Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 69
Filtrar
1.
Neurology ; 43(1): 106-10, 1993 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8423871

RESUMO

We investigated the ability of 25 patients with left unilateral spatial neglect to make a clock face by putting numbers inside a printed circle. Impairment seen in this clock-drawing test did not parallel neglect severity as judged by results of the line-cancellation and line-bisection tests, as well as the copying of a daisy. The score for clock drawing correlated highly with the verbal WAIS score. Most neglect patients with a verbal IQ of 87 or more could draw a clock face fairly well and used planning in placing the numbers 12, 3, 6, and 9 before the others. In clock drawing, verbal intelligence may compensate for left unilateral spatial neglect. We therefore recommend use of the line-cancellation and line-bisection tests, as well as the copying test, but do not recommend use of the clock-drawing test in the diagnosis of left unilateral spatial neglect.


Assuntos
Atenção , Encefalopatias/diagnóstico , Infarto Cerebral/complicações , Testes de Inteligência , Idoso , Encefalopatias/etiologia , Hemiplegia/etiologia , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
2.
Neurology ; 44(2): 294-8, 1994 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8309578

RESUMO

Patients with left unilateral spatial neglect following lesions that mainly involved the right parietal lobe performed a line extension task, extending a horizontal line leftward to double its original length. We examined line extension performances in the left and right hemispaces, as well as in the midline, to assess whether spatial conditions affected these performances. Whatever the severity of neglect found in the line bisection test, the line extension performances of the patients were almost accurate and comparable with those of normal controls across the three spatial conditions. The neglect patients executed movements in or toward the contralesional space as the task oriented their attention sufficiently to the left. The results suggest that the motor component, ie, directional hypokinesia, has little part in left unilateral spatial neglect due to right parietal lesions.


Assuntos
Dano Encefálico Crônico/fisiopatologia , Lobo Parietal , Percepção Espacial , Adulto , Idoso , Dano Encefálico Crônico/psicologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Testes de Inteligência , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fatores de Tempo
3.
Neurology ; 40(9): 1395-8, 1990 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2392225

RESUMO

We examined 8 patients with moderate to severe left unilateral spatial neglect by means of a series of line cancellation tasks. We asked the patients to cross out lines in the 1st trial, to number lines in the 2nd trial, and again to cross out lines in the 3rd trial. In the 2nd trial with numbering, all the patients showed improvement of left unilateral spatial neglect. Numbering involved the successive use of increasing numbers. This process seemed to motivate the patients to continue searching for another line, even at the point when they would be satisfied that they had completed the task if they used simple crossing-out. Insufficient motivation for visuospatial searching may play an important role in unilateral spatial neglect observed in cancellation tasks.


Assuntos
Negação em Psicologia , Percepção Espacial , Idoso , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/psicologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Hemiplegia/fisiopatologia , Hemiplegia/psicologia , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
4.
Neurology ; 55(3): 364-70, 2000 Aug 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10932269

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Unilateral spatial neglect has been rarely reported in patients with AD, although they often have right and left asymmetry of temporoparietal dysfunction. OBJECTIVE: To investigate if patients with AD would show unilateral spatial neglect in the line bisection test, and to reveal the relationship between their neglect and the area of cerebral dysfunction. METHOD: Thirty-two patients with mild to moderate AD and 32 age-matched healthy control subjects underwent an extensive line bisection test. SPECT was also obtained for the patients. RESULTS: Rightward bisection errors exceeded the normal range in 25% of patients with AD. They exhibited greater rightward errors for the longer lines in the left hemispace than in the right hemispace, and with the right hand than with the left hand; this corresponds to the characteristics of neglect seen after right hemisphere lesions. All patients who bisected 200 mm lines with errors over 10 mm showed disproportionate lowering of performance IQ and asymmetric right hemisphere hypoperfusion, especially in the temporoparietal region. Seventy-five percent of the patients performed normally in the center presentation but erred slightly toward the body midline in the right and left hemispaces. CONCLUSION: Left unilateral spatial neglect in mild to moderate AD may be rather common if tested with the line bisection test. Rightward errors over 10 mm suggest right temporoparietal dysfunction. In AD, three or more bisections of 200 mm lines in the center presentation are recommended for detection of neglect. Patients with AD but without neglect may have difficulty in shifting attention into the peripheral sector of the egocentric space.


Assuntos
Agnosia/diagnóstico por imagem , Agnosia/fisiopatologia , Doença de Alzheimer/diagnóstico por imagem , Doença de Alzheimer/fisiopatologia , Percepção Espacial , Idoso , Agnosia/etiologia , Doença de Alzheimer/complicações , Atrofia , Córtex Cerebral/patologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Tomografia Computadorizada de Emissão de Fóton Único , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 25(4): 675-9, 1987.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3658150

RESUMO

Eye-fixation patterns while viewing simple patterns were quantitatively analysed in homonymous hemianopic subjects with or without unilateral spatial neglect (USN), using an eye camera. Hemianopic subjects without USN used the strategy to look at the hemianopic side of the patterns longer in order to compensate for their visual field defect. However, those with USN lacked this compensatory eye-fixation pattern.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/fisiopatologia , Fixação Ocular , Hemianopsia/fisiopatologia , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Campos Visuais
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 37(1): 75-82, 1999 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9920473

RESUMO

Patients with relatively mild unilateral spatial neglect underwent the line bisection task with self-estimation, where they were asked two different questions about the relationship of their subjective midpoint to the line. For the first trial, all patients responded by "yes" when asked whether their subjective midpoint was placed at the center of the line, but by "left" or "right" when asked to indicate the longer segment of the bisected line. In the subsequent trials, they repeatedly reported that their subjective midpoints were placed at the true center, even after they had recognized their bisection errors when indicating the longer segment. Of the 32 patients, 28 kept on showing this pattern of responses in almost all the 30 trials. Moreover, the patients who could estimate the longer segment showed no improvement of line bisection throughout the trials. Their performances strongly depended upon the task demand regarding the subjective midpoint. These results demonstrate that the patients treated the two related tasks as independent. Accordingly, they could not improve their performances on one task with the feedback from their performances on the other. The lack of association between the responses to two related task demands may be one of the factors that prevent patients with unilateral spatial neglect from improving the deficit.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Idoso , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/complicações , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/psicologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Prognóstico
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 34(8): 819-26, 1996 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8817511

RESUMO

We reported a dramatic phenomenon of appearance and disappearance of left unilateral spatial neglect for the same single object. In experiment 1, four patients with mild neglect who copied a single flower satisfactorily showed left neglect in copying of the same flower when it was accompanied with some peripheral stimuli. In experiment 2, we included three additional conditions to determine the stimulus component crucial for left neglect of the flower: a flower with a stem and a flower with either right or left peripheral stimuli were also used for copying. Overall results showed that neglect was most evident for the flower with the right peripheral stimuli. On the other hand, the presence of left stimuli provoked less severe neglect of the flower, which was as mild as that in the single flower condition. The results were explained in terms of an attentional mechanism underlying unilateral spatial neglect. When copying the flower with peripheral stimuli on both sides or on the right side, the stimuli on the right side might attract the patient's attention. Consequently, he exhibited left neglect on the central flower. On the other hand, neglect was less evident in the other conditions, since the figures did not include such attention-attracting stimuli on the right side.


Assuntos
Infarto Cerebral/complicações , Lateralidade Funcional , Transtornos da Memória/complicações , Adulto , Idoso , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
8.
Neuropsychologia ; 34(10): 965-71, 1996 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8843062

RESUMO

The present study examined 13 patients with typical left unilateral spatial neglect to explore the mechanisms of neglect in copying a single object. All patients identified the figure of a sunflower as a flower that had petals all around the central circle. Besides, in the free viewing of the flowers prepared by the examiner, 10 of the 13 patients could discriminate between the presence and absence of the left-sided petals. When copying the flower, however, they always left the left side unfinished without noticing the incompleteness. Whether perception in the left visual field was impaired or not was irrelevant to the performances in the copying and the free viewing of the flowers. We consider the mechanism of left unilateral spatial neglect in copying a flower as follows. Small leftward shift of attention may take place during the free observation of the stimulus flower. While copying, however, neglect patients pay attention exclusively to the right-sided petals they are drawing. The strong engagement of attention to the right side results in the lack of discovery of the left-sided omission. The close integration of attentional and premotor mechanisms seems to be crucial for the appearance of unilateral spatial neglect in copying a single object.


Assuntos
Lobo Frontal/fisiopatologia , Hemianopsia/fisiopatologia , Lobo Occipital/fisiopatologia , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Lobo Temporal/fisiopatologia , Idoso , Feminino , Fixação Ocular , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Campos Visuais
9.
Neuropsychologia ; 38(11): 1466-72, 2000.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10906372

RESUMO

Cube-copying is often used to assess constructional ability of brain-damaged patients and the influence of unilateral spatial neglect is often pointed out in patients with right hemisphere lesions. However, some patients with severe neglect perform cube-copying satisfactorily. The aim of the present study is to identify the factors that affect the performance of cube-copying in patients with left unilateral spatial neglect. Constructional performance was investigated in 100 patients with unilateral spatial neglect using a task to copy the Necker cube. The relationship of the patients' cube-copying performance to the severity of their neglect, as well as other factors (verbal intelligence, age, duration after onset of the disease, educational level, lesion site, piecemeal approach, and side of starting to copy) was analyzed. Twenty-two normal subjects also participated in this study as controls. Among many factors adopted for analysis, neglect severity and verbal intelligence were found to be primary factors affecting the cube-copying performance of the patients with unilateral spatial neglect. The effect of neglect severity on cube-copying performance was apparent in the patients whose verbal intelligence was deteriorated, but was not observed in the patients with preserved verbal intelligence. Similarly, the effect of verbal intelligence on cube-copying performance was apparent in the patients with severe neglect, but not in the patients with mild neglect. We conclude that constructional ability in the copying of a cube is determined by verbal intelligence, as well as by the severity of unilateral spatial neglect.


Assuntos
Inteligência , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Transtornos Psicomotores/psicologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Idoso , Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Percepção de Profundidade/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Inteligência/fisiologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico , Transtornos Psicomotores/fisiopatologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Vocabulário
10.
J Neurol ; 243(1): 1-8, 1996 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8869379

RESUMO

We analysed the performances of 69 patients with left unilateral spatial neglect in copying a multi-object figure, as well as their performance of the line cancellation and line bisection tests. Three patterns of copying were mainly observed. The first pattern was almost satisfactory performance in copying the central figure with occasional omission of the elements located more laterally to the left. The second pattern was omission of the left half of the whole figure, which was the pattern expected of patients with typical left unilateral spatial neglect. The third pattern was left unilateral spatial neglect when copying the central figure, with favourable copying of left lateral objects; this may be regarded as "object-centered" neglect. Performance of the line cancellation test, which requires exploratory ability, was significantly better for the patients with the third pattern than for those with the second pattern. A significant difference was also observed between the two groups of patients in performance of the line bisection test, in which exploratory ability plays a more minor role. No significant difference was found in performance of the line cancellation and line bisection tests between the patients with the third and first patterns, in spite of remarkable differences in their copying performances. No distinctive characteristics were found for any group of patients as regards lesion, age, duration after onset, education, and WAIS scores. Therefore we concluded that in most of our patients with neglect, these diverse patterns of copying deficits resulted from different levels of neglect severity but not from different types of unilateral spatial neglect.


Assuntos
Atenção , Lateralidade Funcional , Percepção Espacial , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtornos Cerebrovasculares/psicologia , Humanos , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
11.
J Neurol ; 242(6): 367-73, 1995 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7561964

RESUMO

Patients with typical left unilateral spatial neglect bisected lines after cueing to the left end-point, the fixation point being monitored with an eye camera. They persisted with the point of initial fixation made after cueing and placed the mark there without searching leftwards again. The rightward shift of fixation to the initial point of fixation thus determined the location of the subjective midpoint. We consider that rightward attentional bias increased the amplitude of this shift that was planned on the basis of the perception of the whole line while cueing. This hypothesis may explain smaller but obvious rightward bisection errors found in the cueing condition.


Assuntos
Atenção , Sinais (Psicologia) , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Processos Mentais , Percepção Espacial , Campos Visuais , Idoso , Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Desempenho Psicomotor
12.
J Neurol ; 243(5): 381-7, 1996 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8741077

RESUMO

We examined the eye-fixation pattern of a patient with severe left unilateral spatial neglect who showed leftward searches of various extent in more than half of line bisection trials. Because of complete left homonymous hemianopia, he perceived only the segment of the line between its right endpoint and the point of the leftmost fixation. In the trials with leftward searches, he frequently placed the subjective midpoint on the right part of the perceived segment. In the trials without leftward searches, he placed it near the left extreme point of the perceived segment. For all these bisections, the subjective midpoint was constantly placed far to the right of the true midpoint of the line irrespective of the length perceived. We consider that in severe left unilateral spatial neglect, rightward attentional bias is the predominant factor that determines where to place the subjective midpoint. Transient attentional shift to the left may produce leftward searches, but it does not induce effective processing of line bisection.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Sinais (Psicologia) , Fixação Ocular/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Idoso , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X
13.
J Neurol ; 244(10): 607-12, 1997 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9402535

RESUMO

We examined the effect of line length and viewing distance on the line bisection performance in near space in five patients with left unilateral spatial neglect following right parietal lesions. A line bisection task by fixation was devised to avoid the influence of manual responses. The rightward deviation measured in visual angle increased linearly as a function of the visual angle of lines 150 mm or more long. This linearity, however, did not hold for lines of 100 mm or less. The deviation measured in length was nearly constant for each of these short lengths, even when the visual angle was varied at different viewing distances. The patients therefore discriminated the objective lengths of the short lines. For small objects, neglect patients may distribute attention mainly on the coordinates scaled for objective size.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Idoso , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos
14.
J Neurol ; 235(2): 69-73, 1987 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2828557

RESUMO

Two "sporadic" cases of amyloid polyneuropathy are reported. There was no family history or plasma cell dyscrasia. Both showed sensorimotor and autonomic polyneuropathy with onset in the seventh decade. Amyloid deposits in both cases reacted with anti-human prealbumin sera but not with antisera to human AA and anti-human immunoglobulin light-chain amyloids, including A kappa and A lambda. One patient had the abnormal serum prealbumin and abnormal DNA sequence found in type I familial amyloid polyneuropathy (FAP)(Japanese type). Investigations in "sporadic" amyloid polyneuropathy should include immunohistochemistry, using antisera to the different amyloid proteins, and the radioimmunoassay and recombinant DNA techniques for diagnosis of FAP.


Assuntos
Amiloidose/metabolismo , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso Periférico/metabolismo , Pré-Albumina/metabolismo , Proteína Amiloide A Sérica/metabolismo , Idoso , Amiloidose/patologia , Amiloidose/fisiopatologia , Histocitoquímica , Humanos , Masculino , Miocárdio/metabolismo , Miocárdio/patologia , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso Periférico/patologia , Doenças do Sistema Nervoso Periférico/fisiopatologia , Pele/metabolismo , Pele/patologia
15.
Brain Lang ; 63(2): 256-75, 1998 Jun 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9654434

RESUMO

Left unilateral spatial neglect (USN) and spatial agraphia are both caused by right hemisphere damage. However, effects of USN on spatial agraphia have not been fully investigated. This study examined performances of patients with or without neglect and normals on single letter writing, using Japanese unique writing systems, kana (phonogram) and kanji (ideogram). Our patients exhibited agraphia of kanji, but not of kana. However, accuracy of writing was not associated to neglect severity, and errors for writing to dictation occurred either on the left or right side of kanji. Right frontal damage seemed to be related to the genesis of duplication or omission errors of repeated elements. Effect of USN was limited to spatial arrangement of letters and copying performance.


Assuntos
Agrafia/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico , Percepção Espacial , Campos Visuais , Idoso , Agrafia/etiologia , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Encéfalo/patologia , Infarto Cerebral/complicações , Infarto Cerebral/diagnóstico , China , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos da Percepção/etiologia , Tomografia Computadorizada por Raios X
16.
Rinsho Shinkeigaku ; 37(12): 1122-4, 1997 Dec.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9577665

RESUMO

Damage to the minor hemisphere or the right hemisphere of most right-handed subjects produces various neuropsychological disorders. Unilateral spatial neglect is the deficit to respond or orient to stimuli on the left side. The inferior parietal lobule is most frequently involved in the lesions of neglect patients, but lesions confined to the lobule may not cause chronic neglect. Severe and lasting neglect is frequently found in patients with an infarction in the territory of the middle cerebral artery or the anterior choroidal artery. Neglect is usually mild or moderate after frontal lesions. The most important mechanism of neglect seems to be disorders in spatial attention: rightward attentional bias, and impairment in disengagement and shift of attention from the right side. Directional hypokinesia is rarely observed in the chronic stage. Non-spatial factors, such as motivational deficit, insufficient compensation with verbal intelligence, or disuse of an appropriate spatial strategy may also contribute to the appearance of neglect. Anosognosia for hemiplegia is found in about half of patients with acute cerebrovascular accidents in the right hemisphere. Generalized attentional and intellectual deficits, diffuse brain atrophy or hypometabolism, and presence of multiple infarction may result in chronic anosognosia. Constructional disability is observed in about 30% patients with either hemisphere damage. Dressing apraxia is more frequent in right hemisphere damage. These two disorders, however, rarely appear as isolated neuropsychological deficits. Most patients show some of the accompanying disorders, such as unilateral spatial neglect, anosognosia, anosodiaphoria, and generalized attentional and intellectual impairment. By contrast, motor neglect may occur independently of the other disorders. Motor neglect follows damage to either hemisphere, although it occurs more frequently after right hemisphere damage. Neuropsychological deficits characteristic of right hemisphere damage may be unilateral spatial neglect in the acute and chronic stages and anosognosia for hemiplegia in the acute stage.


Assuntos
Infarto Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Apraxias/etiologia , Hemiplegia/complicações , Humanos
17.
Rinsho Shinkeigaku ; 41(12): 1128-30, 2001 Dec.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12235816

RESUMO

Directed attention is a function to direct and shift the focus of awareness adequately to behaviorally relevant sensory events. Healthy subjects direct attention evenly to right and left hemispaces. Unilateral spatial neglect is a failure to respond normally to stimuli on the side opposite a cerebral lesion, which is considered to represent a unilateral disruption of directed attention. The established clinical observation that neglect usually occurs after right hemisphere lesions and the results of functional imaging studies suggest the right hemisphere dominance for directed attention. It is hypothesized that the right hemisphere distributes attention to space bilaterally, whereas the left hemisphere distributes attention primarily to right hemispace. However, patients with callosotomy show no apparent neglect with either right or left hand. Ishiai et al. (2001) reported detailed analyses of eye movements when a patient with a callosal infarction bisected lines. Left unilateral spatial neglect may appear, when use of the right hand induces a rightward bias in the attentional control of the left hemisphere and damage to its cingulate gyrus inhibits interhemispheric integration of attention. By contrast, the disconnected but intact right hemisphere may bisect a line accurately by integrating attention to the extents perceived in the left and right visual fields.


Assuntos
Atenção/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Campos Visuais/fisiologia , Conscientização/fisiologia , Corpo Caloso/fisiopatologia , Humanos
18.
Rinsho Shinkeigaku ; 38(9): 801-5, 1998 Sep.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10078030

RESUMO

A 69-year-old women was admitted to Tokyo Medical and Dental University Hospital because of slowly progressive difficulty in speech production over 8-years. On admission, her spontaneous speech was non-fluent, limited to one-syllable utterance, and severely efforty. But her visual and auditory comprehension was preserved. There was no significant general intellectual deterioration. Severe buccofacial apraxia, but no swallowing was observed. So we considered her difficulty in speech as aphemia. Three-dimensionally reconstructed surface MR image clearly showed severe atrophy in the posterior part of the left inferior frontal gyrus and the lower part of the left pre-central gyrus. The FDG-PET demonstrated a focal hypometabolism in the same region. The lesion in this area was suggested to be a cause of speech production difficulty in this case.


Assuntos
Afasia de Broca/diagnóstico , Idoso , Afasia de Broca/patologia , Atrofia , Progressão da Doença , Feminino , Lobo Frontal/patologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Tomografia Computadorizada de Emissão
19.
Rinsho Shinkeigaku ; 34(4): 336-40, 1994 Apr.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8026126

RESUMO

A 67-year-old, right-handed man was admitted to our hospital because of restricted field of vision. Neurological examination revealed left upper quadrantanopsia on confrontation. Axial and coronal T2 weighted MR images showed an increased signal intensity lesion that involved the right fusiform and lingual gyri. A small lesion was also noted in the white matter of the left cuneate gyrus. He was unable to describe the ground plan of his house or the well-known route he lived. No signs of unilateral spatial neglect, prosopangnosia, apraxia, and aphasia were found in specific tests. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale showed verbal IQ 126 and performance IQ 92. However, he scored poorly in the Benton Visual Retention Test (5 correct immediate recalls from 10 items). Recall from memory of Rey-Osterrieth's complex figure was severely impaired. Visual recall subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) scored 30 points when performed immediately, but zero after a 30 minutes delay. Visual paired associates learning from the WMS-R was also severely impaired. These findings suggest that visual memory is one of the important factors in the pathogenesis of topographical disorientation of this patient.


Assuntos
Agnosia/fisiopatologia , Memória , Orientação , Percepção Visual , Idoso , Agnosia/etiologia , Infarto Cerebral/complicações , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Lobo Occipital/irrigação sanguínea
20.
Kyobu Geka ; 50(6): 515-9, 1997 Jun.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9185454

RESUMO

Carcinoid tumor of the thymus is a rare tumor and discovered not infrequently at advanced stage. A 56-year-old male was admitted to our hospital with severe chest pain. Chest X-ray film and CT scan, revealed a tumor mass in the anterior mediastinum. The patient underwent extended thymectomy including tumor completely through median sternotomy in combination with partial resection of pericardium, mediastinal pleura and left upper lobectomy using internal mammary artery (IMA) retractor. The microscopic findings of the tumor revealed carcinoid Invasion to pericardium and lung was found microscopically. After the operation he has been treated by radiotherapy and any regrowth of the tumor has never been detected for 32 months. This case who had been undergone complete resection using IMA retractor followed by radiotherapy seemed to have better prognosis. Accordingly, extended thymectomy including tumor should be carried out for thymic carcinoid, and the IMA retractor is useful for complete surgical resection through median sternotomy.


Assuntos
Tumor Carcinoide/cirurgia , Cirurgia Torácica/instrumentação , Timectomia/métodos , Neoplasias do Timo/cirurgia , Tumor Carcinoide/patologia , Humanos , Masculino , Artéria Torácica Interna , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Instrumentos Cirúrgicos , Neoplasias do Timo/patologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA