Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 27
Filtrar
Mais filtros








Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Sci Adv ; 9(43): eadi5282, 2023 10 27.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37889969

RESUMO

Humans are strategic cooperators; we make decisions on the basis of costs and benefits to maintain high levels of cooperation, and this is thought to have played a key role in human evolution. In comparison, monkeys and apes might lack the cognitive capacities necessary to develop flexible forms of cooperation. We show that Guinea baboons (Papio papio) can use direct reciprocity and partner choice to develop and maintain high levels of cooperation in a prosocial choice task. Our findings demonstrate that monkeys have the cognitive capacities to adjust their level of cooperation strategically using a combination of partner choice and partner control strategies. Such capacities were likely present in our common ancestor and would have provided the foundations for the evolution of typically human forms of cooperation.


Assuntos
Hominidae , Papio papio , Animais , Humanos , Comportamento Cooperativo
2.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 2917, 2022 02 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35190637

RESUMO

When human infants are intentionally addressed by others, they tend to interpret the information communicated as being relevant to them and worth acquiring. For humans, this attribution of relevance leads to a preference to learn from communication, making it possible to accumulate knowledge over generations. Great apes are sensitive to communicative cues, but do these cues also activate an expectation of relevance? In an observational learning paradigm, we demonstrated to a sample of nonhuman great apes (bonobos, chimpanzees, orangutans; N = 24) how to operate on a food dispenser device. When apes had the opportunity to choose between an effective and an ineffective method in the baseline conditions, the majority of them chose the effective method. However, when the ineffective method was demonstrated in a communicative way, they failed to prioritize efficiency, even though they were equally attentive in both conditions. This suggests that the ostensive demonstration elicited an expectation of relevance that modified apes' interpretation of the situation, potentially leading to a preference to learn from communication, as human children do.


Assuntos
Comunicação Animal , Comportamento Animal/fisiologia , Hominidae/psicologia , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Animais , Sinais (Psicologia) , Humanos , Conhecimento , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Pongo
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 287(1937): 20202001, 2020 10 28.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33109010

RESUMO

While widely acknowledged in the cultural evolution literature, ecological factors-aspects of the physical environment that affect the way in which cultural productions evolve-have not been investigated experimentally. Here, we present an experimental investigation of this type of factor by using a transmission chain (iterated learning) experiment. We predicted that differences in the distance between identical tools (drums) and in the order in which they are to be used would cause the evolution of different rhythms. The evidence confirms our predictions and thus provides a proof of concept that ecological factors-here a motor constraint-can influence cultural productions and that their effects can be experimentally isolated and measured. One noteworthy finding is that ecological factors can on their own lead to more complex rhythms.


Assuntos
Evolução Cultural , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Atividade Motora , Humanos
4.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 18135, 2020 10 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33093599

RESUMO

We investigated whether communicative cues help observers to make sense of human interaction. We recorded EEG from an observer monitoring two individuals who were occasionally communicating with each other via either mutual eye contact and/or pointing gestures, and then jointly attending to the same object or attending to different objects that were placed on a table in front of them. The analyses were focussed on the processing of the interaction outcome (i.e. presence or absence of joint attention) and showed that its interpretation is a two-stage process, as reflected in the N300 and the N400 potentials. The N300 amplitude was reduced when the two individuals shared their focus of attention, which indicates the operation of a cognitive process that involves the relatively fast identification and evaluation of actor-object relationships. On the other hand, the N400 was insensitive to the sharing or distribution of the two individuals' attentional focus. Interestingly, the N400 was reduced when the interaction outcome was preceded either by mutual eye contact or by a perceived pointing gesture. This shows that observation of communication "opens up" the mind to a wider range of action possibilities and thereby helps to interpret unusual outcomes of social interactions.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Comunicação , Sinais (Psicologia) , Potenciais Evocados , Gestos , Relações Interpessoais , Adulto , Cognição , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Cogn Sci ; 44(6): e12866, 2020 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32535972

RESUMO

The spatial composition of human portraits obeys historically changing cultural norms. We show that it is also affected by cognitive factors that cause greater spontaneous attention to what is in front rather in the back of an agent. Scenes with more space in front of a directed object are both more often produced and judged as more aesthetically pleasant. This leads to the prediction that, in profile-oriented human portraits, compositions with more space in front of depicted agents (a "forward bias") should be over-represented. By analyzing a large dataset (total N of 1,831 paintings by 582 unique identified European painters from the 15th to the 20th century), we found evidence of this forward bias: Painters tended to put more free space in front of, rather than behind, the sitters. Additionally, we found evidence that this forward bias became stronger when cultural norms of spatial composition favoring centering became less stringent.


Assuntos
Orientação Espacial , Viés , Emoções , Humanos , Pinturas
6.
PLoS One ; 14(10): e0224648, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31665195

RESUMO

What role does children's trust in communication play in their acquisition of new meanings? To answer, we report two experimental studies (N = 81) testing how three- to four-year-olds interpret the meaning of a novel communicative device when it is used by a malevolent and potentially deceptive informant. Children participated in a hiding game in which they had to find a reward hidden in one of two boxes. In the initial phase of the experiments, a malevolent informant always indicated the location of the empty box using a novel communicative device, either a marker (Study 1), or an arrow (Study 2). During that phase, 3- and 4-year-olds learned to avoid the box indicated by the novel communicative device. In the second phase of the test, the malevolent informant was replaced by a benevolent one. Nevertheless, children did not change their search strategy, and they kept avoiding the box tagged by the novel communicative device as often as when it had been produced by the malevolent informant. These results suggest that during the initial phase, children (i) did not consider the possibility that the malevolent informant might intend to deceive them, and (ii) did not ignore the unfamiliar communicative signal or treat it as irrelevant. Instead, children relied on the unfamiliar communicative signal to locate the empty box's location. These results suggest that children's avoidance of the location indicated by an unfamiliar signal is not unambiguous evidence for distrust of such signal. We argue that children's trust in ostensive communication is likely to extend to unfamiliar communicative means, and that this presumption of trustworthiness plays a central role in children's acquisition of new meanings.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Confiança , Pré-Escolar , Enganação , Humanos
7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 42: e183, 2019 Sep 12.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31511107

RESUMO

I argue, with examples, that most human cognitive skills are neither instincts nor gadgets but mechanisms shaped both by evolved dispositions and by cultural inputs. This shaping can work either through evolved skills fulfilling their function with the help of cultural skills that they contribute to shape, or through cultural skills recruiting evolved skills and adjusting to them.

8.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e182, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31064532

RESUMO

Boyer & Petersen (B&P) assume that the intuitive systems underlying folk-economic beliefs (FEBs), and, in particular, emporiophobia, evolved in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness (EEA), before markets. This makes the historical development of markets puzzling. We suggest that what evolved in the EEA are templates that help children develop intuitive systems partly adjusted to their cultural environment. This helps resolve the puzzle.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Cognição , Criança , Humanos
9.
J Child Lang ; 44(5): 1041-1064, 2017 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27748210

RESUMO

We suggest that preschoolers' frequent obliviousness to the risks and opportunities of deception comes from a trusting stance supporting verbal communication. Three studies (N = 125) confirm this hypothesis. Three-year-olds can hide information from others (Study 1) and they can lie (Study 2) in simple settings. Yet when one introduces the possibility of informing others in the very same settings, three-year-olds tend to be honest (Studies 1 and 2). Similarly, four-year-olds, though capable of treating assertions as false, trust deceptive informants (Study 3). We suggest that children's reduced sensitivity to the opportunities of lying, and to the risks of being lied to might help explain their difficulties on standard false belief tasks.


Assuntos
Comunicação , Cultura , Enganação , Otimismo/psicologia , Confiança/psicologia , Comportamento Verbal , Caráter , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Teste de Realidade , Percepção da Fala
10.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 27(4): 692-704, 2015 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25321487

RESUMO

Logical connectives (e.g., or, if, and not) are central to everyday conversation, and the inferences they generate are made with little effort in pragmatically sound situations. In contrast, the neural substrates of logical inference-making have been studied exclusively in abstract tasks where pragmatic concerns are minimal. Here, we used fMRI in an innovative design that employed narratives to investigate the interaction between logical reasoning and pragmatic processing in natural discourse. Each narrative contained three premises followed by a statement. In Fully-deductive stories, the statement confirmed a conclusion that followed from two steps of disjunction-elimination (e.g., Xavier considers Thursday, Friday, or Saturday for inviting his girlfriend out; he removes Thursday before he rejects Saturday and declares "I will invite her out for Friday"). In Implicated-premise stories, an otherwise identical narrative included three premises that twice removed a single option from consideration (i.e., Xavier rejects Thursday for two different reasons). The conclusion therefore necessarily prompts an implication (i.e., Xavier must have removed Saturday from consideration as well). We report two main findings. First, conclusions of Implicated-premise stories are associated with more activity than conclusions of Fully-deductive stories in a bilateral frontoparietal system, suggesting that these regions play a role in inferring an implicated premise. Second, brain connectivity between these regions increases with pragmatic abilities when reading conclusions in Implicated-premise stories. These findings suggest that pragmatic processing interacts with logical inference-making when understanding arguments in narrative discourse.


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Compreensão/fisiologia , Lógica , Pensamento/fisiologia , Estimulação Acústica , Análise de Variância , Encéfalo/irrigação sanguínea , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Vias Neurais/irrigação sanguínea , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Oxigênio/sangue , Leitura , Fatores de Tempo , Adulto Jovem
11.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e63, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786276

RESUMO

As Kline envisages, there is an important relationship between cultural attraction and teaching. The very function of teaching is to make the content taught an attractor. Teaching, moreover, typically fulfills its function by exploiting a variety of factors of cultural attraction that help make its content learnable and teachable.


Assuntos
Diversidade Cultural , Currículo , Humanos
12.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 369(1642): 20130368, 2014 May 19.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24686939

RESUMO

Darwin-inspired population thinking suggests approaching culture as a population of items of different types, whose relative frequencies may change over time. Three nested subtypes of populational models can be distinguished: evolutionary, selectional and replicative. Substantial progress has been made in the study of cultural evolution by modelling it within the selectional frame. This progress has involved idealizing away from phenomena that may be critical to an adequate understanding of culture and cultural evolution, particularly the constructive aspect of the mechanisms of cultural transmission. Taking these aspects into account, we describe cultural evolution in terms of cultural attraction, which is populational and evolutionary, but only selectional under certain circumstances. As such, in order to model cultural evolution, we must not simply adjust existing replicative or selectional models but we should rather generalize them, so that, just as replicator-based selection is one form that Darwinian selection can take, selection itself is one of several different forms that attraction can take. We present an elementary formalization of the idea of cultural attraction.


Assuntos
Evolução Cultural , Modelos Teóricos , Dinâmica Populacional , Humanos
13.
PLoS One ; 8(6): e67371, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23840683

RESUMO

Little is known about the spread of emotions beyond dyads. Yet, it is of importance for explaining the emergence of crowd behaviors. Here, we experimentally addressed whether emotional homogeneity within a crowd might result from a cascade of local emotional transmissions where the perception of another's emotional expression produces, in the observer's face and body, sufficient information to allow for the transmission of the emotion to a third party. We reproduced a minimal element of a crowd situation and recorded the facial electromyographic activity and the skin conductance response of an individual C observing the face of an individual B watching an individual A displaying either joy or fear full body expressions. Critically, individual B did not know that she was being watched. We show that emotions of joy and fear displayed by A were spontaneously transmitted to C through B, even when the emotional information available in B's faces could not be explicitly recognized. These findings demonstrate that one is tuned to react to others' emotional signals and to unintentionally produce subtle but sufficient emotional cues to induce emotional states in others. This phenomenon could be the mark of a spontaneous cooperative behavior whose function is to communicate survival-value information to conspecifics.


Assuntos
Expressão Facial , Medo/psicologia , Felicidade , Percepção Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Comportamento Cooperativo , Sinais (Psicologia) , Músculos Faciais/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Comportamento de Massa , Estimulação Luminosa , Adulto Jovem
14.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(1): 102-13, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23560336

RESUMO

Our discussion of the commentaries begins, at the evolutionary level, with issues raised by our account of the evolution of morality in terms of partner-choice mutualism. We then turn to the cognitive level and the characterization and workings of fairness. In a final section, we discuss the degree to which our fairness-based approach to morality extends to norms that are commonly considered moral even though they are distinct from fairness.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Casamento , Princípios Morais , Parceiros Sexuais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino
15.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(1): 59-78, 2013 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23445574

RESUMO

What makes humans moral beings? This question can be understood either as a proximate "how" question or as an ultimate "why" question. The "how" question is about the mental and social mechanisms that produce moral judgments and interactions, and has been investigated by psychologists and social scientists. The "why" question is about the fitness consequences that explain why humans have morality, and has been discussed by evolutionary biologists in the context of the evolution of cooperation. Our goal here is to contribute to a fruitful articulation of such proximate and ultimate explanations of human morality. We develop an approach to morality as an adaptation to an environment in which individuals were in competition to be chosen and recruited in mutually advantageous cooperative interactions. In this environment, the best strategy is to treat others with impartiality and to share the costs and benefits of cooperation equally. Those who offer less than others will be left out of cooperation; conversely, those who offer more will be exploited by their partners. In line with this mutualistic approach, the study of a range of economic games involving property rights, collective actions, mutual help and punishment shows that participants' distributions aim at sharing the costs and benefits of interactions in an impartial way. In particular, the distribution of resources is influenced by effort and talent, and the perception of each participant's rights on the resources to be distributed.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Casamento , Princípios Morais , Parceiros Sexuais , Adulto , Comportamento Cooperativo , Feminino , Humanos , Relações Interpessoais , Masculino , Comportamento Social
17.
18.
Behav Brain Sci ; 34(2): 57-74; discussion 74-111, 2011 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21447233

RESUMO

Reasoning is generally seen as a means to improve knowledge and make better decisions. However, much evidence shows that reasoning often leads to epistemic distortions and poor decisions. This suggests that the function of reasoning should be rethought. Our hypothesis is that the function of reasoning is argumentative. It is to devise and evaluate arguments intended to persuade. Reasoning so conceived is adaptive given the exceptional dependence of humans on communication and their vulnerability to misinformation. A wide range of evidence in the psychology of reasoning and decision making can be reinterpreted and better explained in the light of this hypothesis. Poor performance in standard reasoning tasks is explained by the lack of argumentative context. When the same problems are placed in a proper argumentative setting, people turn out to be skilled arguers. Skilled arguers, however, are not after the truth but after arguments supporting their views. This explains the notorious confirmation bias. This bias is apparent not only when people are actually arguing, but also when they are reasoning proactively from the perspective of having to defend their opinions. Reasoning so motivated can distort evaluations and attitudes and allow erroneous beliefs to persist. Proactively used reasoning also favors decisions that are easy to justify but not necessarily better. In all these instances traditionally described as failures or flaws, reasoning does exactly what can be expected of an argumentative device: Look for arguments that support a given conclusion, and, ceteris paribus, favor conclusions for which arguments can be found.


Assuntos
Cognição , Tomada de Decisões , Resolução de Problemas , Teoria Psicológica , Pensamento , Evolução Biológica , Comportamento de Escolha , Formação de Conceito , Humanos , Conhecimento
19.
Commun Integr Biol ; 3(4): 350-1, 2010 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20798823

RESUMO

Social learning mechanisms are usually assumed to explain both the spread and the persistence of cultural behavior. In a recent article, we showed that the fidelity of social learning commonly found in transmission chain experiments is not high enough to explain cultural stability. Here we want to both enrich and qualify this conclusion by looking at the case of song transmission in song birds, which can be faithful to the point of being true replication. We argue that this high fidelity results from natural selection pressure on cognitive mechanisms. This observation strengthens our main argument. Social learning mechanisms are unlikely to be faithful enough to explain cultural stability because they are generally selected not for high fidelity but for generalization and adjustment to the individual's needs, capacities and situation.

20.
Science ; 329(5990): 388-9; author reply 389-90, 2010 Jul 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20651133
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA