Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 3 de 3
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Asunto de la revista
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Psychol Sci ; 31(5): 548-567, 2020 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32310712

RESUMEN

Whether acquiring a second language affords any general advantages to executive function has been a matter of fierce scientific debate for decades. If being bilingual does have benefits over and above the broader social, employment, and lifestyle gains that are available to speakers of a second language, then it should manifest as a cognitive advantage in the general population of bilinguals. We assessed 11,041 participants on a broad battery of 12 executive tasks whose functional and neural properties have been well described. Bilinguals showed an advantage over monolinguals on only one test (whereas monolinguals performed better on four tests), and these effects all disappeared when the groups were matched to remove potentially confounding factors. In any case, the size of the positive bilingual effect in the unmatched groups was so small that it would likely have a negligible impact on the cognitive performance of any individual.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva , Multilingüismo , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Lineales , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Clase Social , Adulto Joven
2.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 150(4): 729-738, 2021 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32969685

RESUMEN

The foundational tenet of brain training is that general cognitive functioning can be enhanced by completing computerized games, a notion that is both intuitive and appealing. Moreover, there is strong incentive to improve our cognitive abilities, so much so that it has driven a billion-dollar industry. However, whether brain training can really produce these desired outcomes continues to be debated. This is, in part, because the literature is replete with studies that use ill-defined criteria for establishing transferable improvements to cognition, often using single training and outcome measures with small samples. To overcome these limitations, we conducted a large-scale online study to examine whether practices and beliefs about brain training are associated with better cognition. We recruited a diverse sample of over 1000 participants, who had been using an assortment of brain training programs for up to 5 years. Cognition was assessed using multiple tests that measure attention, reasoning, working memory and planning. We found no association between any measure of cognitive functioning and whether participants were currently "brain training" or not, even for the most committed brain trainers. Duration of brain training also showed no relationship with any cognitive performance measure. This result was the same regardless of participant age, which brain training program they used, or whether they expected brain training to work. Our results pose a significant challenge for "brain training" programs that purport to improve general cognitive functioning among the general population. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Transferencia de Experiencia en Psicología , Juegos de Video/psicología , Adulto , Atención , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Persona de Mediana Edad , Solución de Problemas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Proyectos de Investigación
3.
Sleep ; 41(12)2018 12 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30212878

RESUMEN

Most people will at some point experience not getting enough sleep over a period of days, weeks, or months. However, the effects of this kind of everyday sleep restriction on high-level cognitive abilities-such as the ability to store and recall information in memory, solve problems, and communicate-remain poorly understood. In a global sample of over 10000 people, we demonstrated that cognitive performance, measured using a set of 12 well-established tests, is impaired in people who reported typically sleeping less, or more, than 7-8 hours per night-which was roughly half the sample. Crucially, performance was not impaired evenly across all cognitive domains. Typical sleep duration had no bearing on short-term memory performance, unlike reasoning and verbal skills, which were impaired by too little, or too much, sleep. In terms of overall cognition, a self-reported typical sleep duration of 4 hours per night was equivalent to aging 8 years. Also, sleeping more than usual the night before testing (closer to the optimal amount) was associated with better performance, suggesting that a single night's sleep can benefit cognition. The relationship between sleep and cognition was invariant with respect to age, suggesting that the optimal amount of sleep is similar for all adult age groups, and that sleep-related impairments in cognition affect all ages equally. These findings have significant real-world implications, because many people, including those in positions of responsibility, operate on very little sleep and may suffer from impaired reasoning, problem-solving, and communications skills on a daily basis.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Privación de Sueño/fisiopatología , Sueño/fisiología , Adulto , Trastornos del Conocimiento/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Autoinforme , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA