Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 19 de 19
Filtrar
1.
Oncotarget ; 11(22): 2083-2091, 2020 Jun 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32547706

RESUMO

Despite relevant medical advancements, metastatic breast cancer remains an uncurable disease. HER2 signaling conditions tumor behavior and treatment strategies of HER2 expressing breast cancer. Cancer treatment guidelines uniformly identify dual blockade with pertuzumab and trastuzumab plus a taxane as best first line and trastuzumab emtansine as preferred second line choice. However, there is no prospectively designed available study focusing on the sequence and outcomes of patients treated with T-DM1 following the triplet. In the following report, data concerning a wide series of patients treated in a real-life setting are presented. Results obtained in terms of response and median progression free survival suggests a significant role for T-DM1 in disease control of metastatic HER2 expressing breast cancer.

2.
Child Dev ; 88(3): 900-918, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27739069

RESUMO

The effects of social status on human health can be modeled in captive cohorts of nonhuman primates. This study shows that maternal social rank is associated with broad changes in DNA methylation in placentae of rhesus monkeys (N = 10). Differentially methylated genes between social ranks are enriched in signaling pathways playing major roles in placenta physiology. Moreover, the authors found significant overlaps with genes whose expression was previously associated with social rank in adult rhesus monkeys (Tung et al., 2012) and whose methylation was associated with perinatal stress in newborn humans and rhesus monkeys (Nieratschker et al., 2014). These results are consistent with the hypothesis that system-wide epigenetic changes in multiple tissues are involved in long-term adaptations to the social environment.


Assuntos
Metilação de DNA , Macaca mulatta , Placenta/metabolismo , Predomínio Social , Animais , Feminino , Macaca mulatta/genética , Macaca mulatta/metabolismo , Gravidez
3.
Dev Psychopathol ; 28(4pt2): 1259-1272, 2016 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27687908

RESUMO

Studies in rodents, nonhuman primates, and humans suggest that epigenetic processes mediate between early life experiences and adult phenotype. However, the normal evolution of epigenetic programs during child development, the effect of sex, and the impact of early life adversity on these trajectories are not well understood. This study mapped the genome-wide DNA methylation changes in CD3+ T lymphocytes from rhesus monkeys from postnatal day 14 through 2 years of age in both males and females and determined the impact of maternal deprivation on the DNA methylation profile. We show here that DNA methylation profiles evolve from birth to adolescence and are sex dependent. DNA methylation changes accompany imposed weaning, attenuating the difference between males and females. Maternal separation at birth alters the normal evolution of DNA methylation profiles and targets genes that are also affected by a later stage maternal separation, that is, weaning. Our results suggest that early life events dynamically interfere with the normal developmental evolution of the DNA methylation profile and that these changes are highly effected by sex.


Assuntos
Metilação de DNA/genética , Epigênese Genética/genética , Privação Materna , Estresse Psicológico/complicações , Animais , Feminino , Humanos , Acontecimentos que Mudam a Vida , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Fenótipo , Fatores Sexuais
4.
Fungal Biol ; 119(12): 1237-1245, 2015 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26615746

RESUMO

Mushroom Virus X (MVX) is associated with a range of symptoms observed in mushroom crops. The most prominent symptom in Ireland is the occurrence of 'brown' or 'off-white' mushrooms in white strain crops. The browning symptoms are associated with the presence of four low molecular weight dsRNAs: MVX(0.6), MVX(0.8), MVX(1.8) and MVX(2.0), however viral dsRNAs also occur in non-symptomatic mushrooms. Three virus-infected mushroom cultures containing MVX(1.8) and MVX(2.0) were used to infect experimental crops at different rates and at different times in the crop cycle to test the effect on symptom expression. Mushroom colour was measured by chromometer, and the ΔE value calculated. RT-PCR was used to test for the presence of MVX(1.8) dsRNA in harvested mushrooms. Results indicate that following infection, browning symptom expression is variable both within and between crops. Control mushrooms from 1st and 2nd flush had ΔE values of 7-12, with most being <10. In contrast, 1st flush mushrooms from virus infected treatments had ΔE values of 6-25, with most being >10 while 2nd flush mushrooms had ΔE values similar to controls. Only mushrooms with ΔE > 15 appeared visibly brown or off colour. The transient and inconsistent nature of MVX-associated browning symptoms is discussed.


Assuntos
Agaricus/virologia , Doenças das Plantas/virologia , Vírus de RNA/fisiologia , Verduras/virologia , Agaricus/genética , Agaricus/metabolismo , Proteínas Fúngicas/genética , Proteínas Fúngicas/metabolismo , Doenças das Plantas/genética , Vírus de RNA/genética , RNA de Cadeia Dupla/genética , RNA de Cadeia Dupla/metabolismo , Verduras/genética , Verduras/metabolismo
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(50): 20578-83, 2012 Dec 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23184974

RESUMO

To identify molecular mechanisms by which early life social conditions might influence adult risk of disease in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), we analyze changes in basal leukocyte gene expression profiles in 4-mo-old animals reared under adverse social conditions. Compared with the basal condition of maternal rearing (MR), leukocytes from peer-reared (PR) animals and PR animals provided with an inanimate surrogate mother (surrogate/peer reared, SPR) show enhanced expression of genes involved in inflammation, cytokine signaling, and T-lymphocyte activation, and suppression of genes involved in several innate antimicrobial defenses including type I interferon (IFN) antiviral responses. Promoter-based bioinformatic analyses implicate increased activity of CREB and NF-κB transcription factors and decreased activity of IFN response factors (IRFs) in structuring the observed differences in gene expression. Transcript origin analyses identify monocytes and CD4(+) T lymphocytes as primary cellular mediators of transcriptional up-regulation and B lymphocytes as major sources of down-regulated genes. These findings show that adverse social conditions can become embedded within the basal transcriptome of primate immune cells within the first 4 mo of life, and they implicate sympathetic nervous system-linked transcription control pathways as candidate mediators of those effects and potential targets for health-protective intervention.


Assuntos
Macaca mulatta/genética , Macaca mulatta/imunologia , Meio Social , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Biologia Computacional , Bases de Dados Genéticas , Feminino , Macaca mulatta/psicologia , Masculino , Comportamento Materno , Comportamento Social , Transcriptoma
6.
J Neurosci ; 32(44): 15626-42, 2012 Oct 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23115197

RESUMO

Early-life adversity is associated with a broad scope of life-long health and behavioral disorders. Particularly critical is the role of the mother. A possible mechanism is that these effects are mediated by "epigenetic" mechanisms. Studies in rodents suggest a causal relationship between early-life adversity and changes in DNA methylation in several "candidate genes" in the brain. This study examines whether randomized differential rearing (maternal vs surrogate-peer rearing) of rhesus macaques is associated with differential methylation in early adulthood. The data presented here show that differential rearing leads to differential DNA methylation in both prefrontal cortex and T cells. These differentially methylated promoters tend to cluster by both chromosomal region and gene function. The broad impact of maternal rearing on DNA methylation in both the brain and T cells supports the hypothesis that the response to early-life adversity is system-wide and genome-wide and persists to adulthood. Our data also point to the feasibility of studying the impact of the social environment in peripheral T-cell DNA methylation.


Assuntos
Metilação de DNA/fisiologia , Privação Materna , Córtex Pré-Frontal/metabolismo , Linfócitos T/metabolismo , Animais , Sequência de Bases , Cromossomos/genética , DNA Complementar/metabolismo , Imunoprecipitação , Hibridização In Situ , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Análise em Microsséries , Dados de Sequência Molecular , RNA/biossíntese , RNA/isolamento & purificação , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase em Tempo Real , Meio Social
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(23): 8866-71, 2012 Jun 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22615410

RESUMO

This paper exploits a unique ongoing experiment to analyze the effects of early rearing conditions on physical and mental health in a sample of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). We analyze the health records of 231 monkeys that were randomly allocated at birth across three rearing conditions: mother rearing, peer rearing, and surrogate peer rearing. We show that the lack of a secure attachment relationship in the early years engendered by adverse rearing conditions has detrimental long-term effects on health that are not compensated for by a normal social environment later in life.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica/fisiologia , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Privação Materna , Meio Social , Estresse Psicológico/fisiopatologia , Animais , Feminino , Nível de Saúde , Masculino , Comportamento Materno , Observação , Distribuição Aleatória , Análise de Regressão , Fatores Sexuais
8.
Child Dev ; 80(4): 1057-68, 2009.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19630893

RESUMO

The capacity to imitate facial gestures is highly variable in rhesus macaques and this variability may be related to differences in specific neurobehavioral patterns of development. This study evaluated the differential neonatal imitative response of 41 macaques in relation to the development of sensory, motor, and cognitive skills throughout the 1st month of life. The results show that infants who imitate facial gestures display more developed skills in goal-directed movements (reaching-grasping and fine hand motor control) than nonimitators. These differences might reflect, at least in part, the differential maturation of motor chains in the parietal and motor cortices, which partly overlap with those of the mirror neuron system. Thus, neonatal imitation appears to be a predictor of future neurobehavioral development.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Comportamento Imitativo , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Gestos , Força da Mão , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Córtex Motor/fisiologia , Neurônios Motores/fisiologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Distribuição Aleatória
9.
Am J Primatol ; 71(6): 510-22, 2009 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19373875

RESUMO

Developmental studies of pre- to postnatal continuities in rhesus monkeys sometimes require infants be reared with their mothers. However, complications during pregnancy or experimental designs may require cesarean delivery. Owing to lack of published information on this subject, strategies are needed to introduce mothers to their infants following cesarean delivery. Using positive and negative reinforcement techniques we attempted to unite six infant rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta, to their mothers following c-sections. For our seventh subject, we attempted to cross-foster an infant onto an unrelated female after she had undergone a cesarean surgery for a late-term spontaneous abortion. The mothers varied in age, parity, previous postnatal mothering experience with infants, housing earlier to delivery, and housing subsequent to introduction. Although there were large individual differences among the mother-infant pairs, all seven introductions were successful. The mothers learned to accept and care for their infants from the continuous application of operant conditioning techniques. These data suggest that mother-rearing following cesarean section is a realistic possibility whether required for clinical reasons or for proper experimental control. Furthermore, the ability to successfully mother-rear infants produced from cesarean delivery lessens the impact this potential confound of not being reared by their mothers exerts on many types of developmental studies.


Assuntos
Animais Recém-Nascidos/psicologia , Cesárea/veterinária , Macaca mulatta/psicologia , Mães/psicologia , Animais , Condicionamento Operante , Feminino , Macaca mulatta/cirurgia , Gravidez
10.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 89(2): 145-55, 2008 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18422015

RESUMO

Chen, Lakshminarayanan, and Santos (2006) claim to show in three choice experiments that monkeys react rationally to price and wealth shocks, but, when faced with gambles, display hallmark, human-like biases that include loss aversion. We present three experiments with monkeys and humans consistent with a reinterpretation of their data that attributes their results not to loss aversion, but to differences between choice alternatives in delay of reinforcement.


Assuntos
Afeto , Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Evolução Biológica , Cebus , Comportamento de Escolha , Comportamento Alimentar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reforço Psicológico
11.
Dev Psychobiol ; 50(4): 418-22, 2008 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19810188

RESUMO

A biological mother's movement appears necessary for optimal development in infant monkeys. However, nursery-reared monkeys are typically provided with inanimate surrogate mothers that move very little. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the effects of a novel, highly mobile surrogate mother on motor development, exploration, and reactions to novelty. Six infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) were reared on mobile hanging surrogates (MS) and compared to six infants reared on standard stationary rocking surrogates (RS) and to 9-15 infants reared with their biological mothers (MR) for early developmental outcome. We predicted that MS infants would develop more similarly to MR infants than RS infants. In neonatal assessments conducted at Day 30, both MS and MR infants showed more highly developed motor activity than RS infants on measures of grasping (p = .009), coordination (p = .038), spontaneous crawl (p = .009), and balance (p = .003). At 2-3 months of age, both MS and MR infants displayed higher levels of exploration in the home cage than RS infants (p = .016). In a novel situation in which only MS and RS infants were tested, MS infants spent less time near their surrogates in the first five minutes of the test session than RS infants (p = .05), indicating a higher level of comfort. Collectively, these results suggest that when nursery-rearing of infant monkeys is necessary, a mobile hanging surrogate may encourage more normative development of gross motor skills and exploratory behavior and may serve as a useful alternative to stationary or rocking surrogates.


Assuntos
Animais Recém-Nascidos/psicologia , Macaca mulatta/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Macaca mulatta/psicologia , Animais , Comportamento Exploratório , Feminino , Masculino , Comportamento Materno , Modelos Biológicos , Atividade Motora , Movimento , Orientação
12.
Am J Primatol ; 69(5): 584-90, 2007 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17216620

RESUMO

The development of self-biting behavior in captive monkeys is little understood and poses a serious risk to their well-being. Although early rearing conditions may influence the expression of this behavior, not all animals reared under similar conditions self-bite. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of three rearing conditions on biting behavior and to determine whether early infant behavior can predict later self-biting. The subjects were 370 rhesus macaques born at the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Animal Center between 1994 and 2004. They were reared under three conditions: mother-reared in social groups (n=183), peer-reared in groups of four (n=84), and surrogate-peer-reared (n=103). Significantly more surrogate-peer-reared animals self-bit compared to peer-only or mother-reared animals. There was no sex difference in self-biting, but this result may have been affected by a sex bias in the number of observations. The durations of behaviors exhibited by the surrogate-peer-reared subjects were recorded in 5-min sessions twice a week from 2 to 6 months of age while the animals were in their home cages and play groups. In the play-group situation, surrogate-peer-reared subjects who later self-bit were found to be less social and exhibited less social clinging than those that did not self-bite. Home-cage behavior did not predict later self-biting, but it did change with increasing age: surrogate clinging and self-mouthing decreased, while environmental exploration increased. Our findings suggest that surrogate rearing in combination with lower levels of social contact during play may be risk factors for the later development of self-biting behavior.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Macaca mulatta/psicologia , Comportamento Autodestrutivo , Comportamento Social , Animais , Feminino , Masculino
13.
Am J Primatol ; 69(9): 989-1000, 2007 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17253635

RESUMO

In Experiment 1, three capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) were exposed to a mirror in their home cage for 3 days and then given food treats for touching orange marks located on the surface of an experimental chamber. Following training, a mirror was added to the chamber to see if the monkeys would use it to guide non-reinforced contacts with an orange mark on their foreheads that was only visible as a mirror reflection (mark test). Two monkeys touched the head-mark more often with the mirror present than absent, but no mark touches were emitted while looking at the mirror. In Experiment 2, the monkeys were rewarded for touching orange marks on their bodies that were directly visible, followed by another head-mark test. Again, two monkeys touched the mark more often with the mirror present than absent, but these contacts were not emitted while looking at the mirror. Since facing the mirror while mark touching was not required for reinforcement during training, Experiment 3 further tested the possibility that increased mark touching in the presence of the mirror during Experiments 1 and 2 was the result of a memorial process. For this, a final, novel mark test was conducted using an orange mark on the neck that was only visible as a reflection (Experiment 3). No monkeys passed this test. These are the first mark tests given to capuchin monkeys. The results are consistent with the finding that no monkey species is capable of spontaneous mirror self-recognition. The implications of sequential training and mark testing for comparative evaluations of mirror self-recognition capacity are discussed.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Cebus/fisiologia , Cebus/psicologia , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Animais , Masculino
14.
PLoS Biol ; 4(9): e302, 2006 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16953662

RESUMO

The emergence of social behaviors early in life is likely crucial for the development of mother-infant relationships. Some of these behaviors, such as the capacity of neonates to imitate adult facial movements, were previously thought to be limited to humans and perhaps the ape lineage. Here we report the behavioral responses of infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) to the following human facial and hand gestures: lip smacking, tongue protrusion, mouth opening, hand opening, and opening and closing of eyes (control condition). In the third day of life, infant macaques imitate lip smacking and tongue protrusion. On the first day of life, the model's mouth openings elicited a similar matched behavior (lip smacking) in the infants. These imitative responses are present at an early stage of development, but they are apparently confined to a narrow temporal window. Because lip smacking is a core gesture in face-to-face interactions in macaques, neonatal imitation may serve to tune infants' affiliative responses to the social world. Our findings provide a quantitative description of neonatal imitation in a nonhuman primate species and suggest that these imitative capacities, contrary to what was previously thought, are not unique to the ape and human lineage. We suggest that their evolutionary origins may be traced to affiliative gestures with communicative functions.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Comportamento Imitativo , Macaca mulatta/fisiologia , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Expressão Facial , Feminino , Masculino , Comportamento Materno , Boca , Comunicação não Verbal , Comportamento Social , Fatores de Tempo , Língua
15.
J Comp Psychol ; 120(1): 67-73, 2006 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16551166

RESUMO

Each of 4 female capuchin monkeys ("model") was paired with another female capuchin ("witness") in an adjacent cage. In Phases 1 and 3, a model could remove a grape from the experimenter's hand while the witness watched. The witness was then offered a slice of cucumber, a less preferred food. Trials alternated between subjects 50 times, defining a session. In Phases 2 and 4, both were offered cucumber. Witness rejections of cucumber were infrequent and were not dependent on whether models received grape or cucumber. When models were offered cucumber, they rejected it at higher rates than did witnesses. These results fail to support findings of Brosnan and de Waal. An account based on the frustration effect accommodates these results and those of Brosnan and de Waal.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Comportamento Animal , Frustração , Comportamento Social , Animais , Cebus , Feminino , Alimentos , Distribuição Aleatória
16.
J Comp Psychol ; 120(1): 76, 2006 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16551168

RESUMO

P. G. Roma, A. Silberberg, A. M. Ruggiero, and S. J. Suomi (2006) noted that the results S. F. Brosnan and F. B. M. de Waal (2003) attributed to inequity aversion could also be explained as a frustration effect. Roma et al. redressed this confound by designing a procedure that could have supported either of these interpretations. Nevertheless, they found that only a frustration effect accounted for both their data and those of Brosnan and de Waal (2003). The criticisms Brosnan and de Waal (2006) offered of Roma et al. ignored the fact that Brosnan and de Waal's (2003) research design was not capable of offering an unequivocal demonstration of inequity aversion. This conclusion holds no matter what the claimed inadequacies of Roma et al.'s procedures might have been. Caution is urged in inferring the existence of inequity aversion in nonhuman primates.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem da Esquiva , Comportamento Animal , Comportamento Social , Animais , Frustração , Primatas , Teoria Psicológica
17.
Clin Cancer Res ; 11(13): 4741-8, 2005 Jul 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16000569

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Experimental data suggest a complex cross-talk between HER-2 and estrogen receptor, and it has been hypothesized that HER-2-positive tumors may be less responsive to certain endocrine treatments. Clinical data, however, have been conflicting. We have conducted a meta-analysis on the interaction between the response to endocrine treatment and the overexpression of HER-2 in metastatic breast cancer. EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN: Studies have been identified by searching the Medline, Embase, and American Society of Clinical Oncology abstract databases. Selection criteria were (a) metastatic breast cancer, (b) endocrine therapy (any line of treatment), and (c) evaluation of HER-2 expression (any method). For each study, the relative risk for treatment failure for HER-2-positive over HER-2-negative patients with 95% confidence interval was calculated as an estimate of the predictive effect of HER-2. Pooled estimates of the relative risk were computed by the Mantel-Haenszel method. RESULTS: Twelve studies (n = 2,379 patients) were included in the meta-analysis. The overall relative risk was 1.42 (95% confidence interval, 1.32-1.52; P < 0.00001; test for heterogeneity = 0.380). For studies involving tamoxifen, the pooled relative risk was 1.33 (95% confidence interval, 1.20-1.48; P < 0.00001; test for heterogeneity = 0.97); for studies involving other hormonal drugs, a pooled relative risk of 1.49 (95% confidence interval, 1.36-1.64; P < 0.00001; test for heterogeneity = 0.08) was estimated. A second meta-analysis limited to tumors that were either estrogen receptor positive, estrogen receptor unknown, or estrogen receptor negative/progesterone receptor positive yielded comparable results. CONCLUSIONS: HER-2-positive metastatic breast cancer is less responsive to any type of endocrine treatment. This effect holds in the subgroup of patients with positive or unknown steroid receptors.


Assuntos
Antineoplásicos Hormonais/uso terapêutico , Neoplasias da Mama/tratamento farmacológico , Receptor ErbB-2/metabolismo , Tamoxifeno/uso terapêutico , Neoplasias da Mama/metabolismo , Neoplasias da Mama/patologia , Feminino , Humanos , Receptores de Estrogênio/análise , Receptores de Progesterona/análise , Resultado do Tratamento
18.
Clin Cancer Res ; 9(3): 1039-46, 2003 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12631604

RESUMO

PURPOSE: Tamoxifen (TAM) is increasingly administered to new early breast cancer patients. Because it is not devoid of toxic effects, we studied factors potentially predictive of its efficacy. EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN: From 1978 to 1983, 433 patients were enrolled in the GUN randomized trial: 206 were assigned to TAM versus 227 controls (no-TAM). Premenopausal patients with axillary lymph node involvement (60 TAM versus 65 no-TAM) also received nine CMF cycles. Eight biological markers were retrospectively assayed for most patients: estrogen; progesterone; prolactin receptors (PrlRs); microvessel count (MVC); S-phase fraction; tumor ploidy; epidermal growth factor receptor (EGFR); and HER2. We performed a multivariate test of the TAM/covariate interactions to establish whether these variables predicted for TAM efficacy. Estimates of the TAM effect were expressed as hazard ratio (HR) of death of TAM over no-TAM patients with 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs). RESULTS: At a median follow-up of 15 years, PrlRs, MVC, S-phase fraction, ploidy, and EGFR did not influence TAM efficacy. Differently, HER2 had an overall significant predictive effect: HR = 0.59 (95% CI: 0.40-0.87) in HER2-negative subjects versus HR = 1.09 (95% CI: 0.63-1.87) in HER2-positive subjects (interaction test: P = 0.04). The predictive effect of HER2 was also evident in the subgroup of patients with steroid receptor-positive tumors (HER2 positive: HR = 1.33, 95% CI: 0.70-2.51; HER2 negative: HR = 0.73, 95% CI: 0.47-1.14). CONCLUSIONS: With the statistical power of the present randomized trial, S-phase, ploidy, EGFR, PrlR, and MVC do not seem to predict for TAM efficacy. Conversely, our data support the hypothesis that tumors overexpressing HER2 might not benefit from adjuvant TAM.


Assuntos
Neoplasias da Mama/tratamento farmacológico , Quimioterapia Adjuvante , Tamoxifeno/uso terapêutico , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Biomarcadores Tumorais , Ensaios Clínicos como Assunto , Receptores ErbB/biossíntese , Feminino , Humanos , Metástase Linfática , Microcirculação , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Neovascularização Patológica , Ploidias , Modelos de Riscos Proporcionais , Receptor ErbB-2/biossíntese , Receptores da Prolactina/biossíntese , Fase S , Fatores de Tempo
19.
Clín. méd. H.C.C ; 6(2): 93-96, mayo-ago. 2001. ilus
Artigo em Espanhol | LILACS | ID: lil-350662

RESUMO

La ablación por radiofrecuencia (RF) ha revolucionado el tratamiento de los pacientes con taquicardia supraventricular, ya sea generada por un mecanismo de reentrada en el nodo aurículoventricular o por la presencia de una conexión accesoria aurículoventricular. En los pacientes en los que el sustrato de la taquicardia está confinado en las aurículas y en los que la arritmia o la respuesta ventricular no puedan ser controladas con medicamentos, la ablación por catéter del Haz de His ofrece una opción a considerar. Sin embargo, esta modalidad de tratamiento obliga al implante de un marcapaso permanente sin hacer absolutamente nada por reestablecer la fisiología normal de las aurículas. Por otra parte, el manejo médico de las taquiarritmias auriculares puede ser difícil, en particular después de una cirugía cardiovascular por cardiopatía congénita, pudiendo ser una causa significante de morbi-mortalidad, además de los importantes efectos secundarios producidos por la utilización de medicamentos. Este manuscrito presenta la primera experiencia con éxito en la ciudad capital en la que se realizó ablación por radiofrecuencia de un Flutter (Aleteo) Auricular (FA) posterior al estudio electrofisiológico. Esta experiencia permite afirmar que este método debe ser considerado como una potencial cura para los pacientes con arritmias auriculares, tomando en cosideración la experiencia del operador y del laboratorio de electrofisiología, así como la incidencia de recurrencia de este tipo de patología


Assuntos
Humanos , Masculino , Adulto , Ablação por Cateter , Arritmias Cardíacas , Flutter Atrial , Taquicardia Supraventricular , Cardiologia , Eletrofisiologia , Venezuela
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA