Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 20
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 15(4): 802-8, 2008 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18792507

RESUMO

Everyday tasks often require us to keep track of multiple objects in dynamic scenes. Past studies show that tracking becomes more difficult as objects move faster. In the present study, we show that this trade-off may not be due to increased speed itself but may, instead, be due to the increased crowding that usually accompanies increases in speed. Here, we isolate changes in speed from variations in crowding, by projecting a tracking display either onto a small area at the center of a hemispheric projection dome or onto the entire dome. Use of the larger display increased retinal image size and object speed by a factor of 4 but did not increase interobject crowding. Results showed that tracking accuracy was equally good in the large-display condition, even when the objects traveled far into the visual periphery. Accuracy was also not reduced when we tested object speeds that limited performance in the small-display condition. These results, along with a reinterpretation of past studies, suggest that we might be able to track multiple moving objects as fast as we can a single moving object, once the effect of object crowding is eliminated.


Assuntos
Atenção , Área de Dependência-Independência , Percepção de Movimento , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Tempo de Reação , Percepção de Tamanho , Aceleração , Aglomeração , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Humanos , Orientação
2.
Cognition ; 80(1-2): 127-58, 2001 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11245842

RESUMO

This paper argues that a theory of situated vision, suited for the dual purposes of object recognition and the control of action, will have to provide something more than a system that constructs a conceptual representation from visual stimuli: it will also need to provide a special kind of direct (preconceptual, unmediated) connection between elements of a visual representation and certain elements in the world. Like natural language demonstratives (such as 'this' or 'that') this direct connection allows entities to be referred to without being categorized or conceptualized. Several reasons are given for why we need such a preconceptual mechanism which individuates and keeps track of several individual objects in the world. One is that early vision must pick out and compute the relation among several individual objects while ignoring their properties. Another is that incrementally computing and updating representations of a dynamic scene requires keeping track of token individuals despite changes in their properties or locations. It is then noted that a mechanism meeting these requirements has already been proposed in order to account for a number of disparate empirical phenomena, including subitizing, search-subset selection and multiple object tracking (Pylyshyn et al., Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 48(2) (1994) 260). This mechanism, called a visual index or FINST, is briefly discussed and it is argued that viewing it as performing a demonstrative or preconceptual reference function has far-reaching implications not only for a theory of situated vision, but also for suggesting a new way to look at why the primitive individuation of visual objects, or proto-objects, is so central in computing visual representations. Indexing visual objects is also, according to this view, the primary means for grounding visual concepts and is a potentially fruitful way to look at the problem of visual integration across time and across saccades, as well as to explain how infants' numerical capacity might arise.


Assuntos
Formação de Conceito , Desempenho Psicomotor , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Percepção Visual , Ciência Cognitiva , Humanos , Teoria Psicológica
3.
Cognition ; 80(1-2): 159-77, 2001 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11245843

RESUMO

The notion that visual attention can operate over visual objects in addition to spatial locations has recently received much empirical support, but there has been relatively little empirical consideration of what can count as an 'object' in the first place. We have investigated this question in the context of the multiple object tracking paradigm, in which subjects must track a number of independently and unpredictably moving identical items in a field of identical distractors. What types of feature clusters can be tracked in this manner? In other words, what counts as an 'object' in this task? We investigated this question with a technique we call target merging: we alter tracking displays so that distinct target and distractor locations appear perceptually to be parts of the same object by merging pairs of items (one target with one distractor) in various ways - for example, by connecting item locations with a simple line segment, by drawing the convex hull of the two items, and so forth. The data show that target merging makes the tracking task far more difficult to varying degrees depending on exactly how the items are merged. The effect is perceptually salient, involving in some conditions a total destruction of subjects' capacity to track multiple items. These studies provide strong evidence for the object-based nature of tracking, confirming that in some contexts attention must be allocated to objects rather than arbitrary collections of features. In addition, the results begin to reveal the types of spatially organized scene components that can be independently attended as a function of properties such as connectedness, part structure, and other types of perceptual grouping.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Movimento , Percepção Visual , Análise de Variância , Humanos
4.
Nature ; 408(6809): 196-9, 2000 Nov 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11089972

RESUMO

Visual attention allows an observer to select certain visual information for specialized processing. Selection is readily apparent in 'tracking' tasks where even with the eyes fixed, observers can track a target as it moves among identical distractor items. In such a case, a target is distinguished by its spatial trajectory. Here we show that one can keep track of a stationary item solely on the basis of its changing appearance--specified by its trajectory along colour, orientation, and spatial frequency dimensions--even when a distractor shares the same spatial location. This ability to track through feature space bears directly on competing theories of attention, that is, on whether attention can select locations in space, features such as colour or shape, or particular visual objects composed of constellations of visual features. Our results affirm, consistent with a growing body of psychophysical and neurophysiological evidence, that attention can indeed select specific visual objects. Furthermore, feature-space tracking extends the definition of visual object to include not only items with well defined spatio-temporal trajectories, but also those with well defined featuro-temporal trajectories.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos
5.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 54(1): 1-14, 2000 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10721235

RESUMO

How are attentional priorities set when multiple stimuli compete for access to the limited-capacity visual attention system? According to Pylyshyn (1989) and Yantis and Johnson (1990), a small number of visual objects can be preattentively indexed or tagged and thereby accessed more rapidly by a subsequent attentional process (e.g., the traditional "spotlight of attention"). In the present study, we used the multiple object tracking methodology of Pylyshyn and Storm (1988) to investigate the relation between what we call "visual indexing" and attentional processing. Participants visually tracked a subset of a set of identical, independently randomly moving objects in a display (the targets), and made a speeded identification response when they noticed a target or a nontarget (distractor) object undergo a subtle form transformation. We found that target form changes were identified more rapidly than nontarget form changes, and that the speed of responding to target form changes was unaffected by the number of nontargets in the display when the form-changing targets were successfully tracked. We also found that this enhanced processing only applied to the targets themselves and not to nearby nontarget distractors, showing that the allocation of a broadened region of visual attention (as in the zoom-lens model of attentional allocation) could not account for these findings. These results confirm that visual indexing bestows a processing priority to a number of objects in the visual field.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Movimentos Oculares/fisiologia , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Humanos , Distribuição Aleatória
6.
Cogn Psychol ; 38(2): 259-90, 1999 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10090804

RESUMO

In three experiments, subjects attempted to track multiple items as they moved independently and unpredictably about a display. Performance was not impaired when the items were briefly (but completely) occluded at various times during their motion, suggesting that occlusion is taken into account when computing enduring perceptual objecthood. Unimpaired performance required the presence of accretion and deletion cues along fixed contours at the occluding boundaries. Performance was impaired when items were present on the visual field at the same times and to the same degrees as in the occlusion conditions, but disappeared and reappeared in ways which did not implicate the presence of occluding surfaces (e.g., by imploding and exploding into and out of existence instead of accreting and deleting along a fixed contour). Unimpaired performance did not require visible occluders (i.e., Michotte's tunnel effect) or globally consistent occluder positions. We discuss implications of these results for theories of objecthood in visual attention.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Formação de Conceito/fisiologia , Percepção de Forma/fisiologia , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Mascaramento Perceptivo/fisiologia , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Humanos , Acompanhamento Ocular Uniforme/fisiologia
7.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 24(2): 505-25, 1998 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9554095

RESUMO

Four experiments with undergraduates used illusory line motion (ILM) to contrast Z. W. Pylyshyn's (1989) FINST theory of spatial indexing with predictions made by unitary attention models. Multiple-onset stimuli were able to cause ILM at disparate, noncontiguous spatial locations. Consistent with gradient explanations of ILM and with FINST theory predictions, varying line-drawing speed and the number of stimuli revealed a decrease in ILM and a capacity limitation, respectively. Modeling analyses suggested a limit in the number of locations (5-7) that could elicit the illusion. Requiring participants to report the locations of all stimuli exhibiting illusory motion in a specified direction suggested parallel access to between 2 and 5 display locations simultaneously. The results of all 4 experiments were predicted by FINST theory but not by a broad class of unitary attention hypotheses.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Movimento , Ilusões Ópticas , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Aceleração , Adulto , Sensibilidades de Contraste , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicofísica , Estudantes/psicologia
8.
Spat Vis ; 11(2): 225-58, 1997.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9428097

RESUMO

This paper presents three experiments investigating the claim that the visual system utilizes a primitive indexing mechanism (sometimes called FINSTS; Pylyshyn, 1989) to make non-contiguous features directly accessible for further visual processing. This claim is investigated using a variant of the conjunction search task in which subjects search among a subset of the items in a conjunction search display for targets defined by a conjunction of colour and orientation. The members of the subset were identified by virtue of the late onset of the objects' place-holders. The cued subset was manipulated to include either homogeneous distractors or mixed distractors. Observers were able to select a subset of three items from among fifteen for further processing (Experiment 1); furthermore, a reaction time advantage for homogeneous subsets over mixed subsets was observed, indicating that more than one of the subset is selected for further specialized processing. The homogeneous subset advantage held for subsets of two to five items (Experiment 2), and the time required to process the cued subset did not increase with increased dispersion of the items (Experiment 3). These results support the basic claim of the indexing theory: the claim that multiple visual indexes are used in selecting objects for visual processing.


Assuntos
Atenção , Percepção de Cores , Orientação , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Variações Dependentes do Observador , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Percepção de Tamanho , Percepção Espacial
9.
Psychol Rev ; 101(1): 80-102, 1994 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8121961

RESUMO

"Subitizing," the process of enumeration when there are fewer than 4 items, is rapid (40-100 ms/item), effortless, and accurate. "Counting," the process of enumeration when there are more than 4 items, is slow (250-350 ms/item), effortful, and error-prone. Why is there a difference in the way the small and large numbers of items are enumerated? A theory of enumeration is proposed that emerges from a general theory of vision, yet explains the numeric abilities of preverbal infants, children, and adults. We argue that subitizing exploits a limited-capacity parallel mechanism for item individuation, the FINST mechanism, associated with the multiple target tracking task (Pylyshyn, 1989; Pylyshyn & Storm, 1988). Two kinds of evidence support the claim that subitizing relies on preattentive information, whereas counting requires spatial attention. First, whenever spatial attention is needed to compute a spatial relation (cf. Ullman, 1984) or to perform feature integration (cf. Treisman & Gelade, 1980), subitizing does not occur (Trick & Pylyshyn, 1993a). Second, the position of the attentional focus, as manipulated by cue validity, has a greater effect on counting than subitizing latencies (Trick & Pylyshyn, 1993b).


Assuntos
Atenção , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Resolução de Problemas , Tempo de Reação , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Humanos , Lactente , Orientação
10.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 19(2): 331-51, 1993 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8473843

RESUMO

Subitizing, the enumeration of 1-4 items, is rapid (40-120 ms/item) and accurate. Counting, the enumeration of 5 items or more, is slow (250-350 ms/item) and error-prone. Why are small numbers of items enumerated differently from large numbers of items? It is suggested that subitizing relies on a preattentive mechanism. Ss could subitize heterogeneously sized multicontour items but not concentric multicontour items, which require attentional processing because preattentive gestalt processes misgroup contours from different items to form units. Similarly, Ss could subitize target items among distractors but only if the targets and distractors differed by a feature, a property derived through preattentive analysis. Thus, subitizing must rely on a mechanism that can handle a few items at once, which operates before attention but after preattentive operations of feature detection and grouping.


Assuntos
Atenção , Orientação , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Enquadramento Psicológico , Percepção Espacial , Adulto , Aprendizagem por Discriminação , Feminino , Área de Dependência-Independência , Fixação Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Mascaramento Perceptivo , Psicofísica , Tempo de Reação , Percepção de Tamanho
12.
Spat Vis ; 3(3): 179-97, 1988.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3153671

RESUMO

There is considerable evidence that visual attention is concentrated at a single locus in the visual field, and that this locus can be moved independent of eye movements. Two studies are reported which suggest that, while certain aspects of attention require that locations be scanned serially, at least one operation may be carried out in parallel across several independent loci in the visual field. That is the operation of indexing features and tracking their identity. The studies show that: (a) subjects are able to track a subset of up to 5 objects in a field of 10 identical randomly-moving objects in order to distinguish a change in a target from a change in a distractor; and (b) when the speed and distance parameters of the display are designed so that, on the basis of some very conservative assumptions about the speed of attention movement and encoding times, the predicted performance of a serial scanning and updating algorithm would not exceed about 40% accuracy, subjects still manage to do the task with 87% accuracy. These findings are discussed in relation to an earlier, and independently motivated model of feature-binding--called the FINST model--which posits a primitive identity maintenance mechanism that indexes and tracks a limited number of visual objects in parallel. These indexes are hypothesized to serve the function of binding visual features prior to subsequent pattern recognition.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Visão Ocular/fisiologia , Algoritmos , Movimentos Oculares , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Biológicos , Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Campos Visuais , Vias Visuais/fisiologia
13.
Spat Vis ; 2(4): 263-72, 1987.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3154951

RESUMO

The minimum ISI required for perceiving apparent motion in depth was measured as a function of the 2D separation of stimuli and the physical separation of stimuli in depth. It was found that temporal thresholds increased as a function of the separation of stimuli in depth. This supports the results of previous research indicating that the perceived three-dimensionality of apparent motion in depth increases with ISI. In addition, the rate of threshold increase was significantly greater in displays with short 2D separations of stimuli than in displays with large 2D separations. This robust functional dissociation of thresholds indicates that the short-range system may be involved in the processing of apparent motion in depth in the former case.


Assuntos
Percepção de Movimento/fisiologia , Vias Visuais , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino
17.
J Psycholinguist Res ; 2(1): 21-50, 1973 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24197794

RESUMO

This paper discusses several common misunderstandings regarding theories of competence. Such theories are characterized as being concerned with the epistemological side of cognitive psychology and as being based primarily on evidence of a special kind: intuitions of competent performers. The nature of such evidence is examined in relation to the question of objectivity. The position that competence may be described in terms of implicit rules is discussed from several perspectives. Finally a number of empiricist objections to the notion of competence are examined: particularly ones based on the fact that whereas competence theories are infinitary, deterministic, and formally complete, observations of actual performance suggest that it is best characterized as finitary, probabilistic, and heuristically organized. Finally the question of the psychological reality of competence formalisms is discussed and it is argued that the claim of the psychological reality of amechanism, as opposed to a structural description, is only appropriate when the mechanism accounts parsimoniously for the widest possible range of empirical phenomena.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA