Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 30
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Dyslexia ; 27(2): 224-244, 2021 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32959479

RESUMO

This study examined changes in white matter microstructure and grey matter volume, cortical thickness, and cortical surface area before and after reading intervention. Participants included 22 average readers and 13 dyslexic readers (8-9 years old in third grade); the dyslexic readers were enrolled in reading intervention programs at their elementary school. Participants completed scans of diffusion tensor imaging and T1-weighted MRI before and after 3 months of instruction. An a priori region of interest (ROI) analysis was used. Dyslexic readers, compared to average readers, showed higher mean diffusivity in white matter ROIs including bilateral inferior frontal, bilateral insula, left superior temporal, and right supramarginal gyri across time points. Dyslexic readers also had thicker cortex in left fusiform and bilateral supramarginal gyri; whereas, average readers had greater surface area in right fusiform across time. There were no significant changes in white or grey matter following intervention; however, mean diffusivity in the right hemisphere was associated with reading gains over time. White matter organization in the right hemisphere predicts reading changes, and dyslexic readers may have persistent differences in white and grey matter due to ongoing reading deficits.


Assuntos
Imagem de Tensor de Difusão/métodos , Dislexia/diagnóstico por imagem , Dislexia/terapia , Substância Cinzenta/diagnóstico por imagem , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Leitura , Substância Branca/diagnóstico por imagem , Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagem , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Idioma , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino
2.
Dyslexia ; 25(3): 227-245, 2019 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31020760

RESUMO

Early intervention is known to reduce reading disabilities; however, treatment response is variable, and some students have persistent deficits that require intensive supports. This study examined the immediate and 1-year outcomes of an individualized and intensive reading program for third grade students, which was delivered throughout the school day for an average of 189 hr of instruction over 3 months. These students' performances were compared with two comparison groups, including poor readers who received small group supports and good readers who did not have additional reading instruction. The intensive group showed an improvement in word recognition and decoding fluency immediately after the program and 1 year later, and there was a decrease in significant reading impairments from 62% before intervention to 35% at follow-up. Furthermore, baseline reading, spelling, phonological awareness, and rapid naming skills were predictive of persistent reading deficits at a later time point. Although improvements in reading skills were shown, a significant gap between poor and good readers persisted in the third and fourth grades. This study illustrates the importance of a tertiary intensive reading program, but also the need for continuing supports.


Assuntos
Dislexia/terapia , Deficiências da Aprendizagem/terapia , Leitura , Ensino de Recuperação/métodos , Estudantes/psicologia , Conscientização , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Linguística , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino
3.
J Child Lang ; 42(2): 323-50, 2015 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24703202

RESUMO

To fully acquire a language, especially its phonology, children need linguistic input from native speakers early on. When interaction with native speakers is not always possible - e.g. for children learning a second language that is not the societal language - audios are commonly used as an affordable substitute. But does such non-interactive input work? Two experiments evaluated the usefulness of audio storybooks in acquiring a more native-like second-language accent. Young children, first- and second-graders in Hong Kong whose native language was Cantonese Chinese, were given take-home listening assignments in a second language, either English or Putonghua Chinese. Accent ratings of the children's story reading revealed measurable benefits of non-interactive input from native speakers. The benefits were far more robust for Putonghua than English. Implications for second-language accent acquisition are discussed.


Assuntos
Aprendizagem , Multilinguismo , Estimulação Acústica , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Hong Kong , Humanos , Desenvolvimento da Linguagem , Masculino , Percepção da Fala
4.
Psicothema ; 22(4): 963-9, 2010 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21044539

RESUMO

This study examined the development of literacy skills in children in a district that used a Response to Intervention (RTI) model. The district included children whose first language was English and children who were learning English as a second language (ESL). Tasks measuring phonological awareness, lexical access, and syntactic awareness were administered when the children entered school in kindergarten at age 5. Reading, phonological processing, syntactic awareness, memory, and spelling were administered in grade 7. When the children entered school, significant numbers of them were at risk for literacy difficulties. After systematic instruction and annual monitoring of skills, their reading abilities improved to the extent that only a very small percentage had reading difficulties. The results demonstrated that early identification and intervention and frequent monitoring of basic skills can significantly reduce the incidence of reading problems in both the ESL and language majority children.


Assuntos
Dislexia/diagnóstico , Intervenção Educacional Precoce/métodos , Idioma , Modelos Teóricos , Colúmbia Britânica , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Dislexia/prevenção & controle , Dislexia/psicologia , Dislexia/reabilitação , Emigrantes e Imigrantes , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo , Fonética , Avaliação de Programas e Projetos de Saúde , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Aprendizagem Verbal
5.
Ann Dyslexia ; 70(3): 369-378, 2020 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32880790

RESUMO

We suggest that the American poet E.E. Cummings was probably mildly dyslexic. Evidence, which is drawn in particular from inspection of his archival papers, includes consideration of his spelling, letter formation, handwriting, approach to page orientation, proclivity for exploration of the mirror-image, reading and educational history, struggles in the composition of analytical prose, and notable strengths in lateral thinking and the making of surprising lateral connections. We emphasise the importance of Cummings' modernist literary context as the primary shaping force for his literary aesthetic and we resist any simply reductive explanation of his literary style as a function of dyslexia. However, dyslexia may be one factor that contributes to his unique style.


Assuntos
Dislexia/história , Pessoas Famosas , Escrita Manual , Leitura , Dislexia/diagnóstico , História do Século XIX , História do Século XX , Humanos , Idioma , Masculino , Orientação/fisiologia
6.
Front Public Health ; 8: 469, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33194932

RESUMO

We bridge two analogous concepts of comorbidity, dyslexia-dyscalculia and reading-mathematical disabilities, in neuroscience and education, respectively. We assessed the cognitive profiles of 360 individuals (mean age 25.79 ± 13.65) with disability in reading alone (RD group), mathematics alone (MD group) and both (comorbidity: MDRD group), with tests widely used in both psychoeducational and neuropsychological batteries. As expected, the MDRD group exhibited reading deficits like those shown by the RD group. The former group also exhibited deficits in quantitative reasoning like those shown by the MD group. However, other deficits related to verbal working memory and semantic memory were exclusive to the MDRD group. These findings were independent of gender, age, or socioeconomic and demographic factors. Through a systematic exhaustive review of clinical neuroimaging literature, we mapped the resulting cognitive profiles to correspondingly plausible neuroanatomical substrates of dyslexia and dyscalculia. In our resulting "probing" model, the complex set of domain-specific and domain-general impairments shown in the comorbidity of reading and mathematical disabilities are hypothesized as being related to atypical development of the left angular gyrus. The present neuroeducational approach bridges a long-standing transdisciplinary divide and contributes a step further toward improved early prediction, teaching and interventions for children and adults with combined reading and math disabilities.


Assuntos
Discalculia , Dislexia , Adolescente , Adulto , Criança , Discalculia/epidemiologia , Dislexia/epidemiologia , Humanos , Matemática , Leitura , Instituições Acadêmicas , Adulto Jovem
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 130: 13-25, 2019 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30030194

RESUMO

Children with poor reading skills have differences in brain function when compared to typically-developing readers, and there may also be changes in the brain following reading intervention. However, most functional imaging studies focus on phonological reading tasks with one level of task difficulty. The purpose of this study was to compare good and poor readers on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) tasks of orthography (spelling) and phonology (rhyming) before and after 3 months of school-based intervention. These tasks were also modulated by task difficulty based on printed word frequency. The results showed that primarily left hemisphere regions were activated for the spelling and rhyming tasks, and poor readers showed a pattern of increased activation in bilateral inferior frontal, bilateral insula, right parietal, and left cerebellum following intervention. Activity in left pars triangularis and right parietal regions were associated with gains in decoding skills. Intervention effects appeared across blocks of easy and difficult words, except for the right parietal cortex. In this region, poor readers had greater activity on the easy word blocks after intervention, which indicates that there was increased recruitment of the right parietal cortex for relatively easy words. These results indicate that effects of intervention may be more evident on phonological tasks in comparison to orthographic tasks, and some of these effects may be modulated by relative task difficulty.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagem , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Leitura , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , Córtex Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagem , Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Criança , Dislexia/diagnóstico por imagem , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Intervenção Educacional Precoce , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Lobo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagem , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia , Fonética , Fatores Socioeconômicos , Resultado do Tratamento
8.
Dyslexia ; 14(3): 170-87, 2008 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18697190

RESUMO

Groups of Grade 3 children were tested on measures of word-level literacy and undertook tasks that required the ability to associate sounds with letter sequences and that involved visual, auditory and phonological-processing skills. These groups came from different language backgrounds in which the language of instruction was Arabic, Chinese, English, Hungarian or Portuguese. Similar measures were used across the groups, with tests being adapted to be appropriate for the language of the children. Findings indicated that measures of decoding and phonological-processing skills were good predictors of word reading and spelling among Arabic- and English-speaking children, but were less able to predict variability in these same early literacy skills among Chinese- and Hungarian-speaking children, and were better at predicting variability in Portuguese word reading than spelling. Results were discussed with reference to the relative transparency of the script and issues of dyslexia assessment across languages. Overall, the findings argue for the need to take account of features of the orthography used to represent a language when developing assessment procedures for a particular language and that assessment of word-level literacy skills and a phonological perspective of dyslexia may not be universally applicable across all language contexts.


Assuntos
Comparação Transcultural , Dislexia/etnologia , Idioma , Fonética , Leitura , Semântica , Aprendizagem Verbal , Logro , Criança , Dislexia/diagnóstico , Dislexia/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos , Fatores de Risco , Aprendizagem Seriada , Redação
9.
J Learn Disabil ; 39(1): 25-47, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16512081

RESUMO

The double-deficit hypothesis of developmental dyslexia proposes that deficits in phonological processing and naming speed represent independent sources of dysfunction in dyslexia. The present article is a review of the evidence for the double-deficit hypothesis, including a discussion of recent findings related to the hypothesis. Studies in this area have been characterized by variability in methodology--how dyslexia is defined and identified, and how dyslexia subtypes are classified. Such variability sets limitations on the extent to which conclusions may be drawn with respect to the double-deficit hypothesis. Furthermore, the literature is complicated by the persistent finding that measures of phonological processing and naming speed are significantly correlated, resulting in a statistical artifact that makes it difficult to disentangle the influence of naming speed from that of phonological processing. Longitudinal and intervention studies of the double-deficit hypothesis are needed to accumulate evidence that investigates a naming speed deficit that is independent of a phonological deficit for readers with dyslexia. The existing evidence does not support a persistent core deficit in naming speed for readers with dyslexia.


Assuntos
Dislexia/etiologia , Teoria Psicológica , Criança , Humanos
10.
J Learn Disabil ; 39(4): 364-78, 2006.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16895160

RESUMO

This study presents a longitudinal examination of the development of reading and reading-related skills of 22 Grade 4 children identified as having reading disabilities (RD) who had been followed since kindergarten. The analyses were conducted to investigate the patterns of emergence of RD as well as reading ability and risk status across the 5 years. The findings of the study are presented with an examination of the trajectories of the children with RD as compared to Grade 4 typical readers (matched for grade, gender, language status, and school) with a similar profile on literacy skills in kindergarten. The results demonstrate the heterogeneous nature of the trajectory of RD in school-age children; although many of the children with RD were at risk in kindergarten, there was a subsample who did not demonstrate reading and phonological difficulties until the third and fourth grades.


Assuntos
Dislexia/diagnóstico , Leitura , Estudantes , Criança , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Masculino , Estudos Retrospectivos , Fatores de Risco , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
11.
Paediatr Child Health ; 11(9): 581-7, 2006 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19030329

RESUMO

Dyslexia, or a reading disability, occurs when an individual has significant difficulty with speed and accuracy of word decoding. Comprehension of text and spelling are also affected. The diagnosis of dyslexia involves the use of reading tests, but the continuum of reading performance means that any cutoff point is arbitrary. The IQ score does not play a role in the diagnosis of dyslexia. The cognitive difficulties of dyslexics include problems with speech perception, recognizing and manipulating the basic sounds in a language, language memory, and learning the sounds of letters. Dyslexia is a neurological condition with a genetic basis. There are abnormalities in the brains of dyslexic individuals. There are also differences in the electrophysiological and structural characteristics of the brains of dyslexics. Physicians play a particularly important role in recognizing children who are at risk for dyslexia and helping their parents obtain the proper assessment.

12.
J Learn Disabil ; 49(1): 97-110, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24733819

RESUMO

This article reports the results of two studies examining the effectiveness of the whole-word and analytic instructional methods in teaching different subtypes of readers (students with normal reading performance, surface dyslexics, phonological dyslexics, and both dyslexic patterns) and four kinds of Chinese two-character words (two regular [RR], two irregular [II], one regular, one irregular [RI], and one irregular, one regular [IR]). The approaches employed were the analytic method, which focuses on highlighting the phonological components of words, and the whole-word method, which focuses on learning by sight. Two studies were conducted among a sample of 40 primary school students with different reading patterns. The aim was to examine the relationships among different subtypes of readers, two-character words, and instructional methods. In general, students with a surface dyslexic pattern benefited more from the analytic methods. Regarding combinations of different kinds of two-character words, all subtypes of students performed better in reading RR words than in reading II words.


Assuntos
Dislexia/reabilitação , Terapia da Linguagem/métodos , Linguística , Leitura , Ensino , Criança , Feminino , Hong Kong , Humanos , Masculino
13.
J Learn Disabil ; 49(1): 21-35, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24596111

RESUMO

There are some children who encounter unexpected reading difficulties in the fourth grade. This phenomenon has been described as late emerging reading disabilities (LERD). Using Grade 4 as a starting point, this study examined the reading development of 964 children between kindergarten and Grade 7. The results showed that 72.0% of children had typical reading performance in Grade 4, whereas there was 0.7% with poor word reading, 12.6% with poor reading comprehension, 2.5% with poor word reading and comprehension, and 12.2% with borderline performance. We also showed that there were similar proportions of children who had early versus late emerging reading difficulties; however, most of the late emerging poor readers recovered by Grade 7. Furthermore, our study showed that poor comprehenders showed poorer performance than typical readers on word reading, pseudoword decoding, and spelling between Grade 1 and Grade 7 and poorer performance on a working memory task in kindergarten. Overall, this study showed that most children recover from late emerging reading problems and that working memory may be an early indicator for reading comprehension difficulties.


Assuntos
Dislexia/epidemiologia , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Idade de Início , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino
14.
J Learn Disabil ; 38(5): 473-7, 2005.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16329447

RESUMO

This commentary reviews some of the issues involved in the definition of reading disability and demonstrates how definitions can influence the conclusions reached by a review. In particular, the discrepancy definition of reading disability is shown to be logically flawed. Data from a large unbiased sample show that there are no significant differences between boys and girls in the incidence of reading difficulties.


Assuntos
Dislexia/epidemiologia , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Distribuição por Sexo
15.
Dev Psychol ; 39(6): 1005-19, 2003 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14584981

RESUMO

Patterns of reading development were examined in native English-speaking (L1) children and children who spoke English as a second language (ESL). Participants were 978 (790 L1 speakers and 188 ESL speakers) Grade 2 children involved in a longitudinal study that began in kindergarten. In kindergarten and Grade 2, participants completed standardized and experimental measures including reading, spelling, phonological processing, and memory. All children received phonological awareness instruction in kindergarten and phonics instruction in Grade 1. By the end of Grade 2, the ESL speakers' reading skills were comparable to those of L1 speakers, and ESL speakers even outperformed L1 speakers on several measures. The findings demonstrate that a model of early identification and intervention for children at risk is beneficial for ESL speakers and also suggest that the effects of bilingualism on the acquisition of early reading skills are not negative and may be positive.


Assuntos
Idioma , Leitura , Fala , Conscientização , Criança , Seguimentos , Humanos , Memória/fisiologia , Fonética
16.
J Learn Disabil ; 35(2): 137-57, 2002.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15490742

RESUMO

This study evaluated the effect of sound-symbol association training on visual and phonological memory in children with a history of dyslexia. Pretests of phonological and visual memory, a sound-symbol training procedure, and phonological and visual memory posttests were administered to children with dyslexia, to children whose dyslexia had been compensated through remedial training, and to age- and reading level-matched comparison groups. Deficits in visual and phonological memory and memory for sound-symbol associations were demonstrated in the dyslexia group. For children with dyslexia and children whose dyslexia had been remediated, the sound-symbol training scores were significantly associated with word and pseudoword reading scores and were significantly lower than those of the comparison groups. Children with dyslexia and children whose dyslexia had been compensated showed significantly less facilitation of phonological memory following the training than did typical readers. Skilled readers showed some reduction in accuracy of visual memory following the training, which may be the result of interference of verbalization with a predominantly visual task. A parallel decrease was not observed in the children with dyslexia, possibly because these children did not use the verbal cues. Children with dyslexia and children whose dyslexia had been compensated seemed to have difficulty encoding the novel sounds in memory. As a result, they derived less phonological memory advantage and less visual memory interference from the training than did typical readers. Children in the compensated dyslexia group scored lower on sound-symbol training than their age peers. In other respects, the scores of these children were equivalent to those of the typically reading comparison groups. Children in the compensated dyslexia group exhibited higher phonological rehearsal, iconic memory, and associative memory scores than children in the dyslexia group. Implications for the remediation of dyslexia are discussed.


Assuntos
Percepção Auditiva , Dislexia/fisiopatologia , Memória , Simbolismo , Percepção Visual , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Criança , Dislexia/terapia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória
17.
J Learn Disabil ; 36(1): 48-58, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15490891

RESUMO

Patterns of performance on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) have been proposed as useful tools for the identification of children with learning disabilities (LD). However, most of the studies of WISC-R patterns in children with LD have been plagued by the lack of a typically achieving comparison group, by failure to measure individual patterns, and by the lack of a precise definition of LD. In an attempt to address these flaws and to assess the presence of patterns of performance on the WISC-R, we examined data from 121 children with typical achievement (TA), 143 children with reading disabilities (RD), and 100 children with a specific arithmetic disability (AD), ages 6 to 16 years. The results indicated that the RD and AD groups had significantly lower scores than the TA group on all the Verbal IQ subtests. Many of the children with AD and RD showed a significant difference between Verbal and Performance IQ scores, but so did many of the typically achieving children. Although there were some children with LD who showed the predicted patterns, typically, 65% or more of the children with LD did not. Furthermore, a proportion of the TA group-generally not significantly smaller than that of the RD and AD groups-showed discrepancy patterns as well. Our results indicate that the patterns of performance on intelligence tests are not reliable enough for the diagnosis of LD in individual children. Therefore, it might be more profitable to base the detection of an individual's LD on patterns of achievement test scores.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Deficiências da Aprendizagem/epidemiologia , Escalas de Wechsler , Adolescente , Criança , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
18.
J Learn Disabil ; 36(1): 15-23, 2003.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15490888

RESUMO

This study was designed to examine the role of intelligence (IQ) in the definition of reading disabilities (RD) in languages with different orthographic systems. A sample of 94 Spanish children and 157 English-speaking Canadian children with RD was classified into four groups on the basis of IQ scores from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (< 80; 81-90; 91-109; 110-140). We examined the reading and spelling skills of Canadian and Spanish children as a function of Full Scale, Verbal, and Performance IQ scores. Significant differences between the languages were found when reading performance was analyzed as a function of Verbal IQ scores, in that there were some differences between the groups of Canadian children with RD but not between the groups of Spanish children. The Canadian children with Verbal IQ scores < 80 demonstrated relatively lower performance in reading and spelling skills than the Canadian groups with higher IQ scores. There were differences in reading tasks as a function of Performance IQ in English but not in Spanish. The differences in the role of IQ as a function of orthographic systems may relate to the greater significance of visual-orthographic as opposed to phonological processing in English.


Assuntos
Dislexia/etnologia , Inteligência , Idioma , Adolescente , Canadá/epidemiologia , Criança , Comparação Transcultural , Dislexia/diagnóstico , Dislexia/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Deficiências da Aprendizagem/etnologia , Masculino , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Escalas de Wechsler
19.
J Learn Disabil ; 47(4): 349-65, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23124381

RESUMO

This study investigated the relationship among working memory, processing speed, math performance, and reactivity to stress in 83 Grade 1 children. Specifically, 39 children with math disability (MD) were compared to 44 children who are typically achieving (TA) in mathematics. It is the first study to use a physiological index of stress (salivary cortisol levels) to measure children's reactivity while completing tasks that assess the core components of MD: working memory for numbers, working memory for words, digits backward, letter number sequence, digit span forward, processing speed for numbers and words, block rotation, and math tasks. Grade 1 children with MD obtained significantly lower scores on the letter number sequence and quantitative concepts tasks. Higher levels of reactivity significantly predicted poorer performance on the working memory for numbers, working memory for words, and quantitative concepts tasks for Grade 1 children, regardless of math ability. Grade 1 children with MD and higher reactivity had significantly lower scores on the letter number sequence task than the children with MD and low reactivity. The findings suggest that high reactivity impairs performance in working memory and math tasks in Grade 1 children, and young children with high reactivity may benefit from interventions aimed at lowering anxiety in stressful situations, which may improve learning.


Assuntos
Ansiedade/diagnóstico , Ansiedade/psicologia , Nível de Alerta , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Discalculia/diagnóstico , Discalculia/psicologia , Estresse Psicológico/complicações , Estresse Psicológico/psicologia , Ansiedade/terapia , Canadá , Criança , Educação Inclusiva , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo , Grupos Minoritários/educação , Grupos Minoritários/psicologia , Multilinguismo , Tempo de Reação
20.
Read Writ ; 26(5): 681-704, 2013 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23626405

RESUMO

This study investigated the effects of a 12-week language-enriched phonological awareness instruction on 76 Hong Kong young children who were learning English as a second language. The children were assigned randomly to receive the instruction on phonological awareness skills embedded in vocabulary learning activities or comparison instruction which consisted of vocabulary learning and writing tasks but no direct instruction in phonological awareness skills. They were tested on receptive and expressive vocabulary, phonological awareness at the syllable, rhyme and phoneme levels, reading, and spelling in English before and after the program implementation. The results indicated that children who received the phonological awareness instruction performed significantly better than the comparison group on English word reading, spelling, phonological awareness at all levels and expressive vocabulary on the posttest when age, general intelligence and the pretest scores were controlled statistically. The findings suggest that phonological awareness instruction embedded in vocabulary learning activities might be beneficial to kindergarteners learning English as a second language.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA