911448

RESUMO

The effects of gaze direction on memory for faces were studied in children from three different age groups (6-7, 8-9, and 10-11 years old) using a computerized version of a task devised by Hood, Macrae, Cole-Davies and Dias (2003). Participants were presented with a sequence of faces in an encoding phase, and were then required to judge which faces they had previously encountered in a surprise two-alternative forced-choice recognition test. In one condition, stimulus eye gaze was either direct or deviated at the viewing phase, and eyes were closed at the test phase. In another condition, stimulus eyes were closed at the viewing phase, with either direct or deviated gaze at the test phase. Modulation of gaze direction affected hit rates, with participants demonstrating greater accuracy for direct gaze targets compared to deviated gaze targets in both conditions. Reaction times (RT) to correctly recognized stimuli were faster for direct gaze stimuli at the viewing phase, but not at the test phase. The age group of participants differentially affected these measures: there was a greater hit rate advantage for direct gaze stimuli in older children, although RTs were less affected by age. These findings suggest that while the facilitation of face recognition by gaze direction is robust across encoding and recognition stages, the efficiency of the process is affected by the stage at which gaze is modulated.


Assuntos
Movimentos Oculares , Memória , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Fatores Etários , Criança , Eficiência , Face , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial
17.
Dev Sci ; 9(3): 303-8, 2006 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16669801

RESUMO

Children who could overcome the gravity error on Hood's (1995) tubes task were tested in a condition where they had to monitor two falling balls. This condition significantly impaired search performance with the majority of mistakes being gravity errors. In a second experiment, the effect of monitoring two balls was compared in the tubes task and a spatial transposition task not involving gravity. Again, monitoring two objects produced impaired search performance in the gravity task but not in the spatial transposition task. These findings support the view that divided attention disrupts the ability to exercise inhibitory control over the gravity error and that the performance drop on this task is not due to the additional task demands incurred by divided attention.


Assuntos
Atenção , Gravitação , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Tempo de Reação , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Percepção Visual
18.
Dev Psychol ; 42(1): 142-52, 2006 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16420124

RESUMO

This study examined 4- and 5-year-olds' ability to spontaneously use the relative duration and frequency of another's object-directed gaze for inferring that person's preference. In Experiment 1, analysis revealed a strong age effect for judgment accuracy, which could not be accounted for by cue-monitoring proficiency. Reducing the saliency of the objects in Experiment 2 yielded significant improvement in the younger children's performance. Thus, at 4 years, children already show signs of attending to the temporal dimension of gaze for making mentalistic inferences of preferential liking, but their competence may be undermined by the object choices themselves. By 5 years, they appear to overcome this competition. The obtained developmental difference is discussed in terms of concurrent transitions in attention regulation.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Fixação Ocular , Atenção , Pré-Escolar , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Tempo de Reação , Percepção Social , Fatores de Tempo , Percepção Visual
19.
Perception ; 34(10): 1221-9, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16309116

RESUMO

It has been argued that visual-search tasks provide a valid model for foraging behaviour. However, Gilchrist et al (2001 Perception 30 1459-1464) demonstrated that, whilst some aspects of behaviour transferred to large-scale egocentric search, there were substantially fewer revisits to previously searched locations than would be expected from the visual-search literature. This difference might be a result of the greater effort required to search in a large-scale egocentric context. Here, we present a novel, automated paradigm, for examining the effect of effort on egocentric search behaviour by manipulating the motor difficulty of the task. Children searched for a hidden target amongst a randomised display of lights by activating a switch at each potential location. The motor difficulty of the task was manipulated by requiring children to search with either their dominant or their nondominant hand. We found that when children searched with their nondominant hand, they made significantly more revisits to previously checked locations than they did when using their dominant hand. This suggests that, when the motor response was more effortful, children were less able to efficiently guide their search behaviour. Individuals with a greater visuo-spatial short-term memory span performed the task more quickly than those with a lower span. However, search latencies were unrelated to general fluid intelligence. This highlights the role of spatial working memory in the development of efficient exploration of large-scale space.


Assuntos
Comportamento Infantil/psicologia , Percepção Visual , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Fatores de Tempo , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
20.
Dev Sci ; 7(3): 277-82, 2004 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15595368

RESUMO

When preschoolers overcome persistent error, subsequent patterns of correct choices may identify how the error had been overcome. Children who no longer misrepresented a ball rolling down a bent tube as though it could only fall vertically, were asked sometimes to approach and sometimes to avoid where the ball landed. All children showed requisite task-switching flexibility. The pattern of 4-year-olds' correct choices among different places showed unnecessary avoidance of any place that would previously have tempted them into a vertical-approach error, 5-year-olds rebounded into a reversal, and 7-year-olds were flexible. The data attest to an inhibition mechanism, ruling out competing possibilities.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha , Cognição , Aprendizagem , Percepção Visual , Criança , Comportamento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Formação de Conceito , Sinais (Psicologia) , Comportamento Exploratório , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicologia da Criança , Tempo de Reação
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA