Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 42
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Pers ; 92(2): 620-635, 2024 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37269092

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Across four studies, we examined whether certain personality traits cue prejudice and serve as identity threat cues. BACKGROUND: Stigmatized group members may be vigilant to personality cues that signal prejudice. METHOD: In Study 1 (N = 76), perceivers selected traits and behaviors associated with disagreeableness and closedness to experience as indicators of prejudice. In Studies 2-4, perceivers with stigmatized identities (Total N = 907) learned about a target person who was depicted as disagreeable or agreeable (Studies 2 and 3) and as disagreeable or another trait matched on perceived negativity (i.e., low in conscientiousness, Study 4). RESULTS: Participants perceived the disagreeable target as more discriminatory and hierarchy-endorsing (Studies 2-4), more morally disengaged (Study 3), and more likely to discriminate against stigmatized identity groups (Studies 2 and 4) than the agreeable or low conscientious targets. The relationship between target disagreeableness and perceived discrimination was partially explained by higher perceived hierarchy endorsing beliefs (Studies 2-4) and perceived moral disengagement (Study 3). CONCLUSIONS: This research finds that perceivers with stigmatized identities utilize target disagreeableness as a cue of identity threat, inferring that disagreeable people are more likely to be discriminatory, prejudicial, and hierarchy-endorsing than agreeable and low conscientious people.


Asunto(s)
Personalidad , Prejuicio , Humanos , Principios Morales , Miedo
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(39): 24154-24164, 2020 09 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32929006

RESUMEN

Science is undergoing rapid change with the movement to improve science focused largely on reproducibility/replicability and open science practices. This moment of change-in which science turns inward to examine its methods and practices-provides an opportunity to address its historic lack of diversity and noninclusive culture. Through network modeling and semantic analysis, we provide an initial exploration of the structure, cultural frames, and women's participation in the open science and reproducibility literatures (n = 2,926 articles and conference proceedings). Network analyses suggest that the open science and reproducibility literatures are emerging relatively independently of each other, sharing few common papers or authors. We next examine whether the literatures differentially incorporate collaborative, prosocial ideals that are known to engage members of underrepresented groups more than independent, winner-takes-all approaches. We find that open science has a more connected, collaborative structure than does reproducibility. Semantic analyses of paper abstracts reveal that these literatures have adopted different cultural frames: open science includes more explicitly communal and prosocial language than does reproducibility. Finally, consistent with literature suggesting the diversity benefits of communal and prosocial purposes, we find that women publish more frequently in high-status author positions (first or last) within open science (vs. reproducibility). Furthermore, this finding is further patterned by team size and time. Women are more represented in larger teams within reproducibility, and women's participation is increasing in open science over time and decreasing in reproducibility. We conclude with actionable suggestions for cultivating a more prosocial and diverse culture of science.


Asunto(s)
Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Ciencia/tendencias , Mujeres , Autoria , Humanos , Difusión de la Información , Publicación de Acceso Abierto
3.
Matern Child Health J ; 22(10): 1407-1417, 2018 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29564605

RESUMEN

Objective To examine the association of father early engagement behaviors and infant low birth weight (LBW) among unmarried, urban couples. Methods Participants were from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study, a birth-cohort study of urban families. We conducted cross-sectional analyses of data from interviews with unmarried mothers and fathers (N = 2726) that took place at the time of their child's birth. Early engagement behaviors were based on fathers' self-report of whether during the pregnancy they gave mothers money to buy things for the baby, helped in other ways like providing transportation to prenatal clinics, and attended the birth. Results Most (68.9%) fathers engaged in all three early engagement behaviors; 22% engaged in 2 behaviors; and 9.1% engaged in 1 or 0 early engagement behaviors. LBW more than doubled when comparing infants of fathers who engaged in all three early engagement behaviors (9.6% predicted probability of LBW) to those fathers who engaged in no early engagement behaviors (over 22% predicted probability of LBW). Conclusion Infant and maternal health may benefit from intervention to encourage positive father engagement during pregnancy.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Padre-Hijo , Padre/psicología , Recién Nacido de Bajo Peso , Estado Civil , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Adulto , Niño , Estudios de Cohortes , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Recién Nacido , Masculino , Madres/psicología , Conducta Paterna , Atención Perinatal , Embarazo , Estados Unidos/epidemiología , Población Urbana
4.
Psychol Sci ; 28(4): 445-461, 2017 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28186861

RESUMEN

In the current research, we posited the stigma-by-prejudice-transfer effect, which proposes that stigmatized group members (e.g., White women) are threatened by prejudice that is directed at other stigmatized group members (e.g., African Americans) because they believe that prejudice has monolithic qualities. While most stigma researchers assume that there is a direct correspondence between the attitude of prejudiced individuals and the targets (i.e., sexism affects women, racism affects racial minorities), the five studies reported here demonstrate that White women can be threatened by racism (Study 1, 3, 4, and 5) and men of color by sexism (Study 2). Robust to perceptions of liking and the order in which measures were administered, results showed that prejudice transfers between racism and sexism were driven by the presumed social dominance orientation of the prejudiced individual. In addition, important downstream consequences, such as the increased likelihood of anticipated stigma, expectations of unfair treatment, and the attribution of negative feedback to sexism, appeared for stigmatized individuals.


Asunto(s)
Racismo/psicología , Sexismo/psicología , Predominio Social , Percepción Social , Estigma Social , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
5.
Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol ; 23(1): 154-164, 2017 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27269508

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: This study investigates how racial identity and phenotypicality (i.e., racial ambiguity) shape the perception of biracial individuals in both White and Black perceivers. We investigated complex racial categorization and its downstream consequences, such as perceptions of discrimination. METHOD: We manipulated racial phenotypicality (Black or racially ambiguous) and racial identity (Black or biracial) to test these cues' influence on Black and White race categorizations in a sample of both White (n = 145) and Black (n = 152) identified individuals. RESULTS: Though racial identity and phenotypicality information influenced deliberate racial categorization, White and Black participants used the cues in different ways. For White perceivers, racial identity and phenotypicality additively influenced Black categorization. For Black perceivers, however, racial identity was only used in Black categorization when racial ambiguity was high. Perceived discrimination was related to White (but not Black) perceivers' distribution of minority resources to targets, however Black categorization related to perceived discrimination for Black perceivers only. CONCLUSION: By demonstrating how Black and White individuals use identity and phenotype information in race perceptions, we provide a more complete view of the complexities of racial categorization and its downstream consequences. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Población Negra/psicología , Señales (Psicología) , Fenotipo , Estereotipo , Población Blanca/psicología , Adulto , Diversidad Cultural , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Grupos Minoritarios , Grupos Raciales , Identificación Social
6.
Psychol Sci ; 27(4): 502-17, 2016 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26976082

RESUMEN

In two national samples, we examined the influence of interracial exposure in one's local environment on the dynamic process underlying race perception and its evaluative consequences. Using a mouse-tracking paradigm, we found in Study 1 that White individuals with low interracial exposure exhibited a unique effect of abrupt, unstable White-Black category shifting during real-time perception of mixed-race faces, consistent with predictions from a neural-dynamic model of social categorization and computational simulations. In Study 2, this shifting effect was replicated and shown to predict a trust bias against mixed-race individuals and to mediate the effect of low interracial exposure on that trust bias. Taken together, the findings demonstrate that interracial exposure shapes the dynamics through which racial categories activate and resolve during real-time perceptions, and these initial perceptual dynamics, in turn, may help drive evaluative biases against mixed-race individuals. Thus, lower-level perceptual aspects of encounters with racial ambiguity may serve as a foundation for mixed-race prejudice.


Asunto(s)
Cara , Prejuicio/etnología , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Percepción Social , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Población Negra , Femenino , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Población Blanca
7.
J Sex Med ; 13(9): 1408-1413, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27555510

RESUMEN

INTRODUCTION: Sexual pleasure is a central aspect of human sexuality; however, no validated measurements exist that assess sexual pleasure. We present a preliminary validation study of the psychometric properties of a Sexual Pleasure Scale (SPS), based on the three items developed by Sanchez, Crocker and Boike to measure sexual pleasure. The SPS is a brief and easy-to-implement instrument that assesses the extent of sexual pleasure experienced from sexual relationships, sexual activities, and sexual intimacy. AIM: To assess the validity of the SPS in a subgroup of patients diagnosed with sexual dysfunction (n = 89) and a non-clinical community sample (n = 188) of Portuguese men and women. METHODS: We provide an initial examination of the reliability (eg, Cronbach α), convergent validity (eg, with measurements of sexual satisfaction), and divergent validity (eg, with measurements of body satisfaction) of the SPS. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: The survey included a sociodemographic questionnaire and a set of questionnaires to test the psychometric properties of the SPS. RESULTS: The reliability study showed a high Cronbach value (α = 0.94). Convergent validity of the SPS with the measurements described showed mostly moderate to high statistically significant positive correlations, whereas the criterion-related validity showed the expected low non-significant correlation. The results also showed that the SPS shows strong sensitivity to discriminate people with from those without sexual problems. CONCLUSION: Results from the clinical population indicate that the SPS has good psychometric qualities and is a reliable measurement of sexual pleasure with applicability in clinical practice and clinical research but shows little variability within the community sample.


Asunto(s)
Heterosexualidad/psicología , Orgasmo , Placer , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios/normas , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud , Satisfacción Personal , Psicometría/estadística & datos numéricos , Calidad de Vida , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
8.
Prev Med ; 84: 34-40, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26724519

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Mortality and morbidity data suggest that men have shorter life expectancies than women and outrank women on several leading causes of death. These gendered disparities may be influenced by psychosocial factors like masculinity. METHODS: Three studies (Total N=546) examined the role of masculinity in men's doctor choices and doctor-patient interactions. In Studies 1 and 2, men completed measures of masculinity, gender bias, and doctor preference. Using structural equation modeling, we tested the direct relationship between masculinity and male doctor preference and the indirect relationship of masculinity on male doctor preference through an association with gendered competence stereotypes. Participants in Study 3 disclosed symptoms in private followed by disclosure to a male or female interviewer in a clinical setting. Using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), we examined the interaction among symptom reporting, masculinity and doctor gender, controlling for participant comfort. RESULTS: In Study 1, results suggested that masculinity encouraged choice of a male doctor directly and indirectly via beliefs that men make more competent doctors than women; Study 2 directly replicated the results of Study 1. In Study 3, independent of participant comfort, an interaction between interviewer gender and masculinity emerged such that men scoring higher on masculinity reported symptoms less consistently to male interviewers (relative to higher scoring men reporting to female interviewers); the reverse was found for men scoring low on masculinity. CONCLUSIONS: Taken together these studies suggest that masculinity may affect men's health by encouraging choice of a male doctor with whom doctor-patient communication may be impaired.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Comunicación en Salud , Masculinidad , Relaciones Médico-Paciente , Sexismo , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Análisis de Varianza , Competencia Clínica , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Salud del Hombre , Persona de Mediana Edad
9.
Arch Sex Behav ; 44(4): 961-70, 2015 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25287972

RESUMEN

Women are less likely than men to engage in sexually agentic behavior (e.g., initiating sexual encounters), despite the benefits associated with sexual agency (Kiefer & Sanchez, 2007). Two studies examined possible explanations, related to person perception, for gender differences in sexually agentic behavior. In Study 1, participants viewed the dating profiles of targets who were either high or low on sexual agency and rated sexually agentic targets as more desirable but also riskier sexual partners (i.e., having more previous sexual partners), as well as more selfish partners overall. Participants believed the agentic female targets to be the most desirable but also to have the highest number of previous sexual partners. In Study 2, female participants weighed the importance and consequences of sexual agency differently than male participants. Based on the two studies, we suggest that although men and women are judged similarly for sexual agency, women may refrain from sexual agency because they view the traits and characteristics that are perceived to go hand in hand with sexual agency more negatively.


Asunto(s)
Trabajo Sexual/estadística & datos numéricos , Conducta Sexual/estadística & datos numéricos , Percepción Social , Valores Sociales , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Relaciones Interpersonales , Masculino , Distribución por Sexo , Parejas Sexuales , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
10.
Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol ; 21(1): 65-75, 2015 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25090151

RESUMEN

The present study examined the relationship of perceived parental closeness and parental ethnic identity on personal ethnic identity and colorblindness beliefs in 275 part-White biracial Americans (M age = 23.88). Respondents completed online measures of their personal ethnic identity (minority, White, and multiracial), perceived parental ethnic identity, parental closeness, and attitudes about the state of race relations and the need for social action in the United States. Using path modeling, results show that part-White biracial individuals perceive their ethnic identity to be strongly linked to their parental racial identities, especially when they had closer parental relationships. Moreover, stronger minority identity was linked to less colorblind attitudes, and greater White identity was linked to greater colorblind attitudes suggesting that patterns of identity may influence how biracial individuals view race-relations and the need for social action. Implications for biracial well-being and their understanding of prejudice and discrimination are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Actitud/etnología , Etnicidad/psicología , Percepción/fisiología , Grupos Raciales/psicología , Identificación Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Cultura , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Grupos Minoritarios/psicología , Relaciones Padres-Hijo/etnología , Padres/psicología , Prejuicio , Relaciones Raciales , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
11.
Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol ; 21(3): 315-25, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25313429

RESUMEN

The present studies examined whether colorblind diversity messages, relative to multicultural diversity messages, serve as an identity threat that undermines performance-related outcomes for individuals at the intersections of race and gender. We exposed racial/ethnic majority and minority women and men to either a colorblind or multicultural diversity statement and then measured their expectations about overall diversity, anticipated bias, and group task performance (Study 1, N = 211), as well as their expectations about distinct race and gender diversity and their actual performance on a math test (Study 2, N = 328). Participants expected more bias (Study 1) and less race and gender diversity (Study 2) after exposure to a colorblind versus a multicultural message. However, the colorblind message was particularly damaging for women of color, prompting them to expect the least diversity overall and to perform worse (Study 1), as well as to actually perform worse on a math test (Study 2) than the multicultural message. White women demonstrated the opposite pattern, performing better on the math test in the colorblind versus the multicultural condition, whereas racial minority and majority men's performances were not affected by different messages about diversity. We discuss the importance of examining psychological processes that underscore performance-related outcomes at the junction of race and gender.


Asunto(s)
Diversidad Cultural , Identidad de Género , Política Organizacional , Grupos Raciales/psicología , Adolescente , Adulto , Sesgo , Etnicidad , Femenino , Humanos , Identificación Psicológica , Masculino , Matemática , Grupos Minoritarios/psicología , Población Blanca , Adulto Joven
12.
J Adolesc Health ; 74(2): 327-339, 2024 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37804300

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: Comprehensive sex education (CSE) encourages safer sex behavior for teens and young adults. However, young people recognize a gap between sex education as taught in the classroom and the reality of their sexual experiences. Thus, CSE should take into account the perspectives of its target population. METHODS: The current project assesses young people's sexual uncertainties by analyzing open-ended responses that were anonymously submitted during in-person sex education sessions. These education programs were administered in middle and high schools in New York State by facilitators from Planned Parenthood Hudson Peconic, Inc. The authors analyzed 1,335 responses from a racially diverse sample of students between the ages of 10 and 21, with 75% of participants between 15 and 17 years old. RESULTS: Using content analysis, the authors identified 49 recurring content codes in participants' responses, which were organized into 16 categories. Most responses centered around pregnancy, sexually transmitted infections, and how to prevent these outcomes. However, responses also highlighted topics that are not always covered with nuance and transparency, if at all, in sex education (e.g., withdrawal, effectiveness of condoms and other contraception, anal and oral sex). Additionally, gender analyses indicated that girls made greater reference to pain, while boys made greater reference to pleasure, which has implications for girls' development of a positive sexuality. DISCUSSION: These results should be interpreted with a social equity lens to inform the development of needs-driven, target-based CSE programming.


Asunto(s)
Educación Sexual , Enfermedades de Transmisión Sexual , Masculino , Embarazo , Femenino , Adolescente , Adulto Joven , Humanos , Niño , Adulto , Conducta Sexual , Enfermedades de Transmisión Sexual/prevención & control , Estudiantes , Anticoncepción
13.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 2024 May 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38780609

RESUMEN

Many colleges and universities seek to leverage the promise of intergroup contact theory by adopting housing policies that randomly assign first-year students to roommates, with the goal of increasing intergroup contact. Yet, it is unclear whether random roommate assignment policies increase cross-race contact, whether this (potentially involuntary, but sanctioned by authorities) contact improves racial attitudes or behaviors, or how these effects may differ for racial majority and minority students. The present studies used a natural experiment of random roommate assignment to directly test roommate relationship, attitudinal, and behavioral changes based on roommate race. Across three samples drawn from two student cohorts, the random assignment policy increased the likelihood that students had a cross-race roommate because without the policy, students tended to self-segregate by race. Moreover, selecting (Study 1) or being randomly assigned (Study 2) a cross-race roommate was associated with having more racial outgroup friends and demonstrating more positive verbal and nonverbal behavior during a novel cross-race interaction (Study 3). There were no roommate group (same vs. cross-race roommates) differences in relationship quality, and the results were largely independent of participant race. These findings suggest randomized roommate assignment is a promising avenue for universities to promote cross-race contact amid persistent racial segregation on college campuses with limited negative consequences for relationship quality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

14.
J Sex Res ; : 1-10, 2024 Feb 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38329940

RESUMEN

Orgasm gaps between heterosexual men and women, and for women across sexual orientations, are well-established in research. However, no large-scale assessments of orgasm frequency by race/ethnicity exist. Here, we analyzed 10 years of cross-sectional Singles in America survey data between 2011 and 2021 to investigate the orgasm gap at the intersection of gender and racial/ethnic identity (i.e. White, Black, Hispanic/Latino, and Asian) for heterosexual participants (N = 27,347). White and Hispanic/Latino men reported greater orgasm frequency than Black and Asian men. Hispanic/Latino women reported the greatest orgasm frequency, and Asian women reported the lowest, with White and Black women's frequencies in between. The orgasm gap between men and women was largest for White (d = 0.89) and Asian (d = 0.86) groups, although Asian participants reported a lower orgasm frequency than White participants overall. The orgasm gap was smaller for Hispanic/Latino participants (d = 0.66), because Hispanic/Latino women reported a greater orgasm frequency than other racial/ethnic groups of women. The orgasm gap was smaller for Black participants (d = 0.61), because Black men reported a lower orgasm frequency than some other racial/ethnic groups of men. This descriptive study serves as an important starting point for future research on orgasm experiences across racial/ethnic groups.

15.
Psychol Sci ; 24(7): 1142-50, 2013 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23658253

RESUMEN

Do men base their self-worth on relationships less than do women? In an assessment of lay beliefs, men and women alike indicated that men are less reliant on relationships as a source of self-worth than are women (Study 1). Yet relationships may make a different important contribution to the self-esteem of men. Men reported basing their self-esteem on their own relationship status (whether or not they were in a relationship) more than did women, and this link was statistically mediated by the perceived importance of relationships as a source of social standing (Studies 1 and 2). Finally, when relationship status was threatened, men displayed increased social-standing concerns, whereas women displayed increased interdependence concerns (Study 3). Together, these findings demonstrate that both men and women rely on relationships for self-worth, but that they derive self-esteem from relationships in different ways.


Asunto(s)
Relaciones Interpersonales , Autoimagen , Factores Sexuales , Clase Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Matrimonio , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estereotipo , Adulto Joven
16.
Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol ; 19(4): 461-7, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23914743

RESUMEN

Racial essentialism refers to the widely held belief that race is a biological, stable, and natural category. Although research finds very little evidence that race has biological underpinnings, racial essentialist beliefs persist and are linked to negative outgroup consequences. This study initially demonstrates that label and visual ambiguity concurrently inform racial categorization. It then tests whether exposure to racially ambiguous targets (a) challenges essentialism when ambiguous targets are labeled with biracial categories and (b) reinforces essentialism when ambiguous targets identify with monoracial categories. The results showed that White perceivers (N = 84) who were exposed to racially ambiguous, biracially labeled targets showed reductions in their essentialist thinking about race, whereas perceivers who were exposed to racially ambiguous, monoracially labeled targets showed increases in their essentialist beliefs.


Asunto(s)
Grupos Raciales/psicología , Identificación Social , Percepción Social , Adulto , Negro o Afroamericano/psicología , Negro o Afroamericano/estadística & datos numéricos , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Juicio/fisiología , Masculino , Grupos Raciales/estadística & datos numéricos , Estereotipo , Población Blanca/psicología , Población Blanca/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto Joven
17.
J Soc Psychol ; : 1-19, 2023 Aug 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37598383

RESUMEN

Past research demonstrates that prejudice toward women and Black Americans often co-occur in individuals. The present studies examine factors related to accuracy in estimating the co-occurrence, or overlap, of prejudice toward women and Black Americans. Across two studies, criterion overlap percentages were computed using national datasets and separate participant samples estimated prejudice overlap. Results indicate that beliefs about the generalized nature of prejudice can improve accuracy by reducing faulty underestimation of the overlap in anti-Black racism and sexism. In addition to greater displayed accuracy in perceptions of prototypical perpetrators of prejudice (i.e., estimates of White men compared to White women), the present work suggests that accuracy is improved when estimating sexist attitudes from racist attitudes, rather than vice versa. Together, this work documents the accuracy of prejudice overlap perceptions, for the first time, and factors that facilitate accuracy (i.e., perpetrator prototypicality, known prejudicial attitude), with implications for intergroup dynamics research.

18.
J Health Psychol ; 27(7): 1569-1582, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33678046

RESUMEN

The present work experimentally examines how identity cues that signal minority inclusion contribute to sexual minorities' (SM) healthcare visit expectations. We find that minority representation cues reduced SM's (N = 188) expectations of a healthcare provider's bias and increased perceived provider cultural competency which was, in turn, associated with lower anticipated identity-based devaluation and greater sexual orientation disclosure comfort. Providers' diversity-valuing statements had mixed effects highlighting the importance of more concrete indicators of inclusion in this context. This work suggests that a lack of identity safety cues in healthcare settings may contribute to disparate health outcomes for sexual minority populations.


Asunto(s)
Revelación , Minorías Sexuales y de Género , Señales (Psicología) , Atención a la Salud , Femenino , Identidad de Género , Humanos , Intención , Masculino , Conducta Sexual
19.
Arch Sex Behav ; 40(1): 99-110, 2011 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20174863

RESUMEN

Deriving self-worth from romantic relationships (relationship contingency) may have implications for women's sexual motives in relationships. Because relationship contingency enhances motivation to sustain relationships to maintain positive self-worth, relationship contingent women may engage in sex to maintain and enhance their relationships (relational sex motives). Using structural equation modeling on Internet survey data from a convenience sample of 462 women in heterosexual and lesbian relationships, we found that greater relationship contingency predicted greater relational sex motives, which simultaneously predicted both sexual satisfaction and dissatisfaction via two distinct motivational states. Having sex to improve intimacy with one's partner was associated with greater sexual satisfaction and autonomy, while having sex to earn partner's approval was associated with sexual dissatisfaction and inhibition. While some differences exist between lesbian and heterosexual relationships, relationship contingency had sexual costs and benefits, regardless of relationship type.


Asunto(s)
Heterosexualidad/psicología , Homosexualidad Femenina/psicología , Satisfacción Personal , Autoimagen , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Parejas Sexuales/psicología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Internet , Relaciones Interpersonales , Persona de Mediana Edad , Autorrevelación , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Adulto Joven
20.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 47(5): 705-727, 2021 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32791890

RESUMEN

Research addressing the increasing multiracial population (i.e., identifying with two or more races) is rapidly expanding. This meta-analysis (k = 55) examines categorization patterns consistent with hypodescent, or the tendency to categorize multiracial targets as their lower status racial group. Subgroup analyses suggest that operationalization of multiracial (e.g., presenting photos of racially ambiguous faces, or ancestry information sans picture), target gender, and categorization measurement (e.g., selecting from binary choices: Black or White; or multiple categorization options: Black, White, or multiracial) moderated categorization patterns. Operationalizing multiracial as ancestry, male targets, and measuring categorization with binary or multiple Likert-type scale outcomes supported hypodescent. However, categorizing multiracial targets as not their lower status racial group occurred for female targets or multiple categorization options. Evidence was mixed on whether perceiver and target race were related to categorization patterns. These results point to future directions for understanding categorization processes and multiracial perception.


Asunto(s)
Población Negra , Población Blanca , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Grupos Raciales , Proyectos de Investigación
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA