Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 12 de 12
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 171(3): 407-418, 2020 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31713853

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Climate change is having a significant impact on biodiversity and increasing attention is therefore being devoted to identifying the behavioral strategies that a species uses to cope with climatic stress. We explore how wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) respond to heat stress, and how behavioral adaptations are used to regulate body temperature. MATERIALS AND METHODS: We implanted wild vervet monkeys with temperature-sensitive data loggers and related the body temperature rhythms of these animals to their use of thermoregulatory behaviors. RESULTS: Environmental temperature had a positive effect on the mean, minima and maxima of daily body temperatures. Environmental temperature had a positive effect on the amount of time that vervet monkeys spent in the shade, and animals that spent more time in the shade had lower body temperature maxima. Drinking water did not have a proximate effect on body temperature, most likely a consequence of their regular access to drinking water. Body temperatures were observed to decrease after swimming events, but tended to return to pre-swim temperatures within 1 hr, suggesting a limited thermal benefit of this behavior. CONCLUSIONS: Our data support the view that vervet monkeys cope well in the heat, and use behavior as a means to aid thermoregulation. The ability of primates to be flexible in their use of thermoregulatory behaviors can contribute positively to their capacity to cope with environmental variability. However, given its broad effect on plant productivity and habitat loss, climate change is a major threat to species' biogeographical distribution and survival.


Asunto(s)
Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Temperatura Corporal , Chlorocebus aethiops/fisiología , Ingestión de Líquidos , Aseo Animal , Natación , Animales , Femenino , Masculino , Actividad Motora , Factores Sexuales , Sudáfrica
2.
Elife ; 82019 11 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31711570

RESUMEN

Baboons, members of the genus Papio, comprise six closely related species distributed throughout sub-Saharan Africa and southwest Arabia. The species exhibit more ecological flexibility and a wider range of social systems than many other primates. This article summarizes our current knowledge of the natural history of baboons and highlights directions for future research. We suggest that baboons can serve as a valuable model for complex evolutionary processes, such as speciation and hybridization. The evolution of baboons has been heavily shaped by climatic changes and population expansion and fragmentation in the African savanna environment, similar to the processes that acted during human evolution. With accumulating long-term data, and new data from previously understudied species, baboons are ideally suited for investigating the links between sociality, health, longevity and reproductive success. To achieve these aims, we propose a closer integration of studies at the proximate level, including functional genomics, with behavioral and ecological studies.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Evolución Biológica , Papio/fisiología , Conducta Social , Envejecimiento , Animales , Ecología , Femenino , Genómica , Masculino , Modelos Animales , Papio/genética , Filogeografía
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 272(1575): 1865-75, 2005 Sep 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16191591

RESUMEN

The hypothesis that the enlarged brain size of the primates was selected for by social, rather than purely ecological, factors has been strongly influential in studies of primate cognition and behaviour over the past two decades. However, the Machiavellian intelligence hypothesis, also known as the social brain hypothesis, tends to emphasize certain traits and behaviours, like exploitation and deception, at the expense of others, such as tolerance and behavioural coordination, and therefore presents only one view of how social life may shape cognition. This review outlines work from other relevant disciplines, including evolutionary economics, cognitive science and neurophysiology, to illustrate how these can be used to build a more general theoretical framework, incorporating notions of embodied and distributed cognition, in which to situate questions concerning the evolution of primate social cognition.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Inteligencia/fisiología , Primates/fisiología , Conducta Social , Animales , Encéfalo/anatomía & histología
4.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 7(11): 494-7, 2003 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14585446

RESUMEN

The 'social brain' hypothesis has had a major impact on the study of comparative cognition. However, despite a strong sense, gained from both experimental and observational work, that monkeys and apes differ from each other, we are still no closer to understanding exactly how they differ. We hypothesize that the dispersed social systems characteristic of ape societies explains why monkeys and apes should differ cognitively. The increased cognitive control and analogical reasoning ability needed to cope with life in dispersed societies also suggests a possible route for human cognitive evolution. This hypothesis is supported by behavioural and neurobiological data, but we need more of both if we are to fully understand how our primate cousins see the world.

5.
J Appl Anim Welf Sci ; 8(2): 131-40, 2005.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16277596

RESUMEN

Welfare improvements for nonhuman animals should aim to satisfy the needs of visitors as well as those of the animals. Little research has been conducted, however, and existing work is confined to zoos in developed countries. This article reports the behavioral responses of Chinese visitors to environmental enrichment improvements in a zoo enclosure. Visit, viewing, and stopping behaviors significantly increased at the transformed exhibit, indicating that it provoked greater visitor interest. Furthermore, increased intragroup behaviors suggested that the exhibit probably motivated visitors to interact socially. The positive impact of the exhibit changes supports the enrichment efforts taking place in zoos around the world. The changes also provide encouragement for zoos in developing countries such as China because greater visitor interest provides a strong argument and an incentive for improving welfare standards.


Asunto(s)
Animales de Zoológico , Arquitectura y Construcción de Instituciones de Salud , Vivienda para Animales , Mandrillus , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Animales , Niño , Preescolar , China , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
6.
Eur J Psychol ; 11(3): 484-93, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27247672

RESUMEN

The current study investigated the association between Machiavellianism and children's peer interactions in the playground using observational methods. Primary school children (N = 34; 17 female), aged 9 to 11 years, completed the Kiddie Mach scale and were observed in natural play during 39 recesses (average observed time = 11.70 hours) over a full school year. Correlations for boys revealed that Machiavellianism was related to more time engaging in direct and indirect aggression, being accepted into other peer groups, and accepting peers into their own social group. Correlations revealed that for girls, Machiavellianism was associated with lower levels of indirect aggression, less time being accepted into other groups and less time accepting and rejecting other children into their own group. This preliminary pilot study indicates that Machiavellianism is associated with children's observed social behaviour and aims to promote future observational research in this area.

7.
J Appl Anim Welf Sci ; 7(4): 243-51, 2004.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15857810

RESUMEN

This article investigates visitor circulation and behaviors within a gallery of primate exhibits in relation to their possible implications for nonhuman animal welfare. When entering a primate house, the majority of visitors (84%) turned right, a pattern upheld throughout all times of the day. These findings demonstrate the existence of the "right-turn" principle, a concept previously identified and investigated in the museum setting. The existence of this circulation pattern in zoos has important implications for the practical management of animal welfare issues because unbalanced or large numbers of visitors at specific enclosures could present a stressful influence. The "direction bias" could not be attributed to demographic or behavioral traits, therefore suggesting that the principle, like similar findings from museum research, generalizes across visitor populations and, therefore, zoos. A visitor sample at another exhibit (located outside the exhibit gallery) did not display a direction bias, suggesting that the marked circulation pattern may be specific to exhibit galleries. The article discusses the significance and consequences of visitor circulation with respect to visitor management and animal welfare.


Asunto(s)
Animales de Zoológico , Conducta Exploratoria , Arquitectura y Construcción de Instituciones de Salud , Vivienda para Animales , Primates , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Bienestar del Animal , Animales , Niño , Preescolar , Arquitectura y Construcción de Instituciones de Salud/normas , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Refuerzo en Psicología
8.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 42(6): 967-80, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24469229

RESUMEN

One hundred and forty-nine 8-11 year-old children (86 males; M = 9 years - 4 months and SD = 7 months) from the UK were administered the Trust Beliefs in Peers scale and were observed in the playground over one school year. Quadratic relations were found between trust beliefs in peers and peer interaction, which varied by gender. Compared to girls with the middle range of trust beliefs, girls with very low beliefs and those with very high beliefs (a) were less accepted/more rejected by the peer group (i.e., lower group interaction, and greater negatively received bids), (b) showed greater indirect aggression (engaged in and received), (c) showed greater non-engagement (i.e., being alone), and (d) showed greater concomitant distress. Compared to children with the middle range of trust beliefs, children with those extreme trust beliefs in peers demonstrated greater direct aggression (engaged in and received) and showed passive behavior (for boys only). The findings supported the conclusion that children, primarily girls, who trust peers too little and those who trust too much are at risk for psychosocial maladjustment.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Interpersonales , Grupo Paritario , Confianza/psicología , Agresión/psicología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Masculino , Conducta Social , Aislamiento Social/psicología , Reino Unido
9.
J Comp Physiol B ; 184(6): 799-809, 2014 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24938639

RESUMEN

We used implanted miniature data loggers to obtain the first measurements of body temperature from a free-ranging anthropoid primate. Vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) living in a highly seasonal, semi-arid environment maintained a lower mean 24-h body temperature in winter (34.6 ± 0.5 °C) than in summer (36.2 ± 0.1 °C), and demonstrated increased heterothermy (as indexed by the 24-h amplitude of their body temperature rhythm) in response to proximal environmental stressors. The mean 24-h amplitude of the body temperature rhythm in summer (2.5 ± 0.1 °C) was lower than that in winter (3.2 ± 0.4 °C), with the highest amplitude for an individual monkey (5.6 °C) recorded in winter. The higher amplitude of the body temperature rhythm in winter was a consequence primarily of lower 24-h minimum body temperatures during the nocturnal phase, when monkeys were inactive. These low minimum body temperatures were associated with low black globe temperature (GLMM, ß = 0.046, P < 0.001), short photoperiod (ß = 0.010, P < 0.001) and low rainfall over the previous 2 months, which we used as a proxy for food availability (ß = 0.001, P < 0.001). Despite the lower average winter minimum body temperatures, there was no change in the lower modal body temperature between winter and summer. Therefore, unlike the regulated physiological adjustments proposed for torpor or hibernation, these minimum winter body temperatures did not appear to reflect a regulated reduction in body temperature. The thermoregulatory plasticity nevertheless may have fitness benefits for vervet monkeys.


Asunto(s)
Aclimatación/fisiología , Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Cercopithecinae/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Estaciones del Año , Animales , Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Modelos Lineales , Fotoperiodo , Lluvia , Sudáfrica , Estadísticas no Paramétricas , Telemetría
10.
IEEE Trans Vis Comput Graph ; 19(12): 2169-78, 2013 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24051783

RESUMEN

We suggest a methodology for analyzing movement behaviors of individuals moving in a group. Group movement is analyzed at two levels of granularity: the group as a whole and the individuals it comprises. For analyzing the relative positions and movements of the individuals with respect to the rest of the group, we apply space transformation, in which the trajectories of the individuals are converted from geographical space to an abstract 'group space'. The group space reference system is defined by both the position of the group center, which is taken as the coordinate origin, and the direction of the group's movement. Based on the individuals' positions mapped onto the group space, we can compare the behaviors of different individuals, determine their roles and/or ranks within the groups, and, possibly, understand how group movement is organized. The utility of the methodology has been evaluated by applying it to a set of real data concerning movements of wild social animals and discussing the results with experts in animal ethology.


Asunto(s)
Gráficos por Computador , Aglomeración , Modelos Estadísticos , Movimiento , Reconocimiento de Normas Patrones Automatizadas/métodos , Vigilancia de la Población/métodos , Interfaz Usuario-Computador , Animales , Humanos , Análisis Espacio-Temporal
11.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 41(2): 325-38, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22956297

RESUMEN

The hypothesis that lonely children show hypervigilance for social threat was examined in a series of three studies that employed different methods including advanced eye-tracking technology. Hypervigilance for social threat was operationalized as hostility to ambiguously motivated social exclusion in a variation of the hostile attribution paradigm (Study 1), scores on the Children's Rejection-Sensitivity Questionnaire (Study 2), and visual attention to socially rejecting stimuli (Study 3). The participants were 185 children (11 years-7 months to 12 years-6 months), 248 children (9 years-4 months to 11 years-8 months) and 140 children (8 years-10 months to 12 years-10 months) in the three studies, respectively. Regression analyses showed that, with depressive symptoms covaried, there were quadratic relations between loneliness and these different measures of hypervigilance to social threat. As hypothesized, only children in the upper range of loneliness demonstrated elevated hostility to ambiguously motivated social exclusion, higher scores on the rejection sensitivity questionnaire, and disengagement difficulties when viewing socially rejecting stimuli. We found that very lonely children are hypersensitive to social threat.


Asunto(s)
Ansiedad/psicología , Atención/fisiología , Hostilidad , Relaciones Interpersonales , Soledad/psicología , Modelos Psicológicos , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Niño , Depresión/psicología , Inglaterra , Medidas del Movimiento Ocular , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Análisis de Regresión , Rechazo en Psicología , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Percepción Social , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
12.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 362(1480): 561-75, 2007 Apr 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17255006

RESUMEN

The social brain hypothesis is a well-accepted and well-supported evolutionary theory of enlarged brain size in the non-human primates. Nevertheless, it tends to emphasize an anthropocentric view of social life and cognition. This often leads to confusion between ultimate and proximate mechanisms, and an over-reliance on a Cartesian, narratively structured view of the mind and social life, which in turn lead to views of social complexity that are congenial to our views of ourselves, rather than necessarily representative of primate social worlds. In this paper, we argue for greater attention to embodied and distributed theories of cognition, which get us away from current fixations on 'theory of mind' and other high-level anthropocentric constructions, and allow for the generation of testable hypotheses that combine neurobiology, psychology and behaviour in a mutually reinforcing manner.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Inteligencia/fisiología , Modelos Biológicos , Primates/fisiología , Conducta Social , Medio Social , Animales
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA