Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 136
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Theor Appl Genet ; 135(8): 2627-2639, 2022 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35748907

RESUMO

KEY MESSAGE: Stem rust resistance genes, SrRL5271 and Sr672.1 as well as SrCPI110651, from Aegilops tauschii, the diploid D genome progenitor of wheat, are sequence variants of Sr46 differing by 1-2 nucleotides leading to non-synonymous amino acid substitutions. The Aegilops tauschii (wheat D-genome progenitor) accessions RL 5271 and CPI110672 were identified as resistant to multiple races (including the Ug99) of the wheat stem rust pathogen Puccinia graminis f. sp. tritici (Pgt). This study was conducted to identify the stem rust resistance (Sr) gene(s) in both accessions. Genetic analysis of the resistance in RL 5271 identified a single dominant allele (SrRL5271) controlling resistance, whereas resistance segregated at two loci (SR672.1 and SR672.2) for a cross of CPI110672. Bulked segregant analysis placed SrRL5271 and Sr672.1 in a region on chromosome arm 2DS that encodes Sr46. Molecular marker screening, mapping and genomic sequence analysis demonstrated SrRL5271 and Sr672.1 are alleles of Sr46. The amino acid sequence of SrRL5271 and Sr672.1 is identical but differs from Sr46 (hereafter referred to as Sr46_h1 by following the gene nomenclature in wheat) by a single amino acid (N763K) and is thus designated Sr46_h2. Screening of a panel of Ae. tauschii accessions identified an additional allelic variant that differed from Sr46_h2 by a different amino acid (A648V) and was designated Sr46_h3. By contrast, the protein encoded by the susceptible allele of Ae. tauschii accession AL8/78 differed from these resistance proteins by 54 amino acid substitutions (94% nucleotide sequence gene identity). Cloning and complementation tests of the three resistance haplotypes confirmed their resistance to Pgt race 98-1,2,3,5,6 and partial resistance to Pgt race TTRTF in bread wheat. The three Sr46 haplotypes, with no virulent races detected yet, represent a valuable source for improving stem resistance in wheat.


Assuntos
Aegilops , Basidiomycota , Aegilops/genética , Aminoácidos , Mapeamento Cromossômico , Cromossomos de Plantas , Diploide , Resistência à Doença/genética , Genes de Plantas , Haplótipos , Doenças das Plantas/genética , Puccinia
2.
Theor Appl Genet ; 135(12): 4327-4336, 2022 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36173416

RESUMO

KEY MESSAGE: Stripe rust resistance gene YrAet672 from Aegilops tauschii accession CPI110672 encodes a nucleotide-binding and leucine-rich repeat domain containing protein similar to YrAS2388 and both these members were haplotypes of Yr28. New sources of host resistance are required to counter the continued emergence of new pathotypes of the wheat stripe rust pathogen Puccinia striiformis Westend. f. sp. tritici Erikss. (Pst). Here, we show that CPI110672, an Aegilops tauschii accession from Turkmenistan, carries a single Pst resistance gene, YrAet672, that is effective against multiple Pst pathotypes, including the four predominant Pst lineages present in Australia. The YRAet672 locus was fine mapped to the short arm of chromosome 4D, and a nucleotide-binding and leucine-rich repeat gene was identified at the locus. A transgene encoding the YrAet672 genomic sequence, but lacking a copy of a duplicated sequence present in the 3' UTR, was transformed into wheat cultivar Fielder and Avocet S. This transgene conferred a weak resistance response, suggesting that the duplicated 3' UTR region was essential for function. Subsequent analyses demonstrated that YrAet672 is the same as two other Pst resistance genes described in Ae. tauschii, namely YrAS2388 and Yr28. They were identified as haplotypes encoding identical protein sequences but are polymorphic in non-translated regions of the gene. Suppression of resistance conferred by YrAet672 and Yr28 in synthetic hexaploid wheat lines (AABBDD) involving Langdon (AABB) as the tetraploid parent was associated with a reduction in transcript accumulation.


Assuntos
Aegilops , Basidiomycota , Aegilops/genética , Resistência à Doença/genética , Doenças das Plantas/genética , Mapeamento Cromossômico , Leucina/genética , Genes de Plantas , Basidiomycota/fisiologia , Poaceae/genética , Nucleotídeos
3.
Laterality ; 27(6): 605-615, 2022 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36448725

RESUMO

The symbolism of laterality in images implies that the virtuous figure is represented on the right of the scene whereas the sinful character is depicted on the left. In portraits of male and female characters this has reflected and reinforced stereotypes and inequalities down the ages. Given these premises, we hypothesized that the prototypical representations of Adam and Eve, as a man and a woman conflated with notions of virtue and vice, would show a non-random arrangement. We tested this hypothesis, sampling artistic depictions of the Garden of Eden, from the twelth century to the present day in three separately-collected series of 100, 99, and 142 images respectively. Eve is depicted to Adam's left significantly more often than chance (between 70% and 83%), particularly in pre-1600 artworks. We interpret this asymmetry as reflecting the perceived lesser status of women in relation to men, since the allegorical incipit of humankind. We also provide experimental evidence that this asymmetry, although pervasive, has not been internalized by modern viewers. Cognitive sciences account for this spatial asymmetry in terms of preference for figures placed within the left visual field of the observer, i.e., within the right space of the objective scene.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Religião , Humanos , Masculino , Feminino , Campos Visuais
4.
Laterality ; 27(4): 443-466, 2022 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35940957

RESUMO

This study assessed pseudoneglect using line bisection and perceptual landmark tasks in two matched online sessions. Line bisection bias was characterized by the traditional measure of Directional Bisection Error (DBE), and by Endpoint Weightings Bias (EWB), derived from an "endpoint weightings" analysis, made possible by the independent manipulation of left and right endpoints. EWB is proposed to index the relative attentional allocation to the two ends of the line. The expected leftward bias (pseudoneglect) was found, with larger effect sizes for EWB (d = -0.34 in both sessions) than for DBE (-0.22 in Session 1 and -0.14 in Session 2). Although EWB was slightly less reliable than DBE, it was more sensitive to pseudoneglect, and the endpoint weightings method has further advantages, including the option of an additional measure of non-lateralized attention. A substantial proportion of participants had difficulty following the instructions for the landmark task, which highlights the need for clear instructions and performance checks for this task. This study shows that line bisection can be used to measure pseudoneglect online, and provides grounds to suggest that the task should routinely include the independent manipulation of left and right endpoints, so that an endpoint weightings analysis can be performed.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Desempenho Psicomotor , Humanos , Percepção Espacial , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Atenção
5.
New Phytol ; 229(5): 2812-2826, 2021 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33176001

RESUMO

Pm1a, the first powdery mildew resistance gene described in wheat, is part of a complex resistance (R) gene cluster located in a distal region of chromosome 7AL that has suppressed genetic recombination. A nucleotide-binding, leucine-rich repeat (NLR) immune receptor gene was isolated using mutagenesis and R gene enrichment sequencing (MutRenSeq). Stable transformation confirmed Pm1a identity which induced a strong resistance phenotype in transgenic plants upon challenge with avirulent Blumeria graminis (wheat powdery mildew) pathogens. A high-density genetic map of a B. graminis family segregating for Pm1a avirulence combined with pathogen genome resequencing and RNA sequencing (RNAseq) identified AvrPm1a effector gene candidates. In planta expression identified an effector, with an N terminal Y/FxC motif, that induced a strong hypersensitive response when co-expressed with Pm1a in Nicotiana benthamiana. Single chromosome enrichment sequencing (ChromSeq) and assembly of chromosome 7A suggested that suppressed recombination around the Pm1a region was due to a rearrangement involving chromosomes 7A, 7B and 7D. The cloning of Pm1a and its identification in a highly rearranged region of chromosome 7A provides insight into the role of chromosomal rearrangements in the evolution of this complex resistance cluster.


Assuntos
Ascomicetos , Triticum , Ascomicetos/genética , Cromossomos , Resistência à Doença/genética , Doenças das Plantas/genética , Triticum/genética
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 239(8): 2635-2648, 2021 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34216231

RESUMO

Visual transients can interrupt overt orienting by abolishing the execution of a planned eye movement due about 90 ms later, a phenomenon known as saccadic inhibition (SI). It is not known if the same inhibitory process might influence covert orienting in the absence of saccades, and consequently alter visual perception. In Experiment 1 (n = 14), we measured orientation discrimination during a covert orienting task in which an uninformative exogenous visual cue preceded the onset of an oriented probe by 140-290 ms. In half of the trials, the onset of the probe was accompanied by a brief irrelevant flash, a visual transient that would normally induce SI. We report a time-dependent inhibition of covert orienting in which the irrelevant flash impaired orientation discrimination accuracy when the probe followed the cue by 190 and 240 ms. The interference was more pronounced when the cue was incongruent with the probe location, suggesting an impact on the reorienting component of the attentional shift. In Experiment 2 (n = 12), we tested whether the inhibitory effect of the flash could occur within an earlier time range, or only within the later, reorienting range. We presented probes at congruent cue locations in a time window between 50 and 200 ms. Similar to Experiment 1, discrimination performance was altered at 200 ms after the cue. We suggest that covert attention may be susceptible to similar inhibitory mechanisms that generate SI, especially in later stages of attentional shifting (> 200 ms after a cue), typically associated with reorienting.


Assuntos
Atenção , Movimentos Sacádicos , Sinais (Psicologia) , Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Tempo de Reação , Percepção Visual
7.
Neurocase ; 26(5): 277-284, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32804579

RESUMO

The size-weight illusion is a perceptual illusion where smaller objects are judged as heavier than equally weighted larger objects. A previous informal report suggests that visual form agnosic patient DF does not experience the size-weight illusion when vision is the only available cue to object size. We tested this experimentally, comparing the magnitudes of DF's visual, kinesthetic and visual-kinesthetic size-weight illusions to those of 28 similarly-aged controls. A modified t-test found that DF's visual size-weight illusion was significantly smaller than that of controls (zcc = -1.7). A test of simple dissociation based on the Revised Standardized Difference Test found that the discrepancy between the magnitude of DF's visual and kinesthetic size-weight illusions was not significantly different from that of controls (zdcc = -1.054), thereby failing to establish a dissociation between the visual and kinesthetic conditions. These results are consistent with previous suggestions that visual form agnosia, following ventral visual stream damage, is associated with an abnormally reduced size-weight illusion. The results, however, do not confirm that this reduction is specific to the use of visual size cues to predict object weight, rather than reflecting more general changes in the processing of object size cues or in the use of predictive strategies for lifting.


Assuntos
Agnosia/fisiopatologia , Ilusões/fisiologia , Percepção de Tamanho/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Percepção de Peso/fisiologia , Idoso , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
8.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 170: 207-214, 2018 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29472110

RESUMO

When learning to write, children often mirror-reverse individual letters. For children learning to use the Latin alphabet, in a left-to-right writing culture, letters that appear to face left (such as J and Z) seem to be more prone to reversal than those that appear to face right (such as B and C). It has been proposed that, because most asymmetrical Latin letters face right, children statistically learn this general regularity and are subsequently biased to write any letter rightward. The evidence for this character-facing bias is circumstantial, however, because letter-facing direction is confounded with other factors that could affect error rates; for instance, J and Z are left-facing, but they are also infrequent. We report the first controlled experimental test of the character-facing bias. We taught 43 Scottish primary schoolchildren (aged 4.8-5.8 years) four artificial, letter-like characters, two of which were left-facing and two of which were right-facing. The characters were novel and so were not subject to prior exposure effects, and alternate groups of children were assigned to identical but mirror-reflected character sets. Children were three times more likely to mirror-write a novel character they had learned in a left-facing format than to mirror-write one they had learned in a right-facing format. This provides the first experimental confirmation of the character-facing bias in literacy development and suggests that implicit knowledge acquired from exposure to written language is readily generalized to novel letter-like forms.


Assuntos
Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Aprendizagem/fisiologia , Alfabetização , Redação , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia
9.
Laterality ; 23(6): 664-683, 2018 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29495914

RESUMO

The writing attempts of children often feature mirror-reversals of individual letters. These reversals are thought to arise from an adaptive tendency to mirror-generalize. However, it is unclear whether mirror-writing is driven by mirror-generalisation of the visual letter forms, or of the actions for writing them. We report two studies of the relationship between mirror-writing and the ability to recognize whether a visually presented letter is in the correct orientation, amongst primary and preschool children learning to read and write in English. Children who produced more mirror-writing also made more orientation recognition errors, for uppercase (Study 1, n = 44) and lowercase letters (Study 2, n = 98), and these relationships remained significant when controlling for age. In both studies, the letters more often reversed in writing were also more prone to orientation recognition errors. Moreover, the rates of mirror-writing of different uppercase letters were closely similar between the dominant and non-dominant hands (Study 1). We also note that, in the recognition tasks, children were more likely to accept reversed letters as correct, than to reject correctly oriented letters, consistent with a tendency to mirror-generalize the visual letter forms. In every aspect, these results support a major role for visual representations in developmental mirror-writing.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Destreza Motora , Orientação Espacial , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Redação , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Aprendizagem por Probabilidade
10.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 29(12): 2068-2080, 2017 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28820676

RESUMO

As we look around the world, selecting our targets, competing events may occur at other locations. Depending on current goals, the viewer must decide whether to look at new events or to ignore them. Two experimental paradigms formalize these response options: double-step saccades and saccadic inhibition. In the first, the viewer must reorient to a newly appearing target; in the second, they must ignore it. Until now, the relationship between reorienting and inhibition has been unexplored. In three experiments, we found saccadic inhibition ∼100 msec after a new target onset, regardless of the task instruction. Moreover, if this automatic inhibition is boosted by an irrelevant flash, reorienting is facilitated, suggesting that saccadic inhibition plays a crucial role in visual behavior, as a bottom-up brake that buys the time needed for decisional processes to act. Saccadic inhibition may be a ubiquitous pause signal that provides the flexibility for voluntary behavior to emerge.


Assuntos
Inibição Psicológica , Atividade Motora , Orientação , Movimentos Sacádicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Medições dos Movimentos Oculares , Humanos , Adulto Jovem
11.
Bioelectromagnetics ; 38(5): 356-363, 2017 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28342187

RESUMO

This study considers the computationally determined thermal profile of a finely discretized, heterogeneous human body model, simulating a radiofrequency electromagnetic field (RF-EMF) worker wearing protective clothing subject to RF-EMF exposure, and subject to various environmental conditions including high ambient temperature and high humidity, with full thermoregulatory mechanisms in place. How the human body responds in various scenarios was investigated, and the information was used to consider safety limits in current international RF-EMF safety guidelines and standards. It was found that different environmental conditions had minimal impact on the magnitude of the thermal response due to RF-EMF exposure, and that the current safety factor of 10 applied in international RF-EMF safety guidelines and standards for RF-EMF workers is generally conservative, though it is only narrowly so when workers are subjected to the most adverse environmental conditions. Bioelectromagnetics. 38:356-363, 2017. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Assuntos
Campos Eletromagnéticos/efeitos adversos , Roupa de Proteção , Exposição à Radiação/prevenção & controle , Ondas de Rádio/efeitos adversos , Temperatura , Humanos
12.
J Neurophysiol ; 115(2): 752-62, 2016 Feb 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26631151

RESUMO

Visual transients, such as a bright flash, reduce the proportion of saccades executed, ∼60-125 ms after flash onset, a phenomenon known as saccadic inhibition (SI). Across three experiments, we apply a similar time-course analysis to the amplitudes and velocities of saccades. Alongside the expected reduction of saccade frequency in the key time period, we report two perturbations of the "main sequence": one before and one after the period of SI. First, saccades launched between 30 and 70 ms, following the flash, were hypometric, with peak speed exceeding that expected for a saccade of similar amplitude. This finding was in contrast to the common idea that saccades have passed a "point of no return," ∼60 ms before launching, escaping interference from distractors. The early hypometric saccades observed were not a consequence of spatial averaging between target and distractor locations, as they were found not only following a localized central flash (experiment 1) but also following a spatially generalized flash (experiment 2). Second, across experiments, saccades launched at 110 ms postflash, toward the end of SI, had normal amplitude but a peak speed higher than expected for that amplitude, suggesting increased collicular excitation at the time of launching. Overall, the results show that saccades that escape inhibition following a visual transient are not necessarily unaffected but instead, can reveal interference in spatial and kinematic measures.


Assuntos
Desempenho Psicomotor , Movimentos Sacádicos/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Humanos , Inibição Neural , Tempo de Reação
13.
Theor Appl Genet ; 129(1): 65-76, 2016 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26433828

RESUMO

KEY MESSAGE: Complementary genes for resistance to wheat stripe rust in an Avocet selection mapped to chromosome arms 3DL and 5BL. Susceptible Avocet selections lacked the 5BL gene due to a chromosomal deletion. This study reports the inheritance and genetic mapping of the YrA (temporary name of convenience to describe the specificity) seedling resistance to wheat stripe rust (caused by Puccinia striiformis f. sp. tritici; Pst) in a resistant selection of the Australian cv. Avocet [Avocet R (AvR)-AUS 90660]. Genetic analysis was performed on F2 populations and F3 generation families from crosses between wheats that carried and lacked the YrA resistance. Greenhouse seedling tests with two avirulent Pst pathotypes (104 E137 A- and 108 E141 A-) confirmed that the YrA resistance was inherited as two complementary dominant genes. Ninety-two doubled haploid (DH) lines from a cross between the Australian cv. Teal (Pst susceptible) and AvR were used for DArT-Seq genotypic analysis to map the seedling resistance. Marker-trait association analysis using 9035 DArT-Seq loci mapped the genes to the long arms of chromosomes 3D (3DL) and 5B (5BL), respectively. F2 populations from crosses between susceptible DH lines that carried either the 3DL or 5BL marker genotypes confirmed the complementary gene model. Fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH) analysis determined that Teal carries a reciprocal T5B-7B translocation. FISH analysis also identified a 5BL chromosomal deletion in Avocet S relative to AvR that further validated the complementary gene model and possibly explained the heterogeneity of closely related wheats carrying the YrA resistance. The individual loci of the complementary YrA resistance were designated Yr73 (3DL) and Yr74 (5BL). Candidate single gene reference stocks will be permanently accessioned following cytological analysis to avoid the T5B-7B translocation.


Assuntos
Basidiomycota , Resistência à Doença/genética , Padrões de Herança , Doenças das Plantas/genética , Triticum/genética , Mapeamento Cromossômico , Cruzamentos Genéticos , Genes Dominantes , Genes de Plantas , Ligação Genética , Genótipo , Haploidia , Hibridização in Situ Fluorescente , Fenótipo , Doenças das Plantas/microbiologia , Triticum/microbiologia
14.
Theor Appl Genet ; 129(3): 485-93, 2016 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26660463

RESUMO

KEY MESSAGE: Genetic and mutational analyses of wheat leaf rust resistance gene Lr13 and hybrid necrosis gene Ne2 m indicated that they are the same gene. Hybrid necrosis in wheat characterized by chlorosis and eventual necrosis of plant tissues in certain wheat hybrids is controlled by the interaction of complementary dominant genes Ne1 and Ne2 located on chromosome arms 5BL and 2BS, respectively. Multiple alleles at each locus can be identified by differences in necrotic phenotypes when varieties are crossed with a fixed accession of the other genotype. Some of at least five Ne2 alleles were described as s (strong), m (medium) and w (weak); alleles of Ne1 were similarly described. Ne2m causes moderate necrosis in hybrids with genotypes having Ne1s. Ne2 is located on chromosome arm 2BS in close proximity to Lr13. Most wheat lines with Ne2m carry Lr13, and all wheat lines with Lr13 appear to carry Ne2m. To further dissect the relationship between Lr13 and Ne2m, more than 350 crosses were made between cv. Spica (Triticum aestivum) or Kubanka (T. durum) carrying Ne1s and recombinant inbred lines or doubled haploid lines from three crosses segregating for Lr13. F1 plants from lines carrying Lr13 crossed with Spica (Ne1s) always showed progressive necrosis; those lacking Lr13 did not. Four wheat cultivars/lines carrying Lr13 were treated with the mutagen EMS. Thirty-five susceptible mutants were identified; eight were distinctly less glaucous and late maturing indicative of chromosome 2B or sub-chromosome loss. Hybrids of phenotypically normal Lr13 mutant plants crossed with Spica did not produce symptoms of hybrid necrosis. Thus, Lr13 and one particular Ne2m allele may be the same gene.


Assuntos
Basidiomycota/patogenicidade , Resistência à Doença/genética , Doenças das Plantas/genética , Triticum/genética , Alelos , Mapeamento Cromossômico , Cruzamentos Genéticos , Genes Dominantes , Genes de Plantas , Marcadores Genéticos , Genótipo , Doenças das Plantas/microbiologia , Folhas de Planta/microbiologia , Triticum/microbiologia
15.
Bioelectromagnetics ; 37(8): 563-567, 2016 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27716967

RESUMO

As the availability of Terahertz (THz) sources and their application expands, interaction with biological material needs to be well-understood. In order to accurately estimate the energy absorption pattern in skin and other tissues in the THz and far Infrared regions, values of dielectric parameters ϵ and σ are required. We present an approach for tissue water, which uses literature values of complex refractive index for pure water together with some considerations regarding the effects of dissolved salts. The approach also involves modeling of Debye and Lorentz absorption parameters, which provides a method for the estimation of ϵ and σ in other tissues. Bioelectromagnetics. 37:563-567, 2016. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

16.
Plant J ; 79(6): 904-13, 2014 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24942074

RESUMO

The powdery mildew resistance gene Pm8 derived from rye is located on a 1BL.1RS chromosome translocation in wheat. However, some wheat lines with this translocation do not show resistance to isolates of the wheat powdery mildew pathogen avirulent to Pm8 due to an unknown genetically dominant suppression mechanism. Here we show that lines with suppressed Pm8 activity contain an intact and expressed Pm8 gene. Therefore, the absence of Pm8 function in certain 1BL.1RS-containing wheat lines is not the result of gene loss or mutation but is based on suppression. The wheat gene Pm3, an ortholog of rye Pm8, suppressed Pm8-mediated powdery mildew resistance in lines containing Pm8 in a transient single-cell expression assay. This result was further confirmed in transgenic lines with combined Pm8 and Pm3 transgenes. Expression analysis revealed that suppression is not the result of gene silencing, either in wheat 1BL.1RS translocation lines carrying Pm8 or in transgenic genotypes with both Pm8 and Pm3 alleles. In addition, a similar abundance of the PM8 and PM3 proteins in single or double homozygous transgenic lines suggested that a post-translational mechanism is involved in suppression of Pm8. Co-expression of Pm8 and Pm3 genes in Nicotiana benthamiana leaves followed by co-immunoprecipitation analysis showed that the two proteins interact. Therefore, the formation of a heteromeric protein complex might result in inefficient or absent signal transmission for the defense reaction. These data provide a molecular explanation for the suppression of resistance genes in certain genetic backgrounds and suggest ways to circumvent it in future plant breeding.


Assuntos
Ascomicetos/fisiologia , Resistência à Doença , Doenças das Plantas/imunologia , Proteínas de Plantas/genética , Secale/genética , Triticum/genética , Alelos , Ascomicetos/patogenicidade , Dimerização , Expressão Gênica , Genes Reporter , Genótipo , Imunoprecipitação , Endogamia , Dados de Sequência Molecular , Doenças das Plantas/microbiologia , Folhas de Planta/genética , Proteínas de Plantas/metabolismo , Plantas Geneticamente Modificadas , Mapeamento de Interação de Proteínas , Processamento de Proteína Pós-Traducional , Nicotiana/genética , Nicotiana/imunologia , Nicotiana/microbiologia , Transgenes , Translocação Genética , Triticum/microbiologia
17.
Neurocase ; 21(5): 665-73, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25325717

RESUMO

Mirror writing (MW) has mainly been observed in left-hemisphere-damaged patients writing with the left hand. This study evaluated the presence of MW in 24 patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). We found that MW is not a typical feature of MCI. However, one woman (FC), mislabeled initially with MCI but in fact affected by anxiety, showed florid MW when writing with her left hand, which resolved as her anxiety receded. This case study supports anecdotal reports of MW triggered by anxiety, and the features of FC's performance indicate a motor rather than a perceptual basis for the phenomenon.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/complicações , Ansiedade/psicologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/epidemiologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Escrita Manual , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Disfunção Cognitiva/complicações , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Desempenho Psicomotor
18.
Acta Derm Venereol ; 95(6): 681-5, 2015 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25633058

RESUMO

Using an experimental task in which lay persons were asked to distinguish between 30 images of melanomas and common mimics of melanoma, we compared various training strategies including the ABC(D) method, use of images of both melanomas and mimics of melanoma, and alternative methods of choosing training image exemplars. Based on a sample size of 976 persons, and an online experimental task, we show that all the positive training approaches increased diagnostic sensitivity when compared with no training, but only the simultaneous use of melanoma and benign exemplars, as chosen by experts, increased specificity and diagnostic accuracy. The ABCD method and use of melanoma exemplar images chosen by laypersons decreased specificity in comparison with the control. The method of choosing exemplar images is important. The levels of change in performance are however very modest, with an increase in accuracy between control and best-performing strategy of only 9%.


Assuntos
Educação em Saúde/métodos , Melanoma/patologia , Nevo/patologia , Neoplasias Cutâneas/patologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Autoavaliação Diagnóstica , Detecção Precoce de Câncer , Reações Falso-Negativas , Reações Falso-Positivas , Feminino , Humanos , Internet , Ceratose Seborreica/patologia , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Fotografação , Sensibilidade e Especificidade , Adulto Jovem
19.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 31(4): 350-66, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24593311

RESUMO

Mirror writing is a striking behaviour that is common in children and can reemerge in adults following brain damage. Skilled deliberate mirror writing has also been reported, but only anecdotally. We provide the first quantitative study of skilled deliberate mirror writing. K.B. can write forward or backward, vertically upright or inverted, with the hands acting alone or simultaneously. K.B. is predominantly left handed, but writes habitually with his right hand. Of his writing formats, his left hand mirror writing is by far the most similar in style to his normal handwriting. When writing bimanually, he performs better when his two hands make mirror-symmetrical movements to write opposite scripts than if they move in the same direction to write similar scripts. He has no special facility for reading mirrored text. These features are consistent with prior anecdotal cases and support a motor basis for K.B.'s ability, according to which his skilled mirror writing results from the left hand execution of a low-level motor program for a right hand abductive writing action. Our methods offer a novel framework for investigating the sharing of motor representations across effectors.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Escrita Manual , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Leitura
20.
Neurocase ; 20(3): 338-45, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23548033

RESUMO

Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) is usually assessed by means of individual stimuli or single arrays of stimuli. Seldom are stimuli presented as multiple objects or in spatially separated blocks, except in some tests for object-based neglect. The distance between individual objects or blocks of stimuli in such stimuli is implicitly considered irrelevant. We report on the case of a patient, EC, who showed severe USN in his everyday behavior, yet performed normally on standard tests for USN. Presented with stimuli in separate blocks, he performed flawlessly with 4 cm gaps between blocks, yet ignored all leftward blocks of stimuli when the gap was larger than this. EC's dissociation between good performance on standard tasks and severe neglect with separate groups of stimuli, and the distance-mediated nature of his USN are novel observations with relevant theoretical and clinical implications.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Idoso , Percepção de Distância , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA