Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 10 de 10
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 237(4): 977-987, 2019 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30694342

RESUMO

Functional behaviour affords that we form goals to integrate sensory information about the world around us with suitable motor actions, such as when we plan to grab an object with a hand. However, much research has tested grasping in static scenarios where goals are pursued with repetitive movements, whereas dynamic contexts require goals to be pursued even when changes in the environment require a change in the actions to attain them. To study grasp goals in dynamic environments here, we employed a task where the goal remained the same but the execution of the movement changed; we primed participants to grasp objects either with their right or left hand, and occasionally they had to switch to grasping with both. Switch costs should be minimal if grasp goal representations were used continuously, for example, within the left dominant hemisphere. But remapped or re-computed goal representations should delay movements. We found that switching from right-hand grasping to bimanual grasping delayed reaction times but switching from left-hand grasping to bimanual grasping did not. Further, control experiments showed that the lateralized switch costs were not caused by asymmetric inhibition between hemispheres or switches between usual and unusual tasks. Our results show that the left hemisphere does not serve a general role of sensorimotor grasp goal representation. Instead, sensorimotor grasp goals appear to be represented at intermediate levels of abstraction, downstream from cognitive task representations, yet upstream from the control of the grasping effectors.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Objetivos , Mãos/fisiologia , Atividade Motora/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
2.
Neuroreport ; 29(16): 1355-1359, 2018 11 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30119106

RESUMO

Recent findings suggest that the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), a cortical area in the dorsomedial pathway, is involved in grasp control. It is unclear, however, whether human PMd transfers grasp-related information to the primary motor cortex hand area (M1HAND) during action preparation. The present study tested whether ipsilateral cortico-cortical connections between PMd and M1HAND in the left hemisphere are modulated during grasp preparation. Ten participants performed object-directed grasps and reaches with the right hand. Functional connectivity between left PMd and ipsilateral M1HAND was probed with dual-site transcranial magnetic stimulation. We found that PMd-M1HAND functional interactions were facilitated selectively for the muscles involved in the preparation of the upcoming grasps. The PMd-M1HAND interaction was facilitated for first dorsal interosseous muscle for both precision grip and whole-hand grasps and for abductor digiti minimi muscle for whole-hand grasps. We conclude that human dorsomedial PMd-M1HAND circuit encodes handgrip formation during grasp preparation.


Assuntos
Potencial Evocado Motor/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana/métodos , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
3.
J Neurophysiol ; 117(2): 624-636, 2017 02 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27832593

RESUMO

Skillful interaction with the world requires that the brain uses a multitude of sensorimotor programs and subroutines, such as for reaching, grasping, and the coordination of the two body halves. However, it is unclear how these programs operate together. Networks for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination might converge in common brain areas. For example, Brodmann area 7 (BA7) is known to activate in disparate tasks involving the three types of movements separately. Here, we asked whether BA7 plays a key role in integrating coordinated reach-to-grasp movements for both arms together. To test this, we applied transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to disrupt BA7 activity in the left and right hemispheres, while human participants performed a bimanual size-perturbation grasping task using the index and middle fingers of both hands to grasp a rectangular object whose orientation (and thus grasp-relevant width dimension) might or might not change. We found that TMS of the right BA7 during object perturbation disrupted the bimanual grasp and transport/coordination components, and TMS over the left BA7 disrupted unimanual grasps. These results show that right BA7 is causally involved in the integration of reach-to-grasp movements of the two arms. NEW & NOTEWORTHY: Our manuscript describes a role of human Brodmann area 7 (BA7) in the integration of multiple visuomotor programs for reaching, grasping, and bimanual coordination. Our results are the first to suggest that right BA7 is critically involved in the coordination of reach-to-grasp movements of the two arms. The results complement previous reports of right-hemisphere lateralization for bimanual grasps.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Amplitude de Movimento Articular/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Cinética , Masculino , Movimento , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto Jovem
4.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 16(5): 848-65, 2016 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27215614

RESUMO

Fulfillment of the basic psychological needs for competence, relatedness, and autonomy is believed to facilitate people's integrative tendencies to process psychological conflicts and develop a coherent sense of self. The present study therefore used event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine the relation between need fulfillment and the amplitude of conflict negativity (CN), a neurophysiological measure of conflict during personal decision making. Participants completed a decision-making task in which they made a series of forced choices according to their personal preferences. Three types of decision-making situations were created on the basis of participants' unique preference ratings, which were obtained prior to ERP recording: low-conflict situations (choosing between an attractive and an unattractive option), high-conflict approach-approach situations (choosing between two similarly attractive options), and high-conflict avoidance-avoidance situations (choosing between two similarly unattractive options). As expected, CN amplitudes were larger in high- relative to low-conflict situations, and source localization analyses suggested that the anterior cingulate cortex was the generating structure of the CN. Most importantly, people reporting higher need fulfillment exhibited larger CN amplitudes in avoidance-avoidance situations relative to low-conflict situations; to a lesser extent, they also exhibited larger CN amplitudes in approach-approach situations relative to low-conflict situations. By contrast, people reporting lower need fulfillment exhibited CN amplitudes that poorly discriminated the three decision situations. These results suggest that need fulfillment may promote self-coherent functioning by increasing people's receptivity to and processing of events that challenge their abilities to make efficient, self-congruent choices.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Conflito Psicológico , Tomada de Decisões/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Ocupações , Tempo de Reação , Autorrelato , Adulto Jovem
5.
Cortex ; 64: 169-78, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25461717

RESUMO

Major clues to the human brain mechanisms of spatial attention and visual awareness have come from the syndrome of neglect, where patients ignore one half of space. A longstanding puzzle, though, is that neglect almost always comes from right-hemisphere damage, which suggests that the two sides of the brain play distinct roles. But tests of attention in healthy people have revealed only slight differences between the hemispheres. Here we show that major differences emerge if we look at the timing of brain activity in a task optimized to identify attentional functions. Using EEG to map cortical activity on a millisecond timescale, we found transient (20-30 ms) periods of interhemispheric competition, followed by short phases of marked right-sided activity in the ventral attentional network. Our data are the first to show interhemispheric interactions that, much like a toggle switch, quickly allocate neural resources to one or the other hemisphere.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Conscientização/fisiologia , Córtex Cerebral/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
6.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 782, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25324766

RESUMO

When we grasp an object using one hand, the opposite hemisphere predominantly guides the motor control of grasp movements (Davare et al., 2007; Rice et al., 2007). However, it is unclear whether visual object analysis for grasp control relies more on inputs (a) from the contralateral than the ipsilateral visual field, (b) from one dominant visual field regardless of the grasping hand, or (c) from both visual fields equally. For bimanual grasping of a single object we have recently demonstrated a visual field preference for the left visual field (Le and Niemeier, 2013a,b), consistent with a general right-hemisphere dominance for sensorimotor control of bimanual grasps (Le et al., 2014). But visual field differences have never been tested for unimanual grasping. Therefore, here we asked right-handed participants to fixate to the left or right of an object and then grasp the object either with their right or left hand using a precision grip. We found that participants grasping with their right hand performed better with objects in the right visual field: maximum grip apertures (MGAs) were more closely matched to the object width and were smaller than for objects in the left visual field. In contrast, when people grasped with their left hand, preferences switched to the left visual field. What is more, MGA scaling with the left hand showed greater visual field differences compared to right-hand grasping. Our data suggest that, visual object analysis for unimanual grasping shows a preference for visual information from the ipsilateral visual field, and that the left hemisphere is better equipped to control grasps in both visual fields.

7.
Cereb Cortex ; 24(10): 2591-603, 2014 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23645719

RESUMO

Grasping with 2 limbs in opposition to one another is older than the hand, yet the neural mechanisms for bimanual grasps remain unclear. Similar to unimanual grasping, bimanual grasping may require regions in the parietal cortex that use visual object-feature information to find matching stable grasp points on the object. The localization of matching points is computationally expensive, so it might make sense for the signals to converge in a single cortical area. To examine this, we use transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to probe the contribution of cortical areas known to be associated with unimanual grasping, while participants performed bimanual grasps. We applied TMS to the anterior and caudal portion of the intra-parietal sulcus (aIPS and cIPS) in each hemisphere during a size-perturbation task using the index fingers of both hands to grasp an object whose orientation might or might not change. We found significant interaction effects between TMS and perturbation of the grasp-relevant object dimension that increased grip aperture only for the right aIPS. These results indicate that the aIPS is involved not only in unimanual, but also bimanual grasping, and the right aIPS is critically involved in bimanual grasps. This suggests that information from both hemispheres converges in the right hemisphere to achieve bimanual grasps.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Magnética Transcraniana , Adulto Jovem
8.
Exp Brain Res ; 230(2): 187-96, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23857170

RESUMO

Grasping using two forelimbs in opposition to one another is evolutionary older than the hand with an opposable thumb (Whishaw and Coles in Behav Brain Res 77:135-148, 1996); yet, the mechanisms for bimanual grasps remain unclear. Similar to unimanual grasping, the localization of matching stable grasp points on an object is computationally expensive and so it makes sense for the signals to converge in a single cortical hemisphere. Indeed, bimanual grasps are faster and more accurate in the left visual field, and are disrupted if there is transcranial stimulation of the right hemisphere (Le and Niemeier in Exp Brain Res 224:263-273, 2013; Le et al. in Cereb Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht115, 2013). However, research so far has tested the right hemisphere dominance based on small objects only, which are usually grasped with one hand, whereas bimanual grasping is more commonly used for objects that are too big for a single hand. Because grasping large objects might involve different neural circuits than grasping small objects (Grol et al. in J Neurosci 27:11877-11887, 2007), here we tested whether a left visual field/right hemisphere dominance for bimanual grasping exists with large and thus more ecologically valid objects or whether the right hemisphere dominance is a function of object size. We asked participants to fixate to the left or right of an object and to grasp the object with the index and middle fingers of both hands. Consistent with previous observations, we found that for objects in the left visual field, the maximum grip apertures were scaled closer to the object width and were smaller and less variable, than for objects in the right visual field. Our results demonstrate that bimanual grasping is predominantly controlled by the right hemisphere, even in the context of grasping larger objects.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Movimento/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 224(2): 263-73, 2013 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23109083

RESUMO

To find points on the surface of an object that ensure a stable grasp, it would be most effective to employ one area in one cortical hemisphere. But grasping the object with both hands requires control through both hemispheres. To better understand the control mechanisms underlying this "bimanual grasping", here we examined how the two hemispheres coordinate their control processes for bimanual grasping depending on visual field. We asked if bimanual grasping involves both visual fields equally or one more than the other. To test this, participants fixated either to the left or right of an object and then grasped or pushed it off a pedestal. We found that when participants grasped the object in the right visual field, maximum grip aperture (MGA) was larger and more variable, and participants were slower to react and to show MGA compared to when they grasped the object in the left visual field. In contrast, when participants pushed the object we observed no comparable visual field effects. These results suggest that grasping with both hands, specifically the computation of grasp points on the object, predominantly involves the right hemisphere. Our study provides new insights into the interactions of the two hemispheres for grasping.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fatores de Tempo , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
10.
Cortex ; 47(5): 547-56, 2011 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20447620

RESUMO

Using a gratingscales task as a sensitive measure of the attentional bias, we have recently observed a new form of frequency-specific cross-over; people showed left-biased preferences when comparing the high spatial frequency (HiSF) components of the task and rightward biases when comparing low spatial frequencies (LoSFs). Here we investigated which mechanisms underlie the cross-over. (1) We found that leftward and rightward biases were positively correlated, suggesting that the same set of mechanisms are involved in both versions of the task. (2) When we cued attention to the left or right side we found transient effects on gratingscales biases that were symmetrical for the LoSF condition but asymmetrical for the HiSF condition. This indicates that the HiSF condition itself biased stimulus-driven attention more to the left side than the LoSF condition. (3) When we lowered the contrast of the HiSF or the LoSF stimulus components, specifically the latter case made HiSF and LoSF conditions more different. This suggests that HiSF and LoSF conditions differ because HiSF components are more salient and more likely stir stimulus-driven attention. Our data are consistent with the idea that the attentional bias results from right-dominant control mechanisms of stimulus-driven attention potentially interacting with voluntary control mechanisms.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Análise de Variância , Sensibilidades de Contraste/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Estatísticos , Rede Nervosa/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA