Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Neurophysiol ; 132(2): 335-346, 2024 Aug 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38865580

RESUMO

Saccade adaptation plays a crucial role in maintaining saccade accuracy. The behavioral characteristics and neural mechanisms of saccade adaptation for an externally cued movement, such as visually guided saccades (VGS), are well studied in nonhuman primates. In contrast, little is known about the saccade adaptation of an internally driven movement, such as memory-guided saccades (MGS), which are guided by visuospatial working memory. As the oculomotor plant changes because of growth, aging, or skeletomuscular problems, both types of saccades need to be adapted. Do both saccade types engage a common adaptation mechanism? In this study, we compared the characteristics of amplitude decrease adaptation in MGS with VGS in nonhuman primates. We found that the adaptation speed was faster for MGS than for VGS. Saccade duration changed during MGS adaptation, whereas saccade peak velocity changed during VGS adaptation. We also compared the adaptation field, that is, the gain change for saccade amplitudes other than the adapted. The gain change for MGS declines on both smaller and larger sides of adapted amplitude, more rapidly for larger than smaller amplitudes, whereas the decline in VGS was reversed. Thus, the differences between VGS and MGS adaptation characteristics support the previously suggested hypothesis that the adaptation mechanisms of VGS and MGS are distinct. Furthermore, the result suggests that the MGS adaptation site is a brain structure that influences saccade duration, whereas the VGS adaptation site influences saccade peak velocity. These results should be beneficial for future neurophysiological experiments.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Plasticity helps to overcome persistent motor errors. Such motor plasticity or adaptation can be investigated with saccades. Thus far our knowledge is primarily about visually guided saccades, an externally cued movement, which we can make only when the object is visible at the time of saccade. However, as the world is complex, we can make saccades even when the object is not visible. Here, we investigate the adaptation of an internally driven movement: the memory-guided saccade.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Macaca mulatta , Movimentos Sacádicos , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Animais , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Masculino , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Feminino , Memória/fisiologia
2.
Brain Struct Funct ; 229(8): 1855-1871, 2024 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38240754

RESUMO

Saccade accommodation is a productive model for exploring the role of the cerebellum in behavioral plasticity. In this model, the target is moved during the saccade, gradually inducing a change in the saccade vector as the animal adapts. The climbing fiber pathway from the inferior olive provides a visual error signal generated by the superior colliculus that is believed to be crucial for cerebellar adaptation. However, the primate tecto-olivary pathway has only been explored using large injections of the central portion of the superior colliculus. To provide a more detailed picture, we have made injections of anterograde tracers into various regions of the macaque superior colliculus. As shown previously, large central injections primarily label a dense terminal field within the C subdivision at caudal end of the contralateral medial inferior olive. Several, previously unobserved, sites of sparse terminal labeling were noted: bilaterally in the dorsal cap of Kooy and ipsilaterally in the C subdivision of the medial inferior olive. Small, physiologically directed, injections into the rostral, small saccade portion of the superior colliculus produced terminal fields in the same regions of the medial inferior olive, but with decreased density. Small injections of the caudal superior colliculus, where large amplitude gaze changes are encoded, again labeled a terminal field located in the same areas. The lack of a topographic pattern within the main tecto-olivary projection suggests that either the precise vector of the visual error is not transmitted to the vermis, or that encoding of this error is via non-topographic means.


Assuntos
Núcleo Olivar , Movimentos Sacádicos , Colículos Superiores , Animais , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia , Núcleo Olivar/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
3.
Int Forum Allergy Rhinol ; 14(7): 1206-1217, 2024 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38268115

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Patients with chronic rhinosinusitis (CRS) can experience cognitive dysfunction. The literature on this topic mostly reflects patient-reported measurements. Our goal was to assess cognitive function in patients with CRS using objective measures, including saccadic eye movements-a behavioral response reflecting cognitive and sensory information integration that is often compromised in conditions with impaired cognition. METHODS: Participants (N = 24 with CRS, N = 23 non-CRS healthy controls) enrolled from rhinology clinic underwent sinonasal evaluation, quality of life assessment (Sino-nasal Outcome Test 22 [SNOT-22]), and cognitive assessment with the Neuro-QOL Cognitive Function-Short Form, the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and recording of eye movements using video-oculography. RESULTS: Participants with CRS were more likely to report cognitive dysfunction (Neuro-QOL; 45.8% vs. 8.7%; p = 0.005) and demonstrate mild or greater cognitive impairment (MoCA; 41.7% vs. 8.7%; p = 0.005) than controls. Additionally, participants with CRS performed worse on the MoCA overall and within the executive functioning and memory domains (all p < 0.05) and on the anti-saccade (p = 0.014) and delay saccade (p = 0.044) eye movement tasks. Poorer performance on the MoCA (r = -0.422; p = 0.003) and the anti-saccade (r = -0.347; p = 0.017) and delay saccade (r = -0.419; p = 0.004) eye movement tasks correlated with worse CRS severity according to SNOT-22 scores. CONCLUSION: This study is the first to utilize objective eye movement assessments in addition to researcher-administered cognitive testing in patients with CRS. These patients demonstrated a high prevalence of cognitive dysfunction, most notably within executive functioning and memory domains, with the degree of dysfunction correlating with the severity of CRS.


Assuntos
Cognição , Disfunção Cognitiva , Rinite , Movimentos Sacádicos , Sinusite , Humanos , Sinusite/fisiopatologia , Sinusite/psicologia , Rinite/fisiopatologia , Doença Crônica , Masculino , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Feminino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto , Disfunção Cognitiva/fisiopatologia , Qualidade de Vida , Idoso , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Rinossinusite
4.
Front Neurol ; 14: 1198274, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37780695

RESUMO

Introduction: Loss of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) affects visual acuity during head movements. Patients with unilateral and bilateral vestibular deficits often use saccadic eye movements to compensate for an inadequate VOR. Two types of compensatory saccades have been distinguished, covert saccades and overt saccades. Covert saccades occur during head rotation, whereas overt saccades occur after the head has stopped moving. The generation of covert saccades is part of a central vestibular compensation process that improves visual acuity and suppresses oscillopsia. Understanding the covert saccade mechanism may facilitate vestibular rehabilitation strategies that can improve the patient's quality of life. To understand the brain mechanisms underlying covert saccades at the neural level, studies in an animal model are necessary. In this study, we employed non-human primates whose vestibular end organs are injured. Methods: We examined eye movement during the head-impulse test, which is a clinical test to evaluate the vestibulo-ocular reflex. During this test, the monkeys are required to fixate on a target and the head is rapidly and unexpectedly rotated to stimulate the horizontal semi-circular canals. Results: Similar to human subjects, monkeys made compensatory saccades. We compared these saccades with catch-up saccades following a moving target that simulates the visual conditions during the head impulse test. The shortest latency of the catch-up saccades was 250 ms, which indicates that it requires at least 250 ms to induce saccades by a visual signal. The latency of some compensatory saccades is shorter than 250 ms during the head impulse test, suggesting that such short latency compensatory saccades were not induced visually. The peak velocity of the short latency saccades was significantly lower than that of longer latency saccades. The peak velocity of these longer latency saccades was closer to that of visually guided saccades induced by a stepping target. Conclusion: These results are consistent with studies in human patients. Thus, this study demonstrates, for the first time, compensatory covert saccades in vestibular impaired monkeys.

6.
eNeuro ; 10(9)2023 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37596048

RESUMO

When movements become inaccurate, the resultant error induces motor adaptation to improve accuracy. This error-based motor learning is regarded as a cerebellar function. However, the influence of the other brain areas on adaptation is poorly understood. During saccade adaptation, a type of error-based motor learning, the superior colliculus (SC) sends a postsaccadic error signal to the cerebellum to drive adaptation. Since the SC is directly inhibited by the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr), we hypothesized that the SNr might influence saccade adaptation by affecting the SC error signal. In fact, previous studies indicated that the SNr encodes motivation and motivation influences saccade adaptation. In this study, we first established that the SNr projects to the rostral SC, where small error signals are generated, in nonhuman primates. Then, we examined SNr activity while the animal underwent adaptation. SNr neurons paused their activity in association with the error. This pause was shallower and delayed compared with those of no-error trial saccades. The pause at the end of the adaptation was shallower and delayed compared with that at the beginning of the adaptation. The change in the intertrial interval, an indicator of motivation, and adaptation speed had a positive correlation with the changes in the error-related pause. These results suggest that (1) the SNr exhibits a unique activity pattern during the error interval; (2) SNr activity increases during adaptation, consistent with the decrease in SC activity; and (3) motivational decay during the adaptation session might increase SNr activity and influence the adaptation speed.


Assuntos
Parte Reticular da Substância Negra , Animais , Movimentos Sacádicos , Colículos Superiores , Encéfalo , Cerebelo
7.
Res Sq ; 2023 May 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37398093

RESUMO

Saccade accommodation is a productive model for exploring the role of the cerebellum in behavioral plasticity. In this model, the target is moved during the saccade, gradually inducing a change in the saccade vector as the animal adapts. The climbing fiber pathway from the inferior olive provides a visual error signal generated by the superior colliculus that is believed to be crucial for cerebellar adaptation. However, the primate tecto-olivary pathway has only been explored using large injections of the central portion of the superior colliculus. To provide a more detailed picture, we have made injections of anterograde tracers into various regions of the macaque superior colliculus. As shown previously, large central injections primarily label a dense terminal field within the C subdivision at caudal end of the contralateral medial inferior olive. Several, previously unobserved, sites of sparse terminal labeling were noted: bilaterally in the dorsal cap of Kooy and ipsilaterally in C subdivision of the medial inferior olive. Small, physiologically directed, injections into the rostral, small saccade portion of the superior colliculus produced terminal fields in the same regions of the medial inferior olive, but with decreased density. Small injections of the caudal superior colliculus, where large amplitude gaze changes are encoded, again labeled a terminal field located in the same areas. The lack of a topographic pattern within the main tecto-olivary projection suggests that either the precise vector of the visual error is not transmitted to the vermis, or that encoding of this error is via non-topographic means.

8.
J Vis Exp ; (174)2021 08 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34424236

RESUMO

Optogenetic techniques have revolutionized neuroscience research and are poised to do the same for neurological gene therapy. The clinical use of optogenetics, however, requires that safety and efficacy be demonstrated in animal models, ideally in non-human primates (NHPs), because of their neurological similarity to humans. The number of candidate vectors that are potentially useful for neuroscience and medicine is vast, and no high-throughput means to test these vectors yet exists. Thus, there is a need for techniques to make multiple spatially and volumetrically accurate injections of viral vectors into NHP brain that can be identified unambiguously through postmortem histology. Described herein is such a method. Injection cannulas are constructed from coupled polytetrafluoroethylene and stainless-steel tubes. These cannulas are autoclavable, disposable, and have low minimal-loading volumes, making them ideal for the injection of expensive, highly concentrated viral vector solutions. An inert, red-dyed mineral oil fills the dead space and forms a visible meniscus with the vector solution, allowing instantaneous and accurate measurement of injection rates and volumes. The oil is loaded into the rear of the cannula, reducing the risk of co-injection with the vector. Cannulas can be loaded in 10 min, and injections can be made in 20 min. This procedure is well suited for injections into awake or anesthetized animals. When used to deliver high-quality viral vectors, this procedure can produce robust expression of optogenetic proteins, allowing optical control of neural activity and behavior in NHPs.


Assuntos
Optogenética , Vigília , Animais , Encéfalo , Dependovirus/genética , Vetores Genéticos/genética , Primatas
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 126(4): 1055-1075, 2021 10 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34432996

RESUMO

Analysis of electrophysiological data from Purkinje cells (P-cells) of the cerebellum presents unique challenges to spike sorting. Complex spikes have waveforms that vary significantly from one event to the next, raising the problem of misidentification. Even when complex spikes are detected correctly, the simple spikes may belong to a different P-cell, raising the danger of misattribution. To address these identification and attribution problems, we wrote an open-source, semiautomated software called P-sort, and then tested it by analyzing data from P-cells recorded in three species: marmosets, macaques, and mice. Like other sorting software, P-sort relies on nonlinear dimensionality reduction to cluster spikes. However, it also uses the statistical relationship between simple and complex spikes to merge disparate clusters and split a single cluster. In comparison with expert manual curation, occasionally P-sort identified significantly more complex spikes, as well as prevented misattribution of clusters. Three existing automatic sorters performed less well, particularly for identification of complex spikes. To improve the development of analysis tools for the cerebellum, we provide labeled data for 313 recording sessions, as well as statistical characteristics of waveforms and firing patterns of P-cells in three species.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Algorithms that perform spike sorting depend on waveforms to cluster spikes. However, a cerebellar Purkinje-cell produces two types of spikes; simple and complex spikes. A complex spike coincides with the suppression of generating simple spikes. Here, we recorded neurophysiological data from three species and developed a spike analysis software named P-sort that relies on this statistical property to improve both the detection and the attribution of simple and complex spikes in the cerebellum.


Assuntos
Eletroencefalografia , Fenômenos Eletrofisiológicos/fisiologia , Células de Purkinje/fisiologia , Software , Animais , Callithrix , Eletroencefalografia/instrumentação , Eletroencefalografia/métodos , Feminino , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL
10.
Cell Rep ; 34(13): 108754, 2021 03 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33789096

RESUMO

Viral genetic tools that target specific brain cell types could transform basic neuroscience and targeted gene therapy. Here, we use comparative open chromatin analysis to identify thousands of human-neocortical-subclass-specific putative enhancers from across the genome to control gene expression in adeno-associated virus (AAV) vectors. The cellular specificity of reporter expression from enhancer-AAVs is established by molecular profiling after systemic AAV delivery in mouse. Over 30% of enhancer-AAVs produce specific expression in the targeted subclass, including both excitatory and inhibitory subclasses. We present a collection of Parvalbumin (PVALB) enhancer-AAVs that show highly enriched expression not only in cortical PVALB cells but also in some subcortical PVALB populations. Five vectors maintain PVALB-enriched expression in primate neocortex. These results demonstrate how genome-wide open chromatin data mining and cross-species AAV validation can be used to create the next generation of non-species-restricted viral genetic tools.


Assuntos
Elementos Facilitadores Genéticos , Regulação da Expressão Gênica , Neocórtex/metabolismo , Animais , Cromatina/genética , Cromatina/metabolismo , Bases de Dados Genéticas , Dependovirus/genética , Doença/genética , Epigênese Genética , Vetores Genéticos/metabolismo , Genoma , Humanos , Camundongos , Neurônios/metabolismo , Parvalbuminas/metabolismo , Primatas , Especificidade da Espécie
11.
eNeuro ; 8(2)2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33707204

RESUMO

The basal ganglia have long been considered crucial for associative learning, but whether they also are involved in another type of learning, error-based motor learning, is not clear. Error-based learning has been considered the province of the cerebellum. However, learning to use a robotic arm and saccade adaptation, which use error-based learning, are facilitated by motivation, which is a function of the basal ganglia. Additionally, patients with Parkinson's disease, a basal ganglia deficit, show slower saccade adaptation than age matched controls. To further investigate whether the basal ganglia actually influence error-based learning, we reversibly inactivated the oculomotor portion of the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr) in two monkeys and tested saccade adaptation. Here, we show that nigral inactivation affected saccade adaptation. In particular, the inactivation facilitated the amplitude decrease adaptation of ipsiversive saccades. Consistent with previous studies, no effect was seen on the amplitude of the ipsiversive saccades when we did not induce adaptation. Therefore, the facilitated adaptation was not caused by inactivation directly modulating ipsiversive saccades. On the other hand, the kinematics of corrective saccades, which represent error processing, were changed after the inactivation. Thus, our data suggest that the oculomotor SNr assists saccade adaptation by strengthening the error signal. This effect indicates the basal ganglia influence error-based motor learning, a previously unrecognized function.


Assuntos
Parte Reticular da Substância Negra , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adaptação Fisiológica , Animais , Movimentos Oculares , Haplorrinos , Humanos , Substância Negra
12.
Prog Brain Res ; 249: 169-181, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31325976

RESUMO

In 1980, Dr. Optican established the existence of an adaptive plasticity of saccades and its dependence on the cerebellum with Dr. Robinson. The advantage of saccades is that the neuronal mechanisms underlying their generation have been well established. This knowledge allows us to identify the neuronal elements that participate in saccade adaptation. Briefly, the superior colliculus (SC) produces a saccade command signal, which reaches motoneurons in the abducens nucleus via the brainstem burst generator. The SC saccade command also is sent to the oculomotor vermis (OMV), a saccade-related area of the cerebellar cortex, and finally converges on the same motoneurons via the caudal fastigial nucleus (cFN) and inhibitory burst neurons (IBN). During adaptation, the saccade-related burst of SC neurons does not change; however, the activity of the cerebellum and its downstream targets do. We demonstrate that the SC is the source of the error signal to the OMV, and the error signal increases the probability of complex spike occurrence and decreases simple spike activity in the OMV. This decrease, in turn, is delivered through the cFN and IBN neurons to decrease motoneuron activity and hence saccade amplitude.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Cerebelo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos , Primatas
13.
J Neurophysiol ; 121(6): 2153-2162, 2019 06 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30995136

RESUMO

The neuronal substrate underlying the learning of a sophisticated task has been difficult to study. However, the advent of a behavioral paradigm that deceives the saccadic system into thinking it is making an error has allowed the mechanisms of the adaptation that corrects this error to be revealed in a primate. The neural elements that fashion the command signal for the generation of accurate saccades involve subcortical structures in the brain stem and cerebellum. In this review we show that sites in both those structures also are involved with the gradual adaptation of saccade size, a form of motor learning. Pharmacological manipulation of the oculomotor vermis (lobules VIc and VII) impairs mechanisms that either increase or decrease saccade size during adaptation. The net saccade-related simple spike (SS) activity of its Purkinje cells is correlated with the changes in saccade characteristics that occur during adaptation. These changes in SS activity are driven by an error signal delivered over climbing fibers, which create complex spikes whose probability of occurrence reflects the motor error between the actual and desired saccade size. These climbing fibers originate in the part of the inferior olive that receives projections from the superior colliculus (SC). Disabling the SC prevents adaptation and stimulation of the SC just after a normal saccade produces a surrogate error signal that drives adaptation without an actual visual error. Therefore, the SC provides not only the initial command that generates a saccade, as shown by others, but also the error signal that ensures that saccades remain accurate.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Cerebelo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia , Animais , Humanos
14.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(38): E8987-E8995, 2018 09 18.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30185563

RESUMO

When movements become dysmetric, the resultant motor error induces a plastic change in the cerebellum to correct the movement, i.e., motor adaptation. Current evidence suggests that the error signal to the cerebellum is delivered by complex spikes originating in the inferior olive (IO). To prove a causal link between the IO error signal and motor adaptation, several studies blocked the IO, which, unfortunately, affected not only the adaptation but also the movement itself. We avoided this confound by inactivating the source of an error signal to the IO. Several studies implicate the superior colliculus (SC) as the source of the error signal to the IO for saccade adaptation. When we inactivated the SC, the metrics of the saccade to be adapted were unchanged, but saccade adaptation was impaired. Thus, an intact rostral SC is necessary for saccade adaptation. Our data provide experimental evidence for the cerebellar learning theory that requires an error signal to drive motor adaptation.


Assuntos
Cerebelo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia , Adaptação Fisiológica/efeitos dos fármacos , Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Animais , Eletrodos , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Muscimol/farmacologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos , Recompensa , Movimentos Sacádicos/efeitos dos fármacos
15.
Nat Neurosci ; 21(5): 736-743, 2018 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29662213

RESUMO

The primary output cells of the cerebellar cortex, Purkinje cells, make kinematic predictions about ongoing movements via high-frequency simple spikes, but receive sensory error information about that movement via low-frequency complex spikes (CS). How is the vector space of sensory errors encoded by this low-frequency signal? Here we measured Purkinje cell activity in the oculomotor vermis of animals during saccades, then followed the chain of events from experience of visual error, generation of CS, modulation of simple spikes, and ultimately change in motor output. We found that while error direction affected the probability of CS, error magnitude altered its temporal distribution. Production of CS changed the simple spikes on the next trial, but regardless of the actual visual error, this change biased the movement only along a vector that was parallel to the Purkinje cell's preferred error. From these results, we inferred the anatomy of a sensory-to-motor adaptive controller that transformed visual error vectors into motor-corrections.


Assuntos
Cerebelo/fisiologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Células de Purkinje/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Fenômenos Biomecânicos/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebelar/citologia , Córtex Cerebelar/fisiologia , Cerebelo/citologia , Fenômenos Eletrofisiológicos/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Nervo Oculomotor/citologia , Nervo Oculomotor/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos
16.
Sci Rep ; 7(1): 9566, 2017 08 29.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28852092

RESUMO

Saccadic eye movements provide a valuable model to study the brain mechanisms underlying motor learning. If a target is displaced surreptitiously while a saccade is underway, the saccade appears to be in error. If the error persists gradual neuronal adjustments cause the eye movement again to land near the target. This saccade adaptation typically follows an exponential time course, i.e., adaptation speed slows as adaptation progresses, indicating that the sensitivity to error decreases during adaptation. Previous studies suggested that the superior colliculus (SC) sends error signals to drive saccade adaptation. The objective of this study is to test whether the SC error signal is related to the decrease in the error sensitivity during adaptation. We show here that the visual activity of SC neurons, which is induced by a constant visual error that drives adaptation, decreases during saccade adaptation. This decrease of sensitivity to visual error was not correlated with the changes of primary saccade amplitude. Therefore, a possible interpretation of this result is that the reduction of visual sensitivity of SC neurons contributes an error sensitivity signal that could help control the saccade adaptation process.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Movimentos Sacádicos , Colículos Superiores/fisiologia , Percepção Visual , Potenciais de Ação , Animais , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Neurônios/fisiologia
17.
Neuron ; 95(1): 51-62.e4, 2017 Jul 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28648497

RESUMO

Purkinje cells of the primate cerebellum play critical but poorly understood roles in the execution of coordinated, accurate movements. Elucidating these roles has been hampered by a lack of techniques for manipulating spiking activity in these cells selectively-a problem common to most cell types in non-transgenic animals. To overcome this obstacle, we constructed AAV vectors carrying the channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) gene under the control of a 1 kb L7/Pcp2 promoter. We injected these vectors into the cerebellar cortex of rhesus macaques and tested vector efficacy in three ways. Immunohistochemical analyses confirmed selective ChR2 expression in Purkinje cells. Neurophysiological recordings confirmed robust optogenetic activation. Optical stimulation of the oculomotor vermis caused saccade dysmetria. Our results demonstrate the utility of AAV-L7-ChR2 for revealing the contributions of Purkinje cells to circuit function and behavior, and they attest to the feasibility of promoter-based, targeted, genetic manipulations in primates.


Assuntos
Potenciais de Ação/fisiologia , Vermis Cerebelar/fisiologia , Optogenética/métodos , Células de Purkinje/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Animais , Córtex Cerebelar/citologia , Córtex Cerebelar/fisiologia , Vermis Cerebelar/citologia , Cerebelo/citologia , Cerebelo/fisiologia , Dependovirus/genética , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Imuno-Histoquímica , Macaca mulatta , Células de Purkinje/citologia , Células de Purkinje/metabolismo , Rodopsina/genética , Rodopsina/metabolismo
18.
Neuroscience ; 355: 113-125, 2017 07 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28499971

RESUMO

In this study we tested whether a selective reward could affect the adaptation of saccadic eye movements in monkeys. We induced the adaptation of saccades by displacing the target of a horizontal saccade vertically as the eye moved toward it, thereby creating an apparent vertical dysmetria. The repeated upward target displacement caused the originally horizontal saccade to gradually deviate upward over the course of several hundred trials. We induced this directional adaptation in both right- and leftward saccades in every experiment (n=20). In half of the experiments (n=10), we rewarded monkeys only when they made leftward saccades and in the other half (n=10) only for rightward saccades. The reaction time of saccades in the rewarded direction was shorter and we, like others, interpreted this change as a sign of the reward's preferential effect in that direction. Saccades in the rewarded direction showed more rapid adaptation of their directions than did saccades in the non-rewarded direction, indicating that the selective reward increased the speed of saccade adaptation. The differences in adaptation speed were reflected in changes in saccade metrics, which were usually more noticeable in the deceleration phases of saccades than in their acceleration phases. Because previous studies have shown that the oculomotor cerebellum is involved with saccade deceleration and also participates in saccade adaptation, it is possible that selective reward could influence cerebellar plasticity.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica/fisiologia , Comportamento de Escolha/fisiologia , Recompensa , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Animais , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
19.
Nature ; 526(7573): 439-42, 2015 Oct 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26469054

RESUMO

Execution of accurate eye movements depends critically on the cerebellum, suggesting that the major output neurons of the cerebellum, Purkinje cells, may predict motion of the eye. However, this encoding of action for rapid eye movements (saccades) has remained unclear: Purkinje cells show little consistent modulation with respect to saccade amplitude or direction, and critically, their discharge lasts longer than the duration of a saccade. Here we analysed Purkinje-cell discharge in the oculomotor vermis of behaving rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and found neurons that increased or decreased their activity during saccades. We estimated the combined effect of these two populations via their projections to the caudal fastigial nucleus, and uncovered a simple-spike population response that precisely predicted the real-time motion of the eye. When we organized the Purkinje cells according to each cell's complex-spike directional tuning, the simple-spike population response predicted both the real-time speed and direction of saccade multiplicatively via a gain field. This suggests that the cerebellum predicts the real-time motion of the eye during saccades via the combined inputs of Purkinje cells onto individual nucleus neurons. A gain-field encoding of simple spikes emerges if the Purkinje cells that project onto a nucleus neuron are not selected at random but share a common complex-spike property.


Assuntos
Células de Purkinje/fisiologia , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Potenciais de Ação , Animais , Núcleos Cerebelares/citologia , Núcleos Cerebelares/fisiologia , Vermis Cerebelar/citologia , Vermis Cerebelar/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta , Modelos Neurológicos , Fatores de Tempo
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 114(1): 125-37, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25855693

RESUMO

Shifts in the direction of gaze are accomplished by different kinds of saccades, which are elicited under different circumstances. Saccade types include targeting saccades to simple jumping targets, delayed saccades to visible targets after a waiting period, memory-guided (MG) saccades to remembered target locations, scanning saccades to stationary target arrays, and express saccades after very short latencies. Studies of human cases and neurophysiological experiments in monkeys suggest that separate pathways, which converge on a common locus that provides the motor command, generate these different types of saccade. When behavioral manipulations in humans cause targeting saccades to have persistent dysmetrias as might occur naturally from growth, aging, and injury, they gradually adapt to reduce the dysmetria. Although results differ slightly between laboratories, this adaptation generalizes or transfers to all the other saccade types mentioned above. Also, when one of the other types of saccade undergoes adaptation, it often transfers to another saccade type. Similar adaptation and transfer experiments, which allow inferences to be drawn about the site(s) of adaptation for different saccade types, have yet to be done in monkeys. Here we show that simian targeting and MG saccades adapt more than express, scanning, and delayed saccades. Adaptation of targeting saccades transfers to all the other saccade types. However, the adaptation of MG saccades transfers only to delayed saccades. These data suggest that adaptation of simian targeting saccades occurs on the pathway common to all saccade types. In contrast, only the delayed saccade command passes through the adaptation site of the MG saccade.


Assuntos
Adaptação Fisiológica , Movimentos Sacádicos , Transferência de Experiência , Adaptação Psicológica , Animais , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA