Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 98
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Cerebellum ; 23(2): 431-443, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36995498

RESUMEN

Each cerebellar hemisphere projects to the contralateral cerebral hemisphere. Previous research suggests a lateralization of cognitive functions in the cerebellum that mirrors the cerebral cortex, with attention/visuospatial functions represented in the left cerebellar hemisphere, and language functions in the right cerebellar hemisphere. Although there is good evidence supporting the role of the right cerebellum with language functions, the evidence supporting the notion that attention and visuospatial functions are left lateralized is less clear. Given that spatial neglect is one of the most common disorders arising from right cortical damage, we reasoned that damage to the left cerebellum would result in increased spatial neglect-like symptoms, without necessarily leading to an official diagnosis of spatial neglect. To examine this disconnection hypothesis, we analyzed neglect screening data (line bisection, cancellation, figure copying) from 20 patients with isolated unilateral cerebellar stroke. Results indicated that left cerebellar patients (n = 9) missed significantly more targets on the left side of cancellation tasks compared to a normative sample. No significant effects were observed for right cerebellar patients (n = 11). A lesion overlap analysis indicated that Crus II (78% overlap), and lobules VII and IX (66% overlap) were the regions most commonly damaged in left cerebellar patients. Our results are consistent with the notion that the left cerebellum may be important for attention and visuospatial functions. Given the poor prognosis typically associated with neglect, we suggest that screening for neglect symptoms, and visuospatial deficits more generally, may be important for tailoring rehabilitative efforts to help maximize recovery in cerebellar patients.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Percepción , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Humanos , Percepción Espacial , Lateralidad Funcional , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Accidente Cerebrovascular/diagnóstico por imagen , Trastornos de la Percepción/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Percepción/patología , Corteza Cerebral , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 242(1): 123-135, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37978080

RESUMEN

Boredom is a prominent experience commonly reported in school settings and associated with poor academic achievement. Little is known, however, about the age-related trajectory of boredom. Here we examined self-reported ratings of boredom in a cross-sectional sample of 8 to 15-year olds (n = 185) as a function of resting state EEG. Results indicated that reports of boredom in school rose as a function of age. Resting state EEG showed a decrease in theta power with age perhaps reflective of increased control. While no effects were evident in beta and alpha bands, we did observe an interaction between boredom and age for frontal asymmetry such that for those higher in boredom, the asymmetry increased with age. Finally, for theta to beta ratios there were main effects of age (i.e., a decrease in theta/beta ratio with age) and boredom such that those higher in boredom had higher theta/beta ratios over frontal and central brain areas. The results are discussed in the context of prior work on school-related boredom and provide several important avenues for further research.


Asunto(s)
Tedio , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Adolescente , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Estudios Transversales , Encéfalo , Cabeza
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(10): 4434-4439, 2019 03 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30782798

RESUMEN

Foraging is a goal-directed behavior that balances the need to explore the environment for resources with the need to exploit those resources. In Drosophila melanogaster, distinct phenotypes have been observed in relation to the foraging gene (for), labeled the rover and sitter. Adult rovers explore their environs more extensively than do adult sitters. We explored whether this distinction would be conserved in humans. We made use of a distinction from regulatory mode theory between those who "get on with it," so-called locomotors, and those who prefer to ensure they "do the right thing," so-called assessors. In this logic, rovers and locomotors share similarities in goal pursuit, as do sitters and assessors. We showed that genetic variation in PRKG1, the human ortholog of for, is associated with preferential adoption of a specific regulatory mode. Next, participants performed a foraging task to see whether genetic differences associated with distinct regulatory modes would be associated with distinct goal pursuit patterns. Assessors tended to hug the boundary of the foraging environment, much like behaviors seen in Drosophila adult sitters. In a patchy foraging environment, assessors adopted more cautious search strategies maximizing exploitation. These results show that distinct patterns of goal pursuit are associated with particular genotypes of PRKG1, the human ortholog of for.


Asunto(s)
Proteína Quinasa Dependiente de GMP Cíclico Tipo I/genética , Proteína Quinasa Dependiente de GMP Cíclico Tipo I/metabolismo , Variación Genética , Adolescente , Adulto , Animales , Proteínas Quinasas Dependientes de GMP Cíclico/genética , Proteínas Quinasas Dependientes de GMP Cíclico/metabolismo , Proteínas de Drosophila/genética , Proteínas de Drosophila/metabolismo , Drosophila melanogaster , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 239(6): 1807-1825, 2021 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33829298

RESUMEN

Previous research shows that individuals who tend to get bored frequently and intensely-the highly boredom prone-are more likely to engage in risky behaviors. However, these studies are based largely on self-reports. Here we address this gap and suggest that noisy decision-making (DM) is a potential driver for this relationship between boredom proneness and risk-taking. In Study 1, eighty-six participants completed the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) while EEG was recorded. We found blunted feedback processing with higher boredom proneness, as indexed by reduced feedback-P3 amplitudes. Risk taking, as indexed by the BART, was not higher in the highly boredom prone. In Study 2a (N = 404) we directly tested the noisy DM hypothesis in an online sample using a binary choice task, and found that with higher boredom proneness, participants were more likely to alternate between choices on a trial-to-trial basis, but were not more likely to choose the risky alternative. These findings were replicated in a new sample (Study 2b), and extended to the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Study 3). In the IGT we found increased choice switching and reduced feedback sensitivity with higher boredom proneness. Once again, higher risk taking as indexed by the IGT was not evident in the highly boredom prone. Overall, our findings suggest that boredom proneness is associated with noisy decision-making (i.e., a tendency to alternate more between choice options regardless of risk level), and not risk-seeking per se. That is, the highly boredom prone are not necessarily attracted to risks, but rather, may be insensitive to risks due to reduced feedback sensitivity.


Asunto(s)
Tedio , Juego de Azar , Toma de Decisiones , Retroalimentación , Humanos , Asunción de Riesgos , Autoinforme
5.
Psychol Res ; 85(2): 828-841, 2021 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31858214

RESUMEN

Children until the age of five are only able to reverse an ambiguous figure when they are informed about the second interpretation. In two experiments, we examined whether children's difficulties would extend to a continuous version of the ambiguous figures task. Children (Experiment 1: 66 3- to 5-year olds; Experiment 2: 54 4- to 9-year olds) and adult controls saw line drawings of animals gradually morph-through well-known ambiguous figures-into other animals. Results show a relatively late developing ability to recognize the target animal, with difficulties extending beyond preschool-age. This delay can neither be explained with improvements in theory of mind, inhibitory control, nor individual differences in eye movements. Even the best achieving children only started to approach adult level performance at the age of 9, suggesting a fundamentally different processing style in children and adults.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Niño , Preescolar , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Movimientos Oculares , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
6.
Pers Individ Dif ; 171: 110387, 2021 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35502306

RESUMEN

The state of boredom presents a conundrum: When bored, we want to engage with an activity, but we don't want to engage with whatever is currently available. This conflict is exacerbated when external factors impose restrictions on the range of behaviors we can engage in, which is precisely the scenario we are currently facing, at a global level, during this period of social isolation in response to the COVID-19 pandemic. We collected data from 924 North American participants (530 Male, Mean age = 37.7 years) using the internet-based Mturk platform to examine the relation between self-reports of boredom proneness (using the Short Boredom-Proneness Scale) and individual responses to questions about compliance with social-distancing requirements during the COVID-19 pandemic. Our sample replicated recent findings in boredom research, including a negative correlation between boredom proneness and self-control. We also provide novel evidence that highly boredom prone people have been more likely to break the rules of social isolation in a variety of ways (e.g., fewer hours spent in social isolation, poor adherence to social distancing as evidenced by increased likelihood of holding a social gathering and coming into proximity with more people than recommended). We further demonstrated that boredom proneness substantially mediates the association between self-control and rule-breaking. These results indicate that boredom proneness is a critical factor to consider when encouraging adherence to social isolation.

7.
Brain Cogn ; 142: 105566, 2020 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32402918

RESUMEN

Visual working memory (VWM) involves the encoding and maintenance of visual information over time, with the requirement that object features be accurately bound to spatial locations. We and others have shown that damage to the right hemisphere leads to impaired spatial working memory. Here, we test the notion that right brain damage (RBD) may have consequences for domain general VWM. We had eight RBD patients and a group of healthy control participants perform a VWM task under different loads (1 to 3 items to recall) and spatial competition (high vs. low). All participants were asked to remember the colour of target items presented on the right side of space. Patients showed impaired encoding of information evident in poor precision of memory representations and increased guessing rates even at a set size of only one item. Our data suggests that VWM capacity is severely limited following RBD. Although five of the eight patients presented with neglect, it is not clear whether this deficit in VWM is unique to the syndrome. We suggest that future work should directly pit attention and VWM demands against one another in the same patients to determine whether the confluence of deficits in these domains is the critical determinant of the neglect syndrome. Regardless of the implications for the neglect syndrome, however, our data show that VWM deficits in RBD patients extend into non-spatial feature space.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos de la Memoria , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Percepción Visual , Atención , Corteza Cerebral , Humanos
8.
Cogn Emot ; 34(7): 1517-1523, 2020 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32401144

RESUMEN

We examined the hypothesis that boredom is likely to occur when opportunity costs are high; that is, when there is a high potential value of engaging in activities other than the researcher-assigned activity. To this end, participants were either placed in a room with many possible affordances (e.g. a laptop, puzzle, etc.; affordances condition; n = 121), or they were ushered into an empty room (control condition; n = 107). In both conditions participants were instructed to entertain themselves with only their thoughts (hence, participants in the affordances condition were to refrain from engaging with the available options). As predicted, participants in the affordances condition reported higher levels of boredom compared with those in the control condition. Results suggest that under some conditions, environments that afford alternative activities may be more boring than those that are void of such activities.


Asunto(s)
Tedio , Ambiente , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Dolor , Adulto Joven
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(9): 2507-2518, 2018 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26979438

RESUMEN

Boredom is a ubiquitous human experience that can best be described as an inability to engage with one's environment despite the motivation to do so. Boredom is perceived as a negative experience and demonstrates strong associations with other negatively valenced states including depression and aggression. Although boredom has been shown to be elevated in neurological and psychiatric illnesses, little is known about the neural underpinnings of the state. We scanned the brains of healthy participants under four separate conditions: a resting state scan, a sustained attention task and two video-based mood inductions, one known to produce boredom and another we validated to produce a state of interest or engagement. Using independent components analyses, results showed common regions of correlated activation in posterior regions of the so-called default mode network (DMN) of the brain across all four conditions. The sustained attention and boredom induction scans were differentiated from the resting state scan by the presence of anticorrelated activity-i.e. when DMN regions were active, this region was deactivated-in the anterior insula cortex. This same region demonstrated correlated activity with both the DMN and the regions associated with attentional control during the interest mood induction. We interpret these findings to suggest that boredom represents a failure to engage executive control networks when faced with a monotonous task-in other words, when the task demands some level of engagement (watch the movie, search for infrequent targets), but is so mundane that attempts to do so fail.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Tedio , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Red Nerviosa/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(9): 2493-2505, 2018 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27215775

RESUMEN

Characterized as an agitated state in which the individual is motivated to engage in their environment but all attempts to do so fail to satisfy, boredom represents a disengaged attentional state that is associated with negative affect and poor self-control. There have been anecdotal reports of increased levels of boredom post-traumatic brain injury (TBI). For the first time, we provide objective evidence that TBI patients do indeed experience higher levels of boredom proneness. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that the presence and severity of head injury were a significant positive predictor of levels of boredom proneness and a negative predictor of self-control. As with healthy controls, TBI patients showed a strong negative correlation between boredom proneness and self-control-those with lower levels of self-control exhibited higher levels of boredom proneness. This was despite the fact that our TBI patients reported higher overall levels of self-control (probably concomitant with their older mean age). The TBI patients also showed strong positive correlations between boredom proneness and measures of physical aggression and anger. Together, this suggests that patients with TBI may be more susceptible to increased levels of boredom proneness and other negative affective states that arise as a consequence of failures of self-control.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/fisiología , Ira/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Tedio , Lesiones Traumáticas del Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Autocontrol , Adolescente , Adulto , Conmoción Encefálica/fisiopatología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(6): 1749-1765, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29651518

RESUMEN

We have shown recently that damage to the right hemisphere impairs the ability to update mental models when evidence suggests an old model is no longer appropriate. We argue that this deficit is generic in the sense that it crosses multiple cognitive and perceptual domains. Here, we examined the nature of this updating impairment to determine more precisely the underlying mechanisms. We had right (RBD, N = 12) and left brain damaged (LBD, N = 10) patients perform versions of our picture-morphing task in which pictures gradually morph from one object (e.g., shark) to another (e.g., plane). Performance was contrasted against two groups of healthy older controls, one matched on age (HCO-age-matched, N = 9) and another matched on general level of cognitive ability (HCO-cognitively-matched, N = 9). We replicated our earlier findings showing that RBD patients took longer than LBD patients and HCOs to report seeing the second object in a sequence of morphing images. The groups did not differ when exposed to a morphing sequence a second time, or when responding to ambiguous images outside the morphing context. This indicates that RBD patients have little difficulty alternating between known representations or labeling ambiguous images. Instead, the difficulty lies in generating alternate hypotheses for ambiguous information. Lesion overlay analyses, although speculative given the sample size, are consistent with our fMRI work in healthy individuals in implicating the anterior insular cortex as critical for updating mental models.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/fisiopatología , Función Ejecutiva/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Accidente Cerebrovascular/fisiopatología , Pensamiento/fisiología , Adulto , Malformaciones Vasculares del Sistema Nervioso Central/complicaciones , Malformaciones Vasculares del Sistema Nervioso Central/fisiopatología , Disfunción Cognitiva/etiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Adulto Joven
12.
Conscious Cogn ; 61: 24-37, 2018 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29631194

RESUMEN

Boredom is a common experience associated with a range of negative outcomes. Debate remains as to whether boredom should be considered a high or low arousal state. We employed passages of text to induce either boredom or interest and probed self-reported levels of boredom, arousal, and restlessness. Results replicated known associations between mind-wandering and state boredom (i.e., mind-wandering was highest for the boredom mood induction). Reports of sleepiness (a proxy for arousal level) were highest for the boring induction. While restlessness was not different for the boring and interesting inductions when they were performed first, restlessness was significantly higher for the boredom induction when it was experienced last. We discuss these results within the context of the debate regarding boredom and arousal.


Asunto(s)
Nivel de Alerta/fisiología , Tedio , Agitación Psicomotora/fisiopatología , Somnolencia , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Lectura , Adulto Joven
14.
Cogn Emot ; 31(8): 1741-1748, 2017 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27892776

RESUMEN

Boredom proneness has been linked to various forms of cognitive and affective dysregulation including poor self-control and mind-wandering (MW), as well as depression and aggression. As such, understanding boredom and the associated cognitive and affective components of the experience, represents an important first step in combatting the consequences of boredom for psychological well-being. We surveyed 1928 undergraduate students on measures of boredom proneness, self-control, MW, depression and aggression to investigate how these constructs were related. Hierarchical regression analysis indicated that self-control operated as a strong negative predictor of boredom proneness. Finally, when controlling for age and self-control, we observed large decreases in the magnitudes of the relationships between boredom proneness and our other measures of interest. Together, these results imply a strong relationship between boredom proneness and cognitive and affective dysregulation, and show that individual levels of self-control can account for the lion's share of variance in the relationships between boredom, cognition, and affect.


Asunto(s)
Afecto , Tedio , Cognición , Autocontrol/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Agresión/psicología , Atención , Depresión/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
15.
Conscious Cogn ; 42: 311-324, 2016 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27115875

RESUMEN

We describe a set of Tufnel problems that arise when repeated use of a fixed-point scale precipitates failures to assess a full range of subjective experiences. As empirical evidence, participants in Study 1 periodically reported their depth of mind wandering on either 5- or 7-point Likert scales during a sustained attention task. The proportion of participants providing maximum scale ratings increased quickly over time-on-task and did so more quickly for the 5-point than for the 7-point group. Participants in Study 2 completed the same task using a 10-point scale before indicating whether and where they could have used a scale extended to "11" during the task. Slightly more than 20% of participants reported needing a scale extension. This Need for 11 was associated with differences in both reports of mind wandering depth and task performance. We conclude that Tufnel problems warrant methodological consideration and reflect interesting constraints on human judgment.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Evaluación Ecológica Momentánea/normas , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/normas , Psicometría/instrumentación , Adulto , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto Joven
16.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(1): 201-22, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25701106

RESUMEN

In many research domains, researchers have employed gradually morphing pictures to study perception under ambiguity. Despite their inherent utility, only a limited number of stimulus sets are available, and those sets vary substantially in quality and perceptual complexity. Here we present normative data for 40 morphing picture series. In all sets, line drawings of pictures of common objects are morphed over 15 iterations into a completely different object. Objects are either morphed from an animate to an inanimate object (or vice versa) or morphed within the animate and inanimate object categories. These pictures, together with the normative naming data presented here, will be of value for research on a diverse range of questions, from perceptual processing to decision making.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Investigación Conductal , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/instrumentación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(12): 3517-26, 2015 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26303026

RESUMEN

Effectively engaging with the world depends on accurate representations of the regularities that make up that world-what we call mental models. The success of any mental model depends on the ability to adapt to changes-to 'update' the model. In prior work, we have shown that damage to the right hemisphere of the brain impairs the ability to update mental models across a range of tasks. Given the disparate nature of the tasks we have employed in this prior work (i.e. statistical learning, language acquisition, position priming, perceptual ambiguity, strategic game play), we propose that a cognitive module important for updating mental representations should be generic, in the sense that it is invoked across multiple cognitive and perceptual domains. To date, the majority of our tasks have been visual in nature. Given the ubiquity and import of temporal information in sensory experience, we examined the ability to build and update mental models of time. We had healthy individuals complete a temporal prediction task in which intervals were initially drawn from one temporal range before an unannounced switch to a different range of intervals. Separate groups had the second range of intervals switch to one that contained either longer or shorter intervals than the first range. Both groups showed significant positive correlations between perceptual and prediction accuracy. While each group updated mental models of temporal intervals, those exposed to shorter intervals did so more efficiently. Our results support the notion of generic capacity to update regularities in the environment-in this instance based on temporal information. The task developed here is well suited to investigations in neurological patients and in neuroimaging settings.


Asunto(s)
Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(3): 751-66, 2015 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25430546

RESUMEN

Damage to the right parietal cortex often leads to a syndrome known as unilateral neglect in which the patient fails to attend or respond to stimuli in left space. Recent work attempting to rehabilitate the disorder has made use of rightward-shifting prisms that displace visual input further rightward. After a brief period of adaptation to prisms, many of the symptoms of neglect show improvements that can last for hours or longer, depending on the adaptation procedure. Recent work has shown, however, that differential effects of prisms can be observed on actions (which are typically improved) and perceptual biases (which often remain unchanged). Here, we present a computational model capable of explaining some basic symptoms of neglect (line bisection behaviour), the effects of prism adaptation in both healthy controls and neglect patients and the observed dissociation between action and perception following prisms. The results of our simulations support recent contentions that prisms primarily influence behaviours normally thought to be controlled by the dorsal stream.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Trastornos de la Percepción/rehabilitación , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Atención , Humanos , Trastornos de la Percepción/fisiopatología , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Espacial
19.
Exp Brain Res ; 232(2): 481-91, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24202238

RESUMEN

Research on the experience and expression of boredom is underdeveloped. The purpose of the present study was to explore the psychophysiological signature of the subjective experience of boredom. Healthy undergraduates (n = 72) viewed previously validated and standardized video clips to induce boredom, sadness, and a neutral affective state, while their heart rate (HR), skin conductance levels (SCL), and cortisol levels were measured. Boredom yielded dynamic psychophysiological responses that differed from the other emotional states. Of particular interest, the physiological signature of boredom relative to sadness was characterized by rising HR, decreased SCL, and increased cortisol levels. This pattern of results suggests that boredom may be associated with both increased arousal and difficulties with sustained attention. These findings may help to resolve divergent conceptualizations of boredom in the extant literature and, ultimately, to enhance our understanding and treatment of clinical syndromes in which self-reported boredom is a prominent symptom.


Asunto(s)
Tedio , Psicofísica , Adolescente , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Respuesta Galvánica de la Piel/fisiología , Frecuencia Cardíaca/fisiología , Humanos , Hidrocortisona/metabolismo , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Saliva/metabolismo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
20.
Exp Brain Res ; 232(6): 1971-87, 2014 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24615155

RESUMEN

It has been hypothesized that many of the cognitive impairments commonly seen after right brain damage (RBD) can be characterized as a failure to build or update mental models. We (Danckert et al. in Neglect as a disorder of representational updating. NOVA Open Access, New York, 2012a; Cereb Cortex 22:2745-2760, 2012b) were the first to directly assess the association between RBD and updating and found that RBD patients were unable to exploit a strongly biased play strategy in their opponent in the children's game rock, paper, scissors. Given that this game required many other cognitive capacities (i.e., working memory, sustained attention, reward processing), RBD patients could have failed this task for various reasons other than a failure to update. To assess the generality of updating deficits after RBD, we had RBD, left brain-damaged (LBD) patients and healthy controls (HCs) describe line drawings that evolved gradually from one figure (e.g., rabbit) to another (e.g., duck) in addition to the RPS updating task. RBD patients took significantly longer to alter their perceptual report from the initial object to the final object than did LBD patients and HCs. Although both patient groups performed poorly on the RPS task, only the RBD patients showed a significant correlation between the two, very different, updating tasks. We suggest these data indicate a general deficiency in the ability to update mental representations following RBD.


Asunto(s)
Lesiones Encefálicas/complicaciones , Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Trastornos de la Percepción/etiología , Trastornos Psicomotores/etiología , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Encéfalo/patología , Lesiones Encefálicas/patología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/diagnóstico , Femenino , Juegos Experimentales , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Trastornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA