Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 18 de 18
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 152(12): 3599-3604, 2023 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38047911

RESUMO

Nobes et al. (2019) used updated data from the same source-the British Home Office's Homicide Index-as that used by Daly and Wilson (1994) to investigate the Cinderella effect (increased risk to stepchildren), and in particular their claim (e.g., Daly, 2022; Daly & Wilson, 1994, 2001, 2008) that stepfathers fatally assault their young children at rates more than 100 times those of genetic fathers. Nobes et al. reported much lower-though still substantial-increased risk to young stepchildren, and little or none to older children, particularly when they took the mislabeling of noncohabiting perpetrators into account. In his Commentary, Daly (2022) largely accepts this analysis, but does not acknowledge its implications for his own findings and claims. Nobes et al. also reported that controlling for father's age accounted for much of the remaining increased risk, and argued that this and other confounding variables are likely to explain most or all of the Cinderella effect. Daly says very little about this too, but instead responds with a series of criticisms, many of which misrepresent Nobes et al.'s account, and most of which are incorrect. Young stepchildren are at increased risk, but if stepparenthood per se (i.e., lack of genetic relatedness) contributes to the explanation, its influence is considerably less than Daly claims. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Fatores Etários , Maus-Tratos Infantis , Estrutura Familiar , Adolescente , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Masculino , Maus-Tratos Infantis/mortalidade , Fatores de Confusão Epidemiológicos , Pai , Homicídio , População Branca
2.
J Interpers Violence ; 37(7-8): NP5747-NP5773, 2022 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32281898

RESUMO

Evolutionary psychologists claim that stepparents perpetrate substantially more child physical abuse than genetic parents, and that they do so because they are less invested in genetically unrelated children. The objective of this study was to examine these claims by investigating whether, and why, fathers in a Colombian sample physically abused their stepchildren more than their genetic children. Fathers (N = 86) and their partners living in Bogotá were interviewed by Klevens et al. Half of the fathers had been reported to authorities for child physical abuse, the other half were matched controls. Secondary analysis was conducted of Klevens et al.'s data. Hypotheses from the evolutionary and ecological accounts of child maltreatment were tested using logistic and ordinal regression. Both the prevalence and the frequency of physical abuse by stepfathers were considerably greater than those of genetic fathers. Several indicators of adversity-including parental youth and experience of abuse, fathers' chronic stress, and mothers' poor communication with the child-were associated with both abuse and stepparenthood. Models including these variables indicated that they accounted for much of the stepfathers' higher rates of abuse. Consistent with the ecological account, much of the stepfathers' greater prevalence and frequency of abuse in this sample is likely to have resulted from confounding variables, rather than from the step relationship per se.


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis , Abuso Físico , Adolescente , Criança , Colômbia/epidemiologia , Pai , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Mães
3.
Br J Psychol ; 113(2): 370-395, 2022 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34719018

RESUMO

Two experiments were conducted to investigate the relative influence of agents' negligence and their actions' unintended outcomes on moral judgements. In Study 1, 343 participants were asked in an online questionnaire about a driver whose level of negligence, and the severity of the outcome, were varied systematically. Each judged how much punishment and blame the driver deserved, and rated her negligence, causal responsibility, and intentionality. In Study 2, 341 participants completed the same questionnaire, and also judged the driver's wrongness and the outcome's severity. In both studies, judgements were strongly influenced by negligence; blame was also affected by causal responsibility, and wrongness by intention, but the relatively slight outcome effect on blame and wrongness was largely mediated by negligence. In contrast, both negligence and outcome had substantial effects on punishment judgements; most participants assigned high levels of punishment when, and only when, the outcome was negative and the agent was negligent. These findings shed light on the intriguing phenomenon of moral luck, and indicate that it applies more to punishment judgements than to blame and wrongness. They also indicate that when no negligence information is provided in the description of accidents (as in many previous studies), participants often attribute negligence to agents and judge them accordingly. It seems that the effect of outcome on moral judgements has often been overestimated by researchers, and that of negligence underestimated.


Assuntos
Imperícia , Princípios Morais , Acidentes , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Punição
4.
Br J Health Psychol ; 25(3): 615-638, 2020 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32678937

RESUMO

PURPOSE: People with long-term mental health problems are heavier smokers than the general population, and suffer greater smoking-related morbidity and mortality. Little is known about the effectiveness of psychological smoking cessation interventions for this group. This review evaluates evidence from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) on the effectiveness of psychological interventions, used alone or with pharmacotherapy, in reducing smoking in adults with mental health problems. METHODS: We searched relevant articles between January 1999 and March 2019 and identified 6,200 papers. Two reviewers screened 81 full-text articles. Outcome measures included number of cigarettes smoked per day, 7-day point prevalence abstinence, and continuous abstinence from smoking. RESULTS: Thirteen RCTs, involving 1,497 participants, met the inclusion criteria. Psychological interventions included cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT), motivational interviewing (MI), counselling, and telephone smoking cessation support. Three trials resulted in significant reductions in smoking for patients receiving psychological interventions compared with controls. Two trials showed higher 7-day point prevalence in intervention plus nicotine replacement therapy (NRT) versus standard care groups. Four trials showed that participants who combined pharmacotherapy (bupropion or varenicline) with CBT were more likely to reduce their smoking by 50% than those receiving CBT only. Four out of five trials that compared different psychological interventions (with or without NRT) had positive outcomes regardless of intervention type. CONCLUSIONS: This study contributes to our understanding in a number of ways: The available evidence is consistent with a range of psychological interventions being independently effective in reducing smoking by people with mental health problems; however, too few well-designed studies have been conducted for us to be confident about, for example, which interventions work best for whom, and how they should be implemented. Evidence is clearer for a range of psychological interventions - including CBT, MI, and behavioural or supportive counselling - being effective when used with NRT or pharmacotherapy. Telephone-based and relatively brief interventions appear to be as effective as more intense and longer-term ones. There is also good evidence for a strong dose-response relationship - increased attendance predicts improved outcomes - and for interventions having more positive than negative effects on psychiatric symptoms.


Assuntos
Transtornos Mentais/complicações , Intervenção Psicossocial/métodos , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/psicologia , Adulto , Humanos , Saúde Mental , Ensaios Clínicos Controlados Aleatórios como Assunto , Abandono do Hábito de Fumar/métodos , Dispositivos para o Abandono do Uso de Tabaco
5.
Brain Res ; 1723: 146387, 2019 11 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31419430

RESUMO

This study aimed to identify the neural correlates of aggression-related attentional selectivity to angry faces in physical aggression. Physical aggression in a non-clinical sample of young men (N = 36) was measured using an aggression questionnaire. Visual attentional bias to angry faces was assessed using a dot-probe task during which angry and neutral faces were presented simultaneously, and EEG was recorded. Median split and correlational analyses were conducted to assess the relationship between physical aggression and attentional bias. Behavioural results indicated that higher levels of physical aggression were associated with greater attentional bias to angry faces. ERP results revealed an interaction where males with higher physical aggression had undifferentiated P3 amplitudes to angry and neutral trials, whereas low physical aggression males exhibited greater P3 amplitude to angry than to neutral trials (effect of probe congruency). Increased levels of physical aggression were also significantly correlated with increased P3 amplitude to probes replacing neutral faces, relative to angry faces. It was concluded that the aggressive males selectively attend to angry faces, and that attentional bias is characterized by undifferentiated P3 amplitude. We propose that this results from an inferior ability to downregulate competing angry face distractors when responding to probes replacing neutral faces (as reflected by the P3 response). These findings indicate that attentional bias to angry faces in individuals with higher physical aggression is characterized by a distinctive ERP signature; this could inform the development of therapeutic interventions seeking to reduce aggression.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Viés de Atenção/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Facial/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Ira/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Potenciais Evocados/fisiologia , Face , Expressão Facial , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia
6.
Brain Sci ; 9(6)2019 Jun 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31159467

RESUMO

The relationship between ADHD-in particular hyperactivity-and criminal behavior is well documented. The current study investigated the role of criminogenic cognitions in the explanation of this relationship by examining which symptoms of ADHD are associated with criminogenic cognitions. Community-recruited adults (N = 192) completed self-report questionnaires for symptoms of ADHD and criminogenic cognitions. Symptoms of inattention were consistently and strongly related to criminogenic cognitions. In particular, inattention was significantly related to cutoff, cognitive indolence, and discontinuity. There was also evidence that impulsivity was positively related to criminogenic cognitions, and specifically, to the power orientation subscale. In contrast, and contrary to expectations, symptoms of hyperactivity were not related to criminogenic cognitions. These results indicate that in community-recruited adults, inattention rather than hyperactivity is related to criminogenic cognitions. We discuss the implications of these findings contrasting with those of previous studies that used forensic and clinical samples.

7.
Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol ; 63(2): 159-178, 2019 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30095011

RESUMO

There is now convincing evidence that childhood maltreatment is associated with youth offending; however, relatively little is known about the characteristics and needs of those who are involved in both the child protection and youth justice systems, and the extent to which these might differ according to level of child protection involvement. This study reports the characteristics and needs of 2,045 young people who were under supervision in secure custody or detention in South Australia between 1995 and 2012 according to the level of exposure to the child protection system in an Australian jurisdiction. Five groups of young offenders were compared: (a) no known child protection notifications or substantiated experience of abuse and/or neglect, (b) notifications only, (c) substantiated notifications, (d) notifications or substantiations and subsequent placement in out-of-home care (OHC), and (e) placement in OHC only. The results indicate that young people who have a history of child protection system involvement have significantly greater and more complex needs than those who have no child protection experience. It is concluded that different service responses may be required to meet the diverse needs of these groups of young people under youth justice supervision.


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis/estatística & dados numéricos , Serviços de Proteção Infantil/estatística & dados numéricos , Delinquência Juvenil/estatística & dados numéricos , Adolescente , Experiências Adversas da Infância/estatística & dados numéricos , Ira , Austrália/epidemiologia , Criança , Depressão/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Grupos Raciais , Distribuição por Sexo , Transtornos Relacionados ao Uso de Substâncias/epidemiologia
8.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 148(6): 1091-1102, 2019 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30247060

RESUMO

Daly and Wilson (1994, 2008) reported that rates of fatal assaults of young children by stepfathers are over 100 times those by genetic fathers, and they explain the difference in evolutionary terms. Their study was replicated by comparing updated homicide data and population data from 3 surveys. This indicated that the risk to young stepchildren was approximately 16 times that to genetic children, and stepfathers were twice as likely to kill by beating. However, when we controlled for father's age, the risk from cohabiting stepfathers was approximately 6 times greater. Above the age of 4 years, stepchildren were at no greater risk than genetic children. Children are at risk from fathers primarily when both are young and they do not live together; stepfathers' apparent overrepresentation results largely from their relative youth and from many nonresidential perpetrators being labeled stepfathers. Other factors are also influential, but if these include stepparenthood, its impact is considerably less than previous researchers have claimed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Assuntos
Maus-Tratos Infantis/estatística & dados numéricos , Pai/estatística & dados numéricos , Homicídio/estatística & dados numéricos , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Inglaterra , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , País de Gales
9.
Brain Res ; 2018 07 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29990489

RESUMO

This article has been withdrawn at the request of the authors. The authors have opted to update their article and have resubmitted it to the journal as a new submission. The updated article has now been accepted and can be found here: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146387. The full Elsevier Policy on Article Withdrawal can be found at https://www.elsevier.com/about/our-business/policies/article-withdrawal.

10.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 166: 96-115, 2018 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28888195

RESUMO

This study explored the development of understanding of death in a sample of 4- to 11-year-old British children and adults (N=136). It also investigated four sets of possible influences on this development: parents' religion and spiritual beliefs, cognitive ability, socioeconomic status, and experience of illness and death. Participants were interviewed using the "death concept" interview that explores understanding of the subcomponents of inevitability, universality, irreversibility, cessation, and causality of death. Children understood key aspects of death from as early as 4 or 5years, and with age their explanations of inevitability, universality, and causality became increasingly biological. Understanding of irreversibility and the cessation of mental and physical processes also emerged during early childhood, but by 10years many children's explanations reflected not an improved biological understanding but rather the coexistence of apparently contradictory biological and supernatural ideas-religious, spiritual, or metaphysical. Evidence for these coexistent beliefs was more prevalent in older children than in younger children and was associated with their parents' religious and spiritual beliefs. Socioeconomic status was partly related to children's biological ideas, whereas cognitive ability and experience of illness and death played less important roles. There was no evidence for coexistent thinking among adults, only a clear distinction between biological explanations about death and supernatural explanations about the afterlife.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Compreensão/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Morte , Pais/psicologia , Religião e Psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
11.
Dev Psychol ; 53(10): 1895-1911, 2017 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28758780

RESUMO

Two experiments were conducted to investigate the influences on 4-8 year-olds' and adults' moral judgments. In both, participants were told stories from previous studies that had indicated that children's judgments are largely outcome-based. Building on recent research in which one change to these studies' methods resulted in substantially more intention-based judgment, in Experiment 1 (N = 75) the salience and recency of intention information were increased, and in Experiment 2 (N = 99) carefulness information (i.e., the absence of negligence) was also added. In both experiments even the youngest children's judgments were primarily intention-based, and in Experiment 2 punishment judgments were similar to adults' from 5-6 years. Comparisons of data across studies and experiments indicated that both changes increased the proportion of intention-based punishment judgments-but not acceptability judgments-across age-groups. These findings challenge and help to explain those of much previous research, according to which children's judgments are primarily outcome-based. However, younger participants continued to judge according to outcome more than older participants. This might indicate that young children are more influenced by outcomes than are adults, but other possible explanations are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record


Assuntos
Intenção , Desenvolvimento Moral , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Psicológicos , Punição/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
12.
Cognition ; 157: 190-204, 2016 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27649094

RESUMO

The influence of intention and outcome information on moral judgments was investigated by telling children aged 4-8yearsandadults (N=169) stories involving accidental harms (positive intention, negative outcome) or attempted harms (negative intention, positive outcome) from two studies (Helwig, Zelazo, & Wilson, 2001; Zelazo, Helwig, & Lau, 1996). When the original acceptability (wrongness) question was asked, the original findings were closely replicated: children's and adults' acceptability judgments were based almost exclusively on outcome, and children's punishment judgments were also primarily outcome-based. However, when this question was rephrased, 4-5-year-olds' judgments were approximately equally influenced by intention and outcome, and from 5-6years they were based considerably more on intention than outcome. These findings indicate that, for methodological reasons, children's (and adults') ability to make intention-based judgment has often been substantially underestimated.


Assuntos
Intenção , Julgamento , Princípios Morais , Percepção Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicologia da Criança , Punição , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
13.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 33(1): 31-44, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25262770

RESUMO

This study explored British and Pakistani 4- to 7-year-olds' (N = 188) understanding of death. The aim was to examine possible influences on the acquisition of the subcomponents of the death concept by investigating how they are understood by children of different ages and cultural and religious backgrounds. Three groups of children were compared: White British and British Muslim living in London, and Pakistani Muslim living in rural Pakistan. In line with previous research (Slaughter, 2005, Aust. Psychol., 40(3), 179), irreversibility of death was one of the first subcomponents to be acquired, while causality was the last. The two groups of British children shared many similarities in their understanding of inevitability, applicability, irreversibility, and cessation. Pakistani Muslim children understood irreversibility earlier than did children in both British groups. In all three cultural groups, children's responses demonstrated very limited understanding of causality. Our findings support the view that aspects of a mature understanding of death develop between the ages of 4 and 7 years and that the process of understanding death as a biological event is, to a great extent, universal. They also suggest that aspects of children's reasoning are influenced by culturally specific experiences, particularly those arising from living in rural versus urban settings.


Assuntos
Atitude Frente a Morte/etnologia , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Comparação Transcultural , Islamismo/psicologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Compreensão , Feminino , Humanos , Londres/etnologia , Masculino , Paquistão/etnologia
14.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 32(3): 276-90, 2014 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24628109

RESUMO

This study aimed to identify the age by which children begin to demonstrate a biological understanding of the human body and the idea that the purpose of body functioning is to maintain life. The study also explored the influence of education, culturally specific experiences and religion on knowledge acquisition in this domain. Children aged between 4 and 7 years from three different cultural backgrounds (White British, British Muslim, and Pakistani Muslim) were interviewed about the human body and its functioning. At least half of the 4- to 5-year-olds in each cultural group, and almost all 6- to 7-year-olds, referred to the maintenance of life when explaining organs' functions and so were classified as 'life theorizers'. Pakistani Muslim children gave fewer biological responses to questions about organs' functions and the purpose of eating and breathing, but referred to life more than their British counterparts. Irrespective of cultural group, older children understood organ location and function better than younger children. These findings support Jaakkola and Slaughter's (2002, Br. J. Dev. Psychol., 20, 325) view that children's understanding of the body as a 'life machine' emerges around the ages of 4-5 years. They also suggest that, despite many similarities in children's ideas cross-culturally, different educational input and culturally specific experiences influence aspects of their biological understanding.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento Infantil/fisiologia , Compreensão/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Comparação Transcultural , Fenômenos Fisiológicos/fisiologia , Fatores Etários , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Cultura , Feminino , Corpo Humano , Humanos , Vida , Masculino , Paquistão/etnologia , Religião e Psicologia , Reino Unido/etnologia
15.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 104(4): 382-97, 2009 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19740483

RESUMO

Piaget (1932) and subsequent researchers have reported that young children's moral judgments are based more on the outcomes of actions than on the agents' intentions. The current study investigated whether negligence might also influence these judgments and explain children's apparent focus on outcome. Children (3-8 years of age) and adults (N=139) rated accidental actions in which the valences of intention, negligence, and outcome were varied systematically. Participants of all ages were influenced primarily by intention, and well-intentioned actions were also evaluated according to negligence and outcome. Only two young children based their judgments solely on outcome. It is suggested that previous studies have underestimated children's use of intention because outcome and negligence have been confounded. Negative consequences are considered to be important because children assume that they are caused by negligence. The findings indicate that young children can show sophisticated understanding of the roles of intention and negligence in moral judgments.


Assuntos
Intenção , Julgamento , Princípios Morais , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Desenvolvimento Infantil , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Motivação , Percepção Social , Adulto Jovem
16.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 104(1): 52-67, 2009 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19100995

RESUMO

This study investigated the claim (e.g., Vosniadou & Brewer's, 1992) that children have naive "mental models" of the earth and believe, for example, that the earth is flat or hollow. It tested the proposal that children appear to have these misconceptions because they find the researchers' tasks and questions to be confusing and ambiguous. Participants were 6- and 7-year-olds (N=127) who were given either the mental model theorists' original drawing task or a new version in which the same instructions and questions were rephrased to minimize ambiguity and, thus, possible misinterpretation. In response to the new version, children gave substantially more indication of having scientific understanding and less of having naive mental models, suggesting that the misconceptions reported by the mental model theorists are largely methodological artifacts. There were also differences between the responses to the original version and those reported by Vosniadou and Brewer, indicating that other factors, such as cohort and cultural effects, are also likely to help explain the discrepant findings of previous research.


Assuntos
Compreensão , Formação de Conceito , Planeta Terra , Modelos Psicológicos , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Teoria Psicológica , Ciência , Inquéritos e Questionários , Reino Unido
17.
Br J Psychol ; 100(Pt 2): 347-63, 2009 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18680639

RESUMO

Vosniadou and Brewer (1992) claim that children's drawings and answers to questions show that they have naive, theory-like 'mental models' of the earth; for example, they believe it to be flat, or hollow with people inside. However, recent studies that have used different methods have found little or no evidence of these misconceptions. The contrasting accounts, and possible reasons for the inconsistent findings, were tested by giving adults (N = 484) either the original task (designed for 5-year olds) or a new version in which the same drawing instructions and questions were rephrased and clarified. Many adults' responses to the original version were identical to children's 'naïve' drawings and answers. The new version elicited substantially fewer non-scientific responses. These findings indicate that even adults find the original instructions and questions ambiguous and confusing, and that this is the principal reason for their non-scientific drawings and answers. Since children must find the task even more confusing than adults, this explanation very probably applies to many of their non-scientific responses, too, and therefore accounts for the discrepant findings of previous research. 'Naïve' responses result largely from misinterpretation of Vosniadou and Brewer's apparently simple task, rather than from mental models of the earth.


Assuntos
Arte , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Planeta Terra , Modelos Psicológicos , Projetos de Pesquisa , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Adolescente , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Pré-Escolar , Ciência Cognitiva/métodos , Comunicação , Compreensão/fisiologia , Confusão , Feminino , Humanos , Londres , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Psicológicos , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Estudantes/psicologia , Adulto Jovem
18.
Br J Psychol ; 98(Pt 4): 645-65, 2007 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17535475

RESUMO

When children are asked to draw the Earth they often produce intriguing pictures in which, for example, people seem to be standing on a flat disc or inside a hollow sphere. These drawings, and children's answers to questions, have been interpreted as indicating that children construct naïve, theory-like mental models of the Earth (e.g. Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992). However, recent studies using different methods have found little or no evidence of these mental models, and report that many young children have some scientific knowledge of the Earth. To examine the reasons for these contrasting findings, adults (N=350) were given the drawing task previously given to 5-year-old children. Fewer than half of the adults' pictures were scientific, and 15% were identical to children's 'naïve' drawings. Up to half of the answers to questions (e.g. 'Where do people live?') were non-scientific. Open-ended questions and follow-up interviews revealed that non-scientific responses were given because adults found the apparently simple task confusing and challenging. Since children very probably find it even more difficult, these findings indicate that children's non-scientific responses, like adults', often result from methodological problems with the task. These results therefore explain the discrepant findings of previous research, and support the studies which indicate that children do not have naïve mental models of the Earth.


Assuntos
Formação de Conceito , Planeta Terra , Ciência , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Criança , Cognição , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Inquéritos e Questionários , Ensino
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA