Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 73
Filtrar
1.
J Pers ; 69(4): 511-35, 2001 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11497029

RESUMO

The phenotypic structure of personality traits has been well described, but it has not yet been explained causally. Behavior genetic covariance analyses can identify the underlying causes of phenotypic structure; previous behavior genetic research has suggested that the effects from both genetic and nonshared environmental influences mirror the phenotype. However, nonshared environmental effects are usually estimated as a residualterm that may also include systematic bias, such as that introduced by implicit personality theory. To reduce that bias, we supplemented data from Canadian and German twin studies with cross-observer correlations on the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. The hypothesized five-factor structure was found in both the phenotypic and genetic/familial covariances. When the residual covariance was decomposed into true nonshared environmental influences and method bias, only the latter showed the five-factor structure. True nonshared environmental influences are not structured as genetic influences are, although there was some suggestion that they do affect two personality dimensions, Conscientiousness and Love. These data reaffirm the value of behavior genetic analyses for research on the underlying causes of personality traits.


Assuntos
Meio Ambiente , Personalidade/genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Canadá , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Genética Comportamental/estatística & dados numéricos , Alemanha , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Fenótipo
2.
J Pers ; 69(2): 155-74, 2001 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11339794

RESUMO

Personality disorders (PDs) are usually construed as psychiatric categories characterized by a unique configuration of traits and behaviors. To generate clinical hypotheses from normal personality trait scores, profile agreement statistics can be calculated using a prototypical personality profile for each PD. Multimethod data from 1,909 psychiatric patients in the People's Republic of China were used to examine the accuracy of such hypotheses in the Interpretive Report of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Profile agreement indices from both self-reports and spouse ratings were significantly related to PD symptom scores derived from questionnaires and clinical interviews. However, accuracy of diagnostic classification was only modest to moderate, probably because PDs are not discrete categorical entities. Together with other literature, these data suggest that the current categorical system should be replaced by a more comprehensive system of personality traits and personality-related problems.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Personalidade/diagnóstico , Adolescente , China/epidemiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Personalidade/epidemiologia , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Prevalência , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Autoavaliação (Psicologia) , Inquéritos e Questionários
3.
J Pers ; 69(6): 819-46, 2001 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11767820

RESUMO

Personality traits, studied for decades by Western personality psychologists, have recently been reconceptualized as endogenous basic tendencies that, within a cultural context, give rise to habits, attitudes, skills, beliefs, and other characteristic adaptations. This conceptualization provides a new framework for studying personality and culture at three levels. Transcultural research focuses on identifying human universals, such as trait structure and development; intracultural studies examine the unique expression of traits in specific cultures; and intercultural research characterizes cultures and their subgroups in terms of mean levels of personality traits and seeks associations between cultural variables and aggregate personality traits. As an example of the problems and possibilities of intercultural analyses, data on mean levels of Revised NEO Personality Inventory scales from college age and adult samples (N = 23,031) of men and women from 26 cultures are examined. Results showed that age and gender differences resembled those found in American samples; different subsamples from each culture showed similar levels of personality traits: intercultural factor analysis yielded a close approximation to the Five-Factor Model; and factor scores were meaningfully related to other culture-level variables. However, mean trait levels were not apparent to expert raters, casting doubt on the accuracy of national stereotypes. Trait psychology can serve as a useful complement to cultural perspectives on human nature and personality.


Assuntos
Cultura , Personalidade , Adulto , Comparação Transcultural , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Inventário de Personalidade , Temperamento
4.
J Pers ; 68(6): 1233-52, 2000 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11130739

RESUMO

Studies of personality and problem behaviors may begin with analyses of the problem and develop hypotheses about personality traits that might be relevant; or they may begin with models of personality and explore links to behavior. Because it is well validated and relatively comprehensive, the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality lends itself to systematic exploratory studies that may sometimes lead to unanticipated findings. In this article, we review a program of research in a high-risk, disadvantaged population that illustrates the utility of the FFM in understanding health risk behavior. Previous analyses showed that behavior associated with the risk of HIV infection can be predicted from the personality dispositions of Neuroticism and (low) Conscientiousness.


Assuntos
Infecções por HIV/psicologia , Transtornos Mentais/psicologia , Transtornos da Personalidade/psicologia , Assunção de Riscos , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Infecções por HIV/transmissão , Conhecimentos, Atitudes e Prática em Saúde , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Determinação da Personalidade , Transtornos da Personalidade/diagnóstico , Sexo Seguro
5.
J Pers Disord ; 14(3): 249-63, 2000.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11019748

RESUMO

We examined the reliability, cross-instrument validity, and factor structure of Chinese adaptations of the Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire (PDQ-4+; N = 1,926) and Personality Disorders Interview (PDI-IV; N = 525) in psychiatric patients. Comparisons with data from Western countries suggest that the psychometric properties of these two instruments are comparable across cultures. Low to modest agreement between the PDQ-4+ and PDI-IV was observed for both dimensional and categorical personality disorder evaluations. When the PDI-IV was used as the diagnostic standard, the PDQ-4+ showed higher sensitivity than specificity, and higher negative predictive power than positive predictive power. Factor analyses of both instruments replicated the four-factor structure O'Connor and Dyce (1998) found in Western samples. Results suggested that conceptions and measures of DSM-IV personality disorders are cross-culturally generalizable to Chinese psychiatric populations.


Assuntos
Características Culturais , Transtornos da Personalidade/diagnóstico , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica/normas , Adulto , China , Comparação Transcultural , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos Mentais/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Personalidade/etnologia , Transtornos da Personalidade/psicologia , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Psicometria , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Estados Unidos
6.
Am Psychol ; 55(6): 620-5, 2000 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10892204

RESUMO

Coping is among the most widely studied topics in contemporary psychology. However, the explosion of interest in coping has yielded little and the field is in crisis. This section offers a survey of the state of the art in theory and research on stress and adaptational processes. The four core articles in the section take up, respectively, problems in research design, the neglect of unconscious reactions to stress, the selection of adaptational outcomes, and the link between research on adaptational processes and clinical practice. The final article by Richard S. Lazarus offers a commentary. The present introduction provides the historical backdrop for the section. Then, after a brief overview of research on adaptational processes, the authors summarize the scope and yield of coping research and preview the four core articles in the section. The strengths and limits of individual coping efforts and the need for realistic expectations and redoubled efforts are discussed.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Humanos , Pesquisa
7.
Am J Psychiatry ; 157(8): 1285-90, 2000 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10910792

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: It has been reported that the human temperament dimensions of novelty seeking and harm avoidance are associated with polymorphisms in the D(4) dopamine receptor gene (D4DR) and the serotonin-transporter-linked promoter region (5-HTTLPR), respectively. Although these findings are consistent with Cloninger's hypothesized psychobiological model of temperament and character, many studies failed to replicate these findings. In the present study the authors tested whether the psychobiological model taps the genetic architecture of personality by exploring associations between these candidate genes and the dimensions of the Temperament and Character Inventory and by examining its phenotypic structure. METHOD: Of the 946 male and female participants in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging to whom the Temperament and Character Inventory was administered, 587 were genotyped for a polymorphism with a 48-base-pair repeat in the D4DR gene and 425 were genotyped for a 44-base-pair insertion or deletion in the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism. RESULTS: There was no significant association between D4DR polymorphisms and novelty seeking. The authors also failed to find an association between 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms and harm avoidance. The factor structure of the Temperament and Character Inventory did not reveal the hypothesized phenotypic structure. CONCLUSIONS: This investigation produced no support for the temperament-character model at either the biological or psychological level.


Assuntos
Proteínas de Transporte/genética , Caráter , Glicoproteínas de Membrana/genética , Proteínas de Membrana Transportadoras , Proteínas do Tecido Nervoso , Inventário de Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Personalidade/genética , Receptores de Dopamina D2/genética , Serotonina/genética , Temperamento , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Genótipo , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Genéticos , Modelos Psicológicos , Personalidade/classificação , Determinação da Personalidade/estatística & dados numéricos , Fenótipo , Polimorfismo Genético , Regiões Promotoras Genéticas/genética , Receptores de Dopamina D4 , Proteínas da Membrana Plasmática de Transporte de Serotonina
8.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 78(3): 582-93, 2000 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10743882

RESUMO

Because of the potential for bias and error in questionnaire responding, many personality inventories include validity scales intended to correct biased scores or identify invalid protocols. The authors evaluated the utility of several types of validity scales in a volunteer sample of 72 men and 106 women who completed the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; P. T. Costa & R. R. McCrae, 1992) and the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ; A. Tellegen, 1978/1982) and were rated by 2 acquaintances on the observer form of the NEO-PI-R. Analyses indicated that the validity indexes lacked utility in this sample. A partial replication (N = 1,728) also failed to find consistent support for the use of validity scales. The authors illustrate the use of informant ratings in assessing protocol validity and argue that psychological assessors should limit their use of validity scales and seek instead to improve the quality of personality assessments.


Assuntos
Inventário de Personalidade , Psicometria/métodos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Viés , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise de Regressão
9.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 78(1): 173-86, 2000 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10653513

RESUMO

Temperaments are often regarded as biologically based psychological tendencies with intrinsic paths of development. It is argued that this definition applies to the personality traits of the five-factor model. Evidence for the endogenous nature of traits is summarized from studies of behavior genetics, parent-child relations, personality structure, animal personality, and the longitudinal stability of individual differences. New evidence for intrinsic maturation is offered from analyses of NEO Five-Factor Inventory scores for men and women age 14 and over in German, British, Spanish, Czech, and Turkish samples (N = 5,085). These data support strong conceptual links to child temperament despite modest empirical associations. The intrinsic maturation of personality is complemented by the culturally conditioned development of characteristic adaptations that express personality; interventions in human development are best addressed to these.


Assuntos
Características Culturais , Desenvolvimento da Personalidade , Personalidade , Temperamento , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Comparação Transcultural , República Tcheca , Feminino , Genética Comportamental , Alemanha , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Testes Psicológicos , Espanha , Turquia , Reino Unido
11.
Assessment ; 7(4): 365-78, 2000 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11151962

RESUMO

Although developmental theories and popular accounts suggest that midlife is a time of turmoil and change, longitudinal studies of personality traits have generally found stability of rank order and little or no change in mean levels. Using data from 2,274 men and women in their 40s retested after 6 to 9 years, the present study examined two hypotheses: (a) that retest correlations should be no higher than about .60 and (b) that there should be small decreases in Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness, and small increases in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. The study also explored the effects of recalled life events on subsequent personality scores. Results did not support the first hypothesis; uncorrected retest correlations uniformly exceeded .60. This was true for all personality traits, including facets of Agreeableness and Conscientiousness not previously included in longitudinal studies. The hypothesized decreases in Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness were found, but Conscientiousness showed a small decrease instead of the predicted increase. Life events in general showed very little influence on the levels of personality traits, although some effects were seen for changes in job and marital status that warrant further research.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Desenvolvimento Humano , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Inventário de Personalidade , Personalidade , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Estado Civil , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , North Carolina , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
12.
Assessment ; 7(4): 379-88, 2000 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11151963

RESUMO

The finding of personality stability in adulthood may be counterintuitive to people who perceive a great deal of change in their own personality. The purpose of this study is to determine whether self-reported perceived changes in personality are associated with actual changes based on a 6- to 9-year follow-up of 2,242 middle-aged male and female participants of the UNC Alumni Heart Study (UNCAHS). Respondents completed the Revised NEO Personality Inventory on two occasions and were asked to reflect back over a 6-year period and assess changes in their personality. The majority of respondents (n = 1,177; 52.5%) reported they had "stayed the same," while 863 (38.5%) reported they had "changed a little" and 202 (9%) reported they had "changed a good deal." Coefficients of personality profile agreement computed to evaluate global personality change for the three perceived change groups were essentially equivalent. Further, directional analyses of domain-specific changes in personality showed that perceived changes were weak predictors of residual gain scores. In an absolute sense, perceptions of stability or change were discordant in 8 of 15 (53%) comparisons. Self-perceptions of change are not an adequate substitute for objective assessments.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Personalidade , Autoimagem , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , North Carolina
13.
Dev Psychol ; 35(2): 466-77, 1999 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10082017

RESUMO

Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in the United States have shown consistent changes between college age and middle adulthood. There appear to be declines in 3 of the 5 major factors of personality--Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness--and increases in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. To examine cross-cultural generalizability of these findings, translations of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory were administered to samples in Germany, Italy, Portugal, Croatia, and South Korea (N = 7,363). Similar patterns of age differences were seen in each country, for both men and women. Common trends were also seen for the more specific traits that define the major factors. Because these nations differ substantially in culture and recent history, results suggest the hypothesis that these are universal maturational changes in adult personality.


Assuntos
Cultura , Desenvolvimento da Personalidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Croácia , Comparação Transcultural , Estudos Transversais , Feminino , Alemanha , Humanos , Itália , Coreia (Geográfico) , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Determinação da Personalidade , Portugal
14.
J Pers ; 67(6): 1209-18, 1999 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10637992

RESUMO

In this commentary, I make some general observations about the study of personality and religion and some specific comments about individual articles from the perspective of contemporary personality psychology. Most of the authors represented here treat religion as a domain of human experience and behavior that can be understood in terms of familiar personality principles and processes. I therefore urge greater attention to the Five-Factor Model of personality traits, especially Openness to Experience, in understanding religious phenomena. Mainstream psychologists, including longitudinal researchers, behavior geneticists, and epidemiologists, should consider the inclusion of religious variables in their research designs.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Personalidade , Psicologia , Religião e Psicologia , Humanos
15.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 53(6): P375-83, 1998 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9826970

RESUMO

Life experiences for corresponding age cohorts in the United States (US) and the People's Republic of China (PRC) have been dramatically different. If cohort effects account for cross-sectional age differences in mean levels of personality traits, different patterns of age differences should be seen in samples from the US and the PRC. The present study examined scores on scales from the California Psychological Inventory (CPI; Gough, 1987) in US (N = 348, age = 19-92 years) and PRC (N = 2,093, age = 18-67 years) samples. Very similar patterns of age correlations were seen. To compare results to other cross-cultural studies, CPI scales were interpreted in terms of the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality; an FFM Age-Relatedness Index based on American data accurately predicted CPI age correlations not only in the US but also in the PRC sample. Results are consistent with the hypothesis that there are universal intrinsic maturational changes in personality.


Assuntos
Cultura , Personalidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Análise de Variância , China , Estudos de Coortes , Comparação Transcultural , Estudos Transversais , Dependência Psicológica , Feminino , Previsões , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Introversão Psicológica , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Desenvolvimento da Personalidade , Inventário de Personalidade , Autoimagem , Predomínio Social , Estados Unidos
16.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 74(6): 1556-65, 1998 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9654759

RESUMO

The common variance among personality traits can be summarized in the factors of the five-factor model, which are known to be heritable. This study examined heritability of the residual specific variance in facet-level traits from the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Analyses of raw and residual facet scales across Canadian (183 monozygotic [MZ] and 175 dizogotic [DZ] pairs) and German (435 MZ and 205 DZ pairs) twin samples showed genetic and environmental influences of the same type and magnitude across the 2 samples for most facets. Additive genetic effects accounted for 25% to 65% of the reliable specific variance. Results provide strong support for hierarchical models of personality that posit a large number of narrow traits in addition to a few broader trait factors or domains. Facet-level traits are not simply exemplars of the broad factors they define; they are discrete constructs with their own heritable and thus biological basis.


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Personalidade/genética , Gêmeos/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Canadá , Análise Fatorial , Feminino , Alemanha , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Genéticos
17.
J Pers ; 66(3): 285-313, 1998 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9615420

RESUMO

Self-reports and spouse ratings of personality traits typically show less-than-perfect agreement, but powerful moderators of agreement have not yet been identified. In Study 1, 47 married couples completed the Revised NEO Personality Inventory to describe themselves and their spouses. Extent of agreement was not consistently moderated by response sets; the age, intelligence, or education of the respondent; or the length or quality of the relationship. In Study 2 these couples were interviewed about reasons for substantial disagreements, and an audiotape was content-analyzed. Sixteen reasons were reliably coded, including idiosyncratic understanding of items, reference to different time frames or roles, and unavailability of covert experience to the spouse. Faking good, assumed similarity, and other variables prominent in the psychometric literature were relatively unimportant. Findings (1) suggest that attempts to improve the validity of self-reports and ratings may need to be refocused and (2) underscore the desirability of routinely obtaining multiple sources of information on personality.


Assuntos
Relações Interpessoais , Casamento/psicologia , Determinação da Personalidade , Personalidade , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
18.
J Pers Assess ; 70(1): 87-102, 1998 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9615425

RESUMO

Many clinicians have come to rely on the broad array of validity scales available on the MMPI and the MMPI-2. In this study, we evaluated the utility of 2 MMPI-2 validity scales, the K scale and VRIN scale, in a sample of 692 psychiatric inpatients. Specifically, the effects of the K-correction procedure and the exclusion of protocols based on VRIN scale elevations were examined on the relation between MMPI-2 basic clinical scales and external criteria including both self-report and clinician ratings of psychopathology. Results indicated that the K-correction procedure commonly used with the MMPI and MMPI-2 did not result in higher correlations with external criteria in comparison to non-K-corrected scores. In contrast, MMPI-2 protocols that produced VRIN T-score values > or = 80 generally produced lower correlations with patients self-reports and clinician ratings of psychopathology in comparison to protocols judged to be valid based on VRIN scale results.


Assuntos
MMPI , Transtornos Mentais/diagnóstico , Psicometria , Revelação da Verdade , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Mid-Atlantic Region , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes
19.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 74(4): 1041-55, 1998 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9569658

RESUMO

Prior research (R.R. McCrae, P.T. Costa, & M.S. Yik, 1996) using a Chinese translation of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory suggested substantial differences between Hong Kong and North American undergraduates. Study 1, with a sample of bilingual Hong Kong students (N = 162), showed that prior findings were not due simply to the translation. Study 2, with undergraduates of European and Chinese ancestry living in Canada (N = 633), suggested that more of the differences were cultural in origin. Study 3, which used peer ratings of Chinese students (N = 99), replicated most Study 2 results, suggesting that exposure to Canadian culture increased openness, cheerfulness, and prosocial behavior and attitudes. Differences in sense of competence and vulnerability to stress appeared to be due to different cultural standards for judging these traits. Together, the 3 studies illustrate an integrated approach to interpreting personality differences across cultures.


Assuntos
Aculturação , Comparação Transcultural , Inventário de Personalidade , Personalidade , Psicometria , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Canadá , China/etnologia , Feminino , Hong Kong , Humanos , Masculino , Grupo Associado , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Percepção Social , Tradução
20.
Am Psychol ; 52(5): 509-16, 1997 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9145021

RESUMO

Patterns of covariation among personality traits in English-speaking populations can be summarized by the five-factor model (FFM). To assess the cross-cultural generalizability of the FFM, data from studies using 6 translations of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (P.T. Costa & R. R. McCrae, 1992) were compared with the American factor structure. German, Portuguese, Hebrew, Chinese, Korean, and Japanese samples (N = 7,134) showed similar structures after varimax rotation of 5 factors. When targeted rotations were used, the American factor structure was closely reproduced, even at the level of secondary loadings. Because the samples studied represented highly diverse cultures with languages from 5 distinct language families, these data strongly suggest that personality trait structure is universal.


Assuntos
Personalidade , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Cultura , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inquéritos e Questionários
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA