Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 2 de 2
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Child Dev ; 91(5): 1439-1455, 2020 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31682004

RESUMEN

Condemnation is ubiquitous in the social world and adults treat condemnation as a costly signal. We explore when children begin to treat condemnation as a signal by presenting 4- to 9-year-old children (N = 435) with stories involving a condemner of stealing and a noncondemner. Children were asked to predict who would be more likely to steal as well as who should be punished more harshly for stealing. In five studies, we found that 7- to 9-year-old children treat condemnation as a signal-thinking that a condemner is less likely to steal and should be punished more harshly if caught hypocritically stealing later. We discuss the implications of these results for children's emerging understanding of signaling and moral condemnation.


Asunto(s)
Comprensión , Principios Morales , Castigo/psicología , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Preescolar , Análisis Costo-Beneficio , Crimen/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicología Infantil
2.
Dev Psychol ; 60(7): 1187-1202, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38421786

RESUMEN

Children and even infants have clear intuitions about power early in development; they can infer who is dominant and subordinate from observing a single interaction. However, it is unclear what children infer about each individual's status from these interactions-do they think dominants and subordinates will maintain their status when interacting with novel partners? In three experiments, we investigate this question. Children (4- to 10-year-olds, N = 365) heard stories about a dominant and subordinate agent and predicted the dominant or subordinate agent's behavior with a novel agent. In all studies, we found that 7- to 10-year-olds generalized dominance, thinking the dominant would again be dominant or "in charge," both for social power (e.g., granting permission) in Study 1 and physical dominance (e.g., a fistfight) in Studies 2 and 3. Furthermore, although they believed dominant agents would win dominance contests (fistfights), they did not believe they would win contests unrelated to dominance (math contests). Younger children did not generalize social power (Study 1) but did generalize physical dominance (Studies 2 and 3). However, even for physical dominance, their generalizations were less selective (i.e., they believed the dominant would win fistfights and math contests). Notably, neither age group generalized an agent's submissiveness in any of the studies-they did not believe a subordinate agent would again be subordinate when paired with a novel partner. We discuss how these results extend past work on children's developing intuitions about dominance and prompt deeper questions about the inferences children draw from dominance interactions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil , Predominio Social , Humanos , Niño , Masculino , Femenino , Preescolar , Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Generalización Psicológica/fisiología , Percepción Social
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA