Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 80
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Biol Lett ; 20(6): 20240120, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38863390

RESUMEN

What makes an odour pleasant or unpleasant? The inherent properties of the constituent chemical compounds, or the nose of the beholder, driven by idiosyncratic differences and culture-specific learning? Here, 582 individuals, including Tanzanian Hadza hunter-gatherers, Amazonian Tsimane' horticulturalists, Yali from the Papuan highlands and two industrialized populations (Poles, Malaysians), rated the pleasantness of 15 odour samples. We find considerable similarities in odour assessments across cultures, but our data do not fully support a claim regarding the universality of smell preferences. Despite cross-cultural similarities in olfactory assessments, probably driven by odour properties, we suggest that odour availability in ecological and cultural niches bears an undeniable effect on human odour preferences.


Asunto(s)
Comparación Transcultural , Odorantes , Humanos , Adulto , Masculino , Femenino , Adulto Joven , Olfato/fisiología , Polonia , Persona de Mediana Edad , Malasia , Adolescente , Percepción Olfatoria , África Oriental
2.
Arch Sex Behav ; 53(2): 839-857, 2024 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37884798

RESUMEN

Love is a phenomenon that occurs across the world and affects many aspects of human life, including the choice of, and process of bonding with, a romantic partner. Thus, developing a reliable and valid measure of love experiences is crucial. One of the most popular tools to quantify love is Sternberg's 45-item Triangular Love Scale (TLS-45), which measures three love components: intimacy, passion, and commitment. However, our literature review reveals that most studies (64%) use a broad variety of shortened versions of the TLS-45. Here, aiming to achieve scientific consensus and improve the reliability, comparability, and generalizability of results across studies, we developed a short version of the scale-the TLS-15-comprised of 15 items with 5-point, rather than 9-point, response scales. In Study 1 (N = 7,332), we re-analyzed secondary data from a large-scale multinational study that validated the original TLS-45 to establish whether the scale could be truncated. In Study 2 (N = 307), we provided evidence for the three-factor structure of the TLS-15 and its reliability. Study 3 (N = 413) confirmed convergent validity and test-retest stability of the TLS-15. Study 4 (N = 60,311) presented a large-scale validation across 37 linguistic versions of the TLS-15 on a cross-cultural sample spanning every continent of the globe. The overall results provide support for the reliability, validity, and cross-cultural invariance of the TLS-15, which can be used as a measure of love components-either separately or jointly as a three-factor measure.


Asunto(s)
Amor , Conducta Sexual , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Parejas Sexuales , Lenguaje , Psicometría , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
3.
Horm Behav ; 152: 105360, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37062114

RESUMEN

Elevated levels of nausea and vomiting in pregnancy (NVP) and disgust sensitivity have been observed in the first trimester and both are thought to have a protective function for the mother and her fetus. Their aetiology is not clear, however, with previous studies attributing elevated NVP and disgust to various factors including endocrine changes, immunological changes, and psychological variables. To date, no study has directly assessed the relationship between disgust and NVP. Here, we prospectively collected two independent samples (S1 and S2; n1 = 201, n2 = 391) of women in the first trimester of pregnancy, who completed the Index of Nausea, Vomiting, and Retching and the Disgust Scale-Revised. We also measured free ß-human chorionic gonadotropin (hCG) and pregnancy-associated plasma protein A (PAPP-A) in maternal serum. Our results did not confirm any association between NVP and disgust; in addition, they indicate that NVP and disgust may have different proximate causes. Disgust sensitivity was significantly negatively correlated with free ß-hCG and (only in S1) with PAPP-A. In contrast, NVP was significantly positively associated with free ß-hCG levels and (only in S1) with PAPP-A. While low hCG levels seem to be an important indicator for activation of the behavioral immune system in the first trimester, increased hCG levels play a role in stronger symptoms of NVP, a result consistent with previous studies. Levels of PAPP-A are likely part of a larger network of immunological and endocrine responses and do not appear to provide sufficient information for predicting women's NVP and disgust sensitivity.


Asunto(s)
Asco , Complicaciones del Embarazo , Femenino , Humanos , Embarazo , Biomarcadores , Gonadotropina Coriónica Humana de Subunidad beta , Náusea/etiología , Primer Trimestre del Embarazo , Proteína Plasmática A Asociada al Embarazo/metabolismo , Vómitos/etiología
4.
BMC Pregnancy Childbirth ; 23(1): 175, 2023 Mar 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36918818

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: A recent study focusing on dietary predictors of nausea and vomiting in pregnancy (NVP) found that women with higher levels of partner support, and those who had used oral contraception (OC) when they met the father, both tended to report less severe NVP compared with previous non-users or those with less supportive partners. We provide a further test of these factors, using a large sample of women from four countries who retrospectively scored their NVP experience during their first pregnancy. METHODS: We recruited women who had at least one child to participate in a retrospective online survey. In total 2321 women completed our questionnaire including items on demographics, hormonal contraception, NVP, and partner support. We used general linear models and path analysis to analyse our data. RESULTS: Women who had used OC when they met the father of their first child tended to report lower levels of NVP, but the effect size was small and did not survive adding the participant's country to the model. There was no relationship between NVP and partner support in couples who were still together, but there was a significant effect among those couples that had since separated: women whose ex-partner had been relatively supportive reported less severe NVP. Additional analyses showed that women who were older during their first pregnancy reported less severe NVP, and there were also robust differences between countries. CONCLUSIONS: These results provide further evidence for multiple influences on women's experience of NVP symptoms, including levels of perceived partner support.


Asunto(s)
Anticonceptivos Orales , Náusea , Complicaciones del Embarazo , Parejas Sexuales , Apoyo Social , Vómitos , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Embarazo , Anticoncepción/métodos , Anticoncepción/psicología , Conducta Anticonceptiva/psicología , Anticonceptivos Orales/administración & dosificación , Anticonceptivos Orales/uso terapéutico , Composición Familiar , Encuestas Epidemiológicas , Internet , Náusea/etiología , Náusea/prevención & control , Náusea/psicología , Complicaciones del Embarazo/etiología , Complicaciones del Embarazo/prevención & control , Complicaciones del Embarazo/psicología , Estudios Retrospectivos , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Apoyo Social/psicología , Vómitos/etiología , Vómitos/prevención & control , Vómitos/psicología
6.
Psychol Sci ; 29(6): 996-1005, 2018 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29708849

RESUMEN

Although widely cited as strong evidence that sexual selection has shaped human facial-attractiveness judgments, findings suggesting that women's preferences for masculine characteristics in men's faces are related to women's hormonal status are equivocal and controversial. Consequently, we conducted the largest-ever longitudinal study of the hormonal correlates of women's preferences for facial masculinity ( N = 584). Analyses showed no compelling evidence that preferences for facial masculinity were related to changes in women's salivary steroid hormone levels. Furthermore, both within-subjects and between-subjects comparisons showed no evidence that oral contraceptive use decreased masculinity preferences. However, women generally preferred masculinized over feminized versions of men's faces, particularly when assessing men's attractiveness for short-term, rather than long-term, relationships. Our results do not support the hypothesized link between women's preferences for facial masculinity and their hormonal status.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/metabolismo , Masculinidad , Ciclo Menstrual/metabolismo , Conducta Sexual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Saliva , Adulto Joven
7.
Perception ; 46(3-4): 498-515, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27927973

RESUMEN

People tend to choose perfumes to complement their body odour. As kin share some body odour qualities, their ability to select complementary perfumes for relatives might be higher compared with selection for nonrelatives. We tested this in two studies, comparing selection of a perfume for a target man by himself and by either a familiar but unrelated individual (girlfriend; Study 1) or a relative (sister; Study 2). Target men applied the two perfumes (own or other's choice) to their axillae and then wore cotton pads for 12 hr. Collected perfume-body odour blends and perfumes alone were assessed by rater panels. In Study 1, the blends were rated as nominally more pleasant when body odours were mixed with the perfumes selected by girlfriends compared with those selected by target men themselves. In Study 2, body odours mixed with perfumes selected by sisters were rated significantly more attractive than those mixed with perfumes selected by target men. No significant differences were found for attractiveness and pleasantness ratings when perfumes were rated alone, suggesting that it was the resulting blends that were uniquely different. Our results indicate that sisters might be particularly tuned to select suitable perfumes for their siblings.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Familia/psicología , Odorantes , Adolescente , Adulto , Composición Corporal , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Perfumes , Adulto Joven
8.
Arch Sex Behav ; 45(8): 2117-2122, 2016 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26704416

RESUMEN

Women who are regularly cycling exhibit different partner preferences than those who use hormonal contraception. Preliminary evidence appears to suggest that during pregnancy women's partner preferences also diverge from those prevalent while regularly cycling. This is consistent with the general assertion that women's mate preferences are impacted by hormonal variation. During pregnancy, women's preferences are thought to closely resemble those displayed by women who are using hormonal contraception. Here, based on this literature, we compared levels of sexual desire among pregnant women who met their partner while using hormonal contraception and pregnant women who met their partner while regularly cycling. We predicted that women who met their partner while using hormonal contraception would experience higher levels of in-pair sexual desire during pregnancy since these women will have partner preferences that more closely match those prevalent at the time of their partner choice. Our results provided support for the idea that previous contraceptive use/non-use may impact subsequent sexual desire for the partner during pregnancy. Pregnant women who met their partner while using hormonal contraception (N = 37) were shown to have higher levels of in-pair sexual desire than those who met while regularly cycling (N = 47). In contrast, levels of extra-pair desire were not related to previous use/non-use of hormonal contraception. These findings were robust when controlling for a number of relevant individual difference variables known to impact sexual desire. Our results contribute to our understanding of factors affecting relationship functioning during pregnancy.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Anticonceptiva , Conducta Sexual/efectos de los fármacos , Parejas Sexuales , Adulto , Anticonceptivos/farmacología , Femenino , Humanos , Libido , Motivación , Embarazo
9.
Appetite ; 97: 8-15, 2016 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26551789

RESUMEN

Beneficial health properties of garlic, as well as its most common adverse effect - distinctive breath odour - are well-known. In contrast, analogous research on the effect of garlic on axillary odour is currently missing. Here, in three studies varying in the amount and nature of garlic provided (raw garlic in study 1 and 2, garlic capsules in study 3), we tested the effect of garlic consumption on the quality of axillary odour. A balanced within-subject experimental design was used. In total, 42 male odour donors were allocated to either a "garlic" or "non-garlic" condition, after which they wore axillary pads for 12 h to collect body odour. One week later, the conditions were reversed. Odour samples were then judged for their pleasantness, attractiveness, masculinity and intensity by 82 women. We found no significant differences in ratings of any characteristics in study 1. However, the odour of donors after an increased garlic dosage was assessed as significantly more pleasant, attractive and less intense (study 2), and more attractive and less intense in study 3. Our results indicate that garlic consumption may have positive effects on perceived body odour hedonicity, perhaps due to its health effects (e.g., antioxidant properties, antimicrobial activity).


Asunto(s)
Ajo , Odorantes/análisis , Percepción Olfatoria , Adulto , Índice de Masa Corporal , Peso Corporal , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Masculinidad , Adulto Joven
10.
Aggress Behav ; 41(4): 331-4, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25236530

RESUMEN

Recent research has reported an association between facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR) and both fighting performance and judgments of formidability in a sample of mixed martial arts (MMA) combatants. The results provide evidence of fWHR being associated with sporting performance and aggression in men. However, it has been argued that the effect of fWHR might be a by-product of associations between body size and behavioral measures. Here we tested whether fWHR is associated with perceived aggressiveness, fighting ability and success in physical confrontation, while controlling for body size, also in a sample of MMA fighters. We found that perceived fighting ability was predicted by weight but not by fWHR. In contrast, both fWHR and body weight independently predicted perceived aggressiveness. Furthermore, we found positive associations between fWHR and fighting performance which appear to be independent of body size. Our findings provide further support for the proposal that fWHR is associated with fighting ability and perceived aggression, and that these effects are independent of body size. Therefore, fWHR might be considered as a viable and reliable marker for inference of success in male intra-sexual competition.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/fisiología , Cara/anatomía & histología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Artes Marciales/fisiología , Percepción Social , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
11.
Psychol Sci ; 25(7): 1497-503, 2014 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24818612

RESUMEN

Hormonal fluctuation across the menstrual cycle explains temporal variation in women's judgment of the attractiveness of members of the opposite sex. Use of hormonal contraceptives could therefore influence both initial partner choice and, if contraceptive use subsequently changes, intrapair dynamics. Associations between hormonal contraceptive use and relationship satisfaction may thus be best understood by considering whether current use is congruent with use when relationships formed, rather than by considering current use alone. In the study reported here, we tested this congruency hypothesis in a survey of 365 couples. Controlling for potential confounds (including relationship duration, age, parenthood, and income), we found that congruency in current and previous hormonal contraceptive use, but not current use alone, predicted women's sexual satisfaction with their partners. Congruency was not associated with women's nonsexual satisfaction or with the satisfaction of their male partners. Our results provide empirical support for the congruency hypothesis and suggest that women's sexual satisfaction is influenced by changes in partner preference associated with change in hormonal contraceptive use.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Anticoncepción/métodos , Heterosexualidad/psicología , Satisfacción Personal , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Masculinidad , Ciclo Menstrual , Persona de Mediana Edad , Conducta Sexual , Adulto Joven
12.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(28): 11710-4, 2011 Jul 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21709272

RESUMEN

Contextual cues of genetic relatedness to familiar individuals, such as cosocialization and maternal-perinatal association, modulate prosocial and inbreeding-avoidance behaviors toward specific potential siblings. These findings have been interpreted as evidence that contextual cues of kinship indirectly influence social behavior by affecting the perceived probability of genetic relatedness to familiar individuals. Here, we test a more general alternative model in which contextual cues of kinship can influence the kin-recognition system more directly, changing how the mechanisms that regulate social behavior respond to cues of kinship, even in unfamiliar individuals for whom contextual cues of kinship are absent. We show that having opposite-sex siblings influences inbreeding-relevant perceptions of facial resemblance but not prosocial perceptions. Women with brothers were less attracted to self-resembling, unfamiliar male faces than were women without brothers, and both groups found self-resemblance to be equally trustworthy for the same faces. Further analyses suggest that this effect is driven by younger, rather than older, brothers, consistent with the proposal that only younger siblings exhibit the strong kinship cue of maternal-perinatal association. Our findings provide evidence that experience with opposite-sex siblings can directly influence inbreeding-avoidance mechanisms and demonstrate a striking functional dissociation between the mechanisms that regulate inbreeding and the mechanisms that regulate prosocial behavior toward kin.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Modelos Psicológicos , Relaciones entre Hermanos , Hermanos/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Belleza , Consanguinidad , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Conducta Social , Adulto Joven
13.
Psychol Sci ; 24(9): 1664-72, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23818656

RESUMEN

Accurate assessment of competitive ability is a critical component of contest behavior in animals, and it could be just as important in human competition, particularly in human ancestral populations. Here, we tested the role that facial perception plays in this assessment by investigating the association between both perceived aggressiveness and perceived fighting ability in fighters' faces and their actual fighting success. Perceived aggressiveness was positively associated with the proportion of fights won, after we controlled for the effect of weight, which also independently predicted perceived aggression. In contrast, perception of fighting ability was confounded by weight, and an association between perceived fighting ability and actual fighting success was restricted to heavyweight fighters. Shape regressions revealed that aggressive-looking faces are generally wider and have a broader chin, more prominent eyebrows, and a larger nose than less aggressive-looking faces. Our results indicate that perception of aggressiveness and fighting ability might cue different aspects of success in male-male physical confrontation.


Asunto(s)
Agresión/psicología , Rendimiento Atlético/psicología , Conducta Competitiva/fisiología , Expresión Facial , Artes Marciales/psicología , Percepción Social , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
14.
Chem Senses ; 38(2): 175-86, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23196070

RESUMEN

Emotion and odor scales (EOS) measuring odor-related affective feelings were recently developed for three different countries (Switzerland, United Kingdom, and Singapore). The first aim of this study was to investigate gender and cultural differences in verbal affective response to odors, measured with EOS and the usual pleasantness scale. To better understand this variability, the second aim was to investigate the link between affective reports and olfactory knowledge (familiarity and identification). Responses of 772 participants smelling 56-59 odors were collected in the three countries. Women rated odors as more intense and identified them better in all countries, but no reliable sex differences were found for verbal affective responses to odors. Disgust-related feelings revealed odor-dependent sex differences, due to sex differences in identification and categorization. Further, increased odor knowledge was related to more positive affects as reported with pleasantness and odor-related feeling evaluations, which can be related to top-down influences on odor representation. These top-down influences were thought, for example, to relate to beliefs about odor properties or to categorization (edible vs. nonedible). Finally, the link between odor knowledge and olfactory affect was generally asymmetrical and significant only for pleasant odors, not for unpleasant ones that seemed to be more resistant to cognitive influences. This study, for the first time using emotional scales that are appropriate to the olfactory domain, brings new insights into the variability of affective responses to odors and its relationship to odor knowledge.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Odorantes/análisis , Percepción Olfatoria , Adulto , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Factores Sexuales , Singapur , Suiza , Reino Unido , Adulto Joven
15.
Nat Genet ; 35(1): 103-6, 2003 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12937417

RESUMEN

Females express mate preferences for genetically dissimilar males, especially with respect to the major histocompatibility complex, MHC, and for males whose sexually selected signals indicate high genetic quality. The balance of selection pressure on each trait will depend on how females weight these desirable qualities under different conditions, but this has not been tested empirically. Here we show in mice that although MHC dissimilarity and a 'good genes' indicator (investment in scent-marking) both have a role in determining female preference, their relative influence can vary depending on the degree of variability in each trait among available males. Such interactions between condition-dependent and disassortative mate choice criteria suggest a mechanism by which female choice can contribute to maintenance of additive genetic variance in both the MHC and condition-dependent traits, even under consistent directional selection.


Asunto(s)
Complejo Mayor de Histocompatibilidad , Conducta Sexual Animal , Animales , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Congénicos
16.
Behav Brain Sci ; 36(1): 34-5, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23211367

RESUMEN

McCullough et al. propose an evolved cognitive revenge system which imposes retaliatory costs on aggressors. They distinguish between this and other forms of punishment (e.g., those administered by judges) which are not underpinned by a specifically designed evolutionary mechanism. Here we outline mechanisms and circumstances through which the revenge system might nonetheless infiltrate decision-making within the criminal justice system.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica , Agresión/psicología , Cognición , Perdón , Motivación , Humanos
17.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 10587, 2023 06 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37391459

RESUMEN

As a social species, humans deprived of contact find loneliness a potentially distressing condition. Recent research emphasises the influence of touch on alleviating loneliness. This research found that touch reduces feelings of neglect, a subscale of loneliness. Affectionate touch, which demonstrates care or affection, has been previously linked to well-being in couples. Here, we investigated whether the effect of simulated touch during a video conversation might be sufficient to influence feelings of loneliness. Sixty participants answered a survey about their home life and relationships, including items that assessed the frequency of touch and feelings of loneliness. Following this, they participated in an online video call with three conditions: audio only, audio and video, or audio, video with simulated touch (a virtual 'high-five'). Finally, immediately after the call, they repeated the loneliness questionnaire. We found that loneliness scores were reduced following the call, but there was no difference among conditions and no effect of a virtual touch. However, we did find a significant association between the frequency of touch in a relationship and the expression of loneliness, with individuals in low-touch relationships having loneliness scores more comparable to single participants than to those in high-touch relationships. Additionally, extraversion played a major role in moderating the effect of touch in relationships. These results emphasise the importance of physical contact in lowering feelings of loneliness within relationships and the ability of calls to lower feelings of loneliness, regardless of whether they include video or simulated touch.


Asunto(s)
Emociones , Soledad , Humanos , Placer , Comunicación , Extraversión Psicológica
18.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 4752, 2023 03 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36959238

RESUMEN

Disgust is an essential part of the behavioral immune system, protecting the individual from infection. According to the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis (CPH), disgust sensitivity increases in times of immunosuppression, potentially including pregnancy. We aimed to replicate a previous study observing longitudinal changes in disgust sensitivity in pregnant women. Additionally, for the first time, we explored how recent health problems influence these changes. To do this, we obtained disgust sensitivity measures from 94 women in each trimester and in early postpartum. In contrast to the original study, where disgust sensitivity was highest in the first trimester, we found that overall and animal reminder disgust increased across pregnancy and after birth. In line with the CPH, women who were recently sick in the first trimester had elevated disgust sensitivity at that time. Although disgust sensitivity was significantly higher in the second trimester and postpartum period compared to the first trimester in mothers pregnant with a male fetus, the overall results regarding the effect of fetus sex on disgust sensitivity were mixed. It seems that changing levels of disgust sensitivity during pregnancy and postpartum result from a suite of physiological and psychological changes that occur during this sensitive period of a woman's life.


Asunto(s)
Asco , Embarazo , Femenino , Masculino , Humanos , Periodo Posparto , Mujeres Embarazadas/psicología , Trimestres del Embarazo , Parto
19.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; : 17456916231188147, 2023 Sep 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37669015

RESUMEN

Although chemical signaling is an essential mode of communication in most vertebrates, it has long been viewed as having negligible effects in humans. However, a growing body of evidence shows that the sense of smell affects human behavior in social contexts ranging from affiliation and parenting to disease avoidance and social threat. This article aims to (a) introduce research on human chemical communication in the historical context of the behavioral sciences; (b) provide a balanced overview of recent advances that describe individual differences in the emission of semiochemicals and the neural mechanisms underpinning their perception, that together demonstrate communicative function; and (c) propose directions for future research toward unraveling the molecular principles involved and understanding the variability in the generation, transmission, and reception of chemical signals in increasingly ecologically valid conditions. Achieving these goals will enable us to address some important societal challenges but are within reach only with the aid of genuinely interdisciplinary approaches.

20.
Proc Biol Sci ; 279(1732): 1430-6, 2012 Apr 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21993500

RESUMEN

Hormonal variation over the menstrual cycle alters women's preferences for phenotypic indicators of men's genetic or parental quality. Hormonal contraceptives suppress these shifts, inducing different mate preference patterns among users and non-users. This raises the possibility that women using oral contraception (OC) choose different partners than they would do otherwise but, to date, we know neither whether these laboratory-measured effects are sufficient to exert real-world consequences, nor what these consequences would be. Here, we test for differences in relationship quality and survival between women who were using or not using OC when they chose the partner who fathered their first child. Women who used OC scored lower on measures of sexual satisfaction and partner attraction, experienced increasing sexual dissatisfaction during the relationship, and were more likely to be the one to initiate an eventual separation if it occurred. However, the same women were more satisfied with their partner's paternal provision, and thus had longer relationships and were less likely to separate. These effects are congruent with evolutionary predictions based on cyclical preference shifts. Our results demonstrate that widespread use of hormonal contraception may contribute to relationship outcome, with implications for human reproductive behaviour, family cohesion and quality of life.


Asunto(s)
Anticonceptivos Orales/efectos adversos , Conducta Sexual/efectos de los fármacos , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ciclo Menstrual/efectos de los fármacos , Ciclo Menstrual/psicología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Satisfacción Personal
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA