Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 43
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
País de afiliación
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 35(8): 1246-1261, 2023 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37172135

RESUMEN

Visual perception is closely related to body movements and action, and it is known that processing visual stimuli is facilitated at the hand or at the hand-movement goal. Such facilitation suggests that there may be an attentional process associated with the hands or hand movements. To investigate the underlying mechanisms of visual attention at a hand-movement goal, we conducted two experiments to examine whether attention at the hand-movement goal is a process independent from endogenous attention. Endogenous attention is attention that is intentionally focused on a location, feature, or object. We controlled the hand-movement goal and endogenous attention separately to investigate the spatial profiles of the two types of attention. A visual target was presented either at the goal of hand movement (same condition) or at its opposite side (opposite condition) while steady-state visual-evoked potential (SSVEP) was used to estimate the spatial distributions of the facilitation effect from the 2 types of attention around the hand-movement goal and around the visual target through EEG. We estimated the spatial profile of attentional modulation for the hand-movement goal by taking the difference in SSVEP amplitude between conditions with and without hand movement, thereby obtaining the effect of visual endogenous attention alone. The results showed a peak at the hand-movement goal, independent of the location of the visual target where participants intentionally focused their attention (endogenous attention). We also found differences in the spatial extent of attentional modulation. Spatial tuning was narrow around the hand-movement goal (i.e., attentional facilitation only at the goal location) but was broadly tuned around the focus of endogenous attention (i.e., attentional facilitation spreading over adjacent stimulus locations), which was obtained from the condition without hand movement. These results suggest the existence of two separate mechanisms, one underlying the attention at the hand-movement goal and another underlying endogenous attention.


Asunto(s)
Objetivos , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Potenciales Evocados Visuales , Movimiento/fisiología , Electroencefalografía
2.
Perception ; 51(9): 658-671, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35979618

RESUMEN

Previous studies established that a salient collinear structure impairs local visual search. A display organization hypothesis states that the vertical grouping of elemental bars in the search display may selectively increase the salience of the local target in the background than that in the collinear distractor, leading to the collinear search impairment. Three displays were designed to test this hypothesis. A classical search display was adopted as a baseline. A diagonal search display was created with tilted bars, making perceptual organization diagonal and should reduce collinear search impairment. An illusory search display was designed by using abutting line illusion to emphasize the vertical grouping direction, which should increase collinear search impairment. A manipulation check was conducted with an online survey to understand the perceptual organization of the three displays. Results showed that the probability to perceive the stimuli grouping in the vertical direction was strongest in the illusory display and the least in the diagonal display. Nevertheless, the collinear search impairment did not vary with these manipulations, argue against the display organization hypothesis. We speculate that the search impairment might associate with the perceptual organization of the collinear distractor per se, rather than the perceptual organization of the background.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Atención , Humanos , Percepción Visual
3.
J Vis ; 20(3): 6, 2020 03 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32207770

RESUMEN

While visual size preferences regarding still objects have been investigated and linked to the "canonical size" effect-where preferred on-screen size was significantly related to objects' real-world size-the visual size preferences related to moving images of natural scenes has not been researched. In this study, we measured the preferred size of moving images of natural scenes and short duration and investigated the effect of viewing distance on size preferences. Our results showed that the preferred size varied strongly depending on content, and we found moving images' canonical size effect. The preferred size in images of scenery was significantly larger than in images of persons, and there was a positive correlation between the preferred size and the real-world physical size of the main subjects in the images. When the viewing distance was doubled, the preferred size increased about 10% as a ratio to screen size-in contrast to the findings of a previous study. While the rationale for these findings is not yet clear, our analysis suggests that neither the motion component in the images nor the nature of their background area are contributing factors. We suggest that environment, viewing distance, and screen size may contribute to this effect.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven
4.
J Vis ; 20(12): 6, 2020 11 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33196769

RESUMEN

Previous claims of the number of color categories and corresponding basic color terms in modern Mandarin Chinese remain irreconcilable, mainly due to the shortage in objectively evaluating the basicness of color terms with statistical significance. Therefore the present study applied k-means cluster analysis to investigate native Mandarin Chinese speakers' color naming data of 330 color chips similar to those used in World Color Survey. Results confirmed that there are 11 basic color categories among modern Mandarin speakers in Taiwan, one corresponding to each basic color term. Results also showed that observers overwhelmingly agreed in their use of Mandarin color terms, including those that had yielded ambiguous results in previous studies (gray, brown, pink, and orange). There is significant cross-language similarity when comparing the distribution of color categories in the World Color Survey chart with American English and Japanese data. The motif analysis and group mutual information analysis suggest that Mandarin color terms used in Taiwan describe very similar categories and are, hence, similarly precise in communicating color information as those in Japanese and American English. These results show that three languages of fundamentally different cultures and histories have very similar basic color terms.


Asunto(s)
Pueblo Asiatico , Clasificación , Color , Lenguaje , Adulto , China , Análisis por Conglomerados , Percepción de Color , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estados Unidos , Adulto Joven
5.
J Vis ; 18(9): 17, 2018 09 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30242388

RESUMEN

When a rotating object (inducer) is briefly replaced by a static face image (test stimulus), the orientation of the face appears to shift in the rotation direction of the inducer (object orientation induction, OOI). The OOI effect suggests that there is a process to continuously analyze and update the orientation of an object in motion. We investigated the perception of object orientation in motion, examining potential factors that contribute to OOI. Experiment 1 showed that the phenomenon is general to objects rather than specific to faces; OOI could be observed with non-face objects. Experiment 2 showed that OOI is a 3D effect, as the orientation shift for a bent-wire object depended on its configuration in the depth dimension. Experiment 3 showed that salient features are necessary to indicate the intrinsic orientation of the inducing object for producing OOI. Experiment 4 showed that change in the facing direction of the inducer object is a crucial factor for OOI, but neither the object shape nor its identity is important. A strong OOI effect was observed even when the inducer kept changing its shape and identity, as long as its direction change generated continuous rotation. Finally, Experiment 5 showed that OOI is a phenomenon in the pathway for fast visual processing. A single inducer presented shorter than 100ms before influenced the perceived orientation of the test stimulus. Together these results suggest that there is a predictive process that continuously analyzes and updates the orientation of rotating objects, independently of their identification.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Orientación Espacial/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Rotación , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
6.
J Vis ; 17(3): 1, 2017 03 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28249298

RESUMEN

Despite numerous prior studies, important questions about the Japanese color lexicon persist, particularly about the number of Japanese basic color terms and their deployment across color space. Here, 57 native Japanese speakers provided monolexemic terms for 320 chromatic and 10 achromatic Munsell color samples. Through k-means cluster analysis we revealed 16 statistically distinct Japanese chromatic categories. These included eight chromatic basic color terms (aka/red, ki/yellow, midori/green, ao/blue, pink, orange, cha/brown, and murasaki/purple) plus eight additional terms: mizu ("water")/light blue, hada ("skin tone")/peach, kon ("indigo")/dark blue, matcha ("green tea")/yellow-green, enji/maroon, oudo ("sand or mud")/mustard, yamabuki ("globeflower")/gold, and cream. Of these additional terms, mizu was used by 98% of informants, and emerged as a strong candidate for a 12th Japanese basic color term. Japanese and American English color-naming systems were broadly similar, except for color categories in one language (mizu, kon, teal, lavender, magenta, lime) that had no equivalent in the other. Our analysis revealed two statistically distinct Japanese motifs (or color-naming systems), which differed mainly in the extension of mizu across our color palette. Comparison of the present data with an earlier study by Uchikawa & Boynton (1987) suggests that some changes in the Japanese color lexicon have occurred over the last 30 years.


Asunto(s)
Biometría/métodos , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Análisis por Conglomerados , Color , Femenino , Humanos , Japón , Masculino
7.
J Vis ; 15(11): 12, 2015 Aug 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26275213

RESUMEN

Pursuit eye movements correlate with perceived motion in both velocity and direction, even without retinal motion. Cortical cells in the monkey medial temporal region generate signals for initiating pursuit eye movements and respond to retinal motion for perception. However, recent studies suggest multiple motion processes, fast and slow, even for low-level motion. Here we investigated whether the relationship with pursuit eye movements is different for fast and slow motion processes, using a motion aftereffect technique with superimposed low- and high-spatial-frequency gratings. A previous study showed that the low- and high-spatial-frequency gratings adapt the fast and slow motion processes, respectively, and that a static test probes the slow motion process and a flicker test probes the fast motion process (Shioiri & Matsumiya, 2009). In the present study, an adaptation stimulus was composed of two gratings with different spatial frequencies and orientations but the same temporal frequency, moving in the orthogonal direction of ±45° from the vertical. We measured the directions of perceived motion and pursuit eye movements to a test stimulus presented after motion adaptation with changing relative contrasts of the two adapting gratings. Pursuit eye movements were observed in the same direction as that of the motion aftereffects, independent of the relative contrasts of the two adapting gratings, for both the static and flicker tests. These results suggest that pursuit eye movements and perception share motion signals in both slow and fast motion processes.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Seguimiento Ocular Uniforme/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica , Adulto , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Retina/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal , Adulto Joven
8.
J Vis ; 15(14): 3, 2015.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26448145

RESUMEN

We investigated the influence of attention and motion on the sensitivity of flicker detection for a target among distractors. Experiment 1 showed that when the target and distractors were moving, detection performance plummeted compared to when they were not moving, suggesting that the most sensitive detectors were local, temporal frequency-tuned receptive fields. With the stimuli in motion, a qualitatively different strategy was required and this led to much reduced performance. Cueing, which specified the target location with 100% validity, had no effect for targets that had little or no motion, suggesting that the flicker was sufficiently salient in this case to attract attention to the target without requiring any search. For targets with medium to high speeds, however, cueing provided a strong increase in sensitivity over uncued performance. This suggests a significant advantage for localizing and tracking the target and so sampling the luminance changes from only one trajectory. Experiment 2 showed that effect of attention was to increase the efficiency and duration of signal integration for the moving target. Overall, the results show that flicker sensitivity for a moving target relies on a much less efficient process than detection of static flicker, and that this less efficient process is facilitated when attention can select the relevant trajectory and ignore the others.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos
9.
Vis Neurosci ; 31(6): 387-400, 2014 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25136894

RESUMEN

The appearance of colors can be affected by their spatiotemporal context. The shift in color appearance according to the surrounding colors is called color induction or chromatic induction; in particular, the shift in opponent color of the surround is called chromatic contrast. To investigate whether chromatic induction occurs even when the chromatic surround is imperceptible, we measured chromatic induction during interocular suppression. A multicolor or uniform color field was presented as the surround stimulus, and a colored continuous flash suppression (CFS) stimulus was presented to the dominant eye of each subject. The subjects were asked to report the appearance of the test field only when the stationary surround stimulus is invisible by interocular suppression with CFS. The resulting shifts in color appearance due to chromatic induction were significant even under the conditions of interocular suppression for all surround stimuli. The magnitude of chromatic induction differed with the surround conditions, and this difference was preserved regardless of the viewing conditions. The chromatic induction effect was reduced by CFS, in proportion to the magnitude of chromatic induction under natural (i.e., no-CFS) viewing conditions. According to an analysis with linear model fitting, we revealed the presence of at least two kinds of subprocesses for chromatic induction that reside at higher and lower levels than the site of interocular suppression. One mechanism yields different degrees of chromatic induction based on the complexity of the surround, which is unaffected by interocular suppression, while the other mechanism changes its output with interocular suppression acting as a gain control. Our results imply that the total chromatic induction effect is achieved via a linear summation of outputs from mechanisms that reside at different levels of visual processing.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Ocular/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Color , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicometría
10.
J Imaging ; 10(2)2024 Jan 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38392084

RESUMEN

Head-mounted displays (HMDs) are becoming more and more popular as a device for displaying a virtual reality space, but how real are they? The present study attempted to quantitatively evaluate the degree of reality achieved with HMDs by using a perceptual phenomenon as a measure. Lightness constancy is an ability that is present in human visual perception, in which the perceived reflectance (i.e., the lightness) of objects appears to stay constant across illuminant changes. Studies on color/lightness constancy in humans have shown that the degree of constancy is high, in general, when real objects are used as stimuli. We asked participants to make lightness matches between two virtual environments with different illuminant intensities, as presented in an HMD. The participants' matches showed a high degree of lightness constancy in the HMD; our results marked no less than 74.2% (84.8% at the maximum) in terms of the constancy index, whereas the average score on the computer screen was around 65%. The effect of head-tracking ability was confirmed by disabling that function, and the result showed a significant drop in the constancy index but that it was equally effective when the virtual environment was generated by replay motions. HMDs yield a realistic environment, with the extension of the visual scene being accompanied by head motions.

11.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 24(8): 1779-93, 2012 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22360591

RESUMEN

Endogenous attention modulates the amplitude and phase coherence of steady-state visual-evoked potentials (SSVEPs). In efforts to decipher the neural mechanisms of attentional modulation, we compared the time course of attentional modulation of SSVEP amplitude (thought to reflect the magnitude of neural population activity) and phase coherence (thought to reflect neural response synchronization). We presented two stimuli flickering at different frequencies in the left and right visual hemifields and asked observers to shift their attention to either stimulus. Our results demonstrated that attention increased SSVEP phase coherence earlier than it increased SSVEP amplitude, with a positive correlation between the attentional modulations of SSVEP phase coherence and amplitude. Furthermore, the behavioral dynamics of attention shifts were more closely associated with changes in phase coherence than with changes in amplitude. These results are consistent with the possibility that attention increases neural response synchronization, which in turn leads to increased neural population activity.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Electroencefalografía/métodos , Potenciales Evocados Visuales/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Electroencefalografía/instrumentación , Electrooculografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Estadísticos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Relación Señal-Ruido , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
12.
J Vis ; 12(6)2012 Jun 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22739268

RESUMEN

We investigated the low-level motion mechanisms for color and luminance and their integration process using 2D and 3D motion aftereffects (MAEs). The 2D and 3D MAEs obtained in equiluminant color gratings showed that the visual system has the low-level motion mechanism for color motion as well as for luminance motion. The 3D MAE is an MAE for motion in depth after monocular motion adaptation. Apparent 3D motion can be perceived after prolonged exposure of one eye to lateral motion because the difference in motion signal between the adapted and unadapted eyes generates interocular velocity differences (IOVDs). Since IOVDs cannot be analyzed by the high-level motion mechanism of feature tracking, we conclude that a low-level motion mechanism is responsible for the 3D MAE. Since we found different temporal frequency characteristics between the color and luminance stimuli, MAEs in the equiluminant color stimuli cannot be attributed to a residual luminance component in the color stimulus. Although a similar MAE was found with a luminance and a color test both for 2D and 3D motion judgments after adapting to either color or luminance motion, temporal frequency characteristics were different between the color and luminance adaptation. The visual system must have a low-level motion mechanism for color signals as for luminance ones. We also found that color and luminance motion signals are integrated monocularly before IOVD analysis, showing a cross adaptation effect between color and luminance stimuli. This was supported by an experiment with dichoptic presentations of color and luminance tests. In the experiment, color and luminance tests were presented in the different eyes dichoptically with four different combinations of test and adaptation: color or luminance test in the adapted eye after color or luminance adaptation. Findings of little or no influence of the adaptation/test combinations indicate the integration of color and luminance motion signals prior to the binocular IOVD process.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Visión de Colores/fisiología , Humanos , Iluminación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología
13.
Cereb Cortex Commun ; 3(1): tgac005, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35224493

RESUMEN

Facilitation of visual processing has been reported in the space near the hand. To understand the underlying mechanism of hand proximity attention, we conducted experiments that isolated hand-related effects from top-down attention, proprioceptive information from visual information, the position effect from the influence of action, and the distance effect from the peripersonal effect. The flash-lag effect was used as an index of attentional modulation. Because the results showed that the flash-lag effect was smaller at locations near the hand, we concluded that there was a facilitation effect of the visual stimuli around the hand location identified through proprioceptive information. This was confirmed by conventional reaction time measures. We also measured steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP) in order to investigate the spatial properties of hand proximity attention and top-down attention. The results showed that SSVEP reflects the effect of top-down attention but not that of hand proximity attention. This suggests that the site of hand proximity attention is at a later stage of visual processing, assuming that SSVEP responds to neural activities at the early stages. The results of left-handers differed from those of right-handers, and this is discussed in relation to handedness variation.

14.
Brain Cogn ; 75(3): 292-8, 2011 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21330028

RESUMEN

The effect of the visual hemifield to which spatial attention was oriented on the activities of the posterior parietal and occipital visual cortices was examined using functional near-infrared spectroscopy in order to investigate the neural substrates of voluntary visuospatial attention. Our brain imaging data support the theory put forth in a previous psychophysical study, namely, the attentional resources for the left and right visual hemifields are distinct. Increasing the attentional load asymmetrically increased the brain activity. Increase in attentional load produced a greater increase in brain activity in the case of the left visual hemifield than in the case of the right visual hemifield. This asymmetry was observed in all the examined brain areas, including the right and left occipital and parietal cortices. These results suggest the existence of asymmetrical inhibitory interactions between the hemispheres and the presence of an extensive inhibitory network.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Inhibición Psicológica , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Espectroscopía Infrarroja Corta , Percepción Visual/fisiología
15.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 28(4): 704-12, 2011 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21478969

RESUMEN

We modified a two-stage model for color discrimination proposed in a previous study [Color Res. Appl.25, 105 (2000)]; in order to extend the model to wider conditions, we considered the conditions with luminance modulations in addition to color modulations. Using the modified model, we successfully predicted color discrimination data with test color changes along both the chromatic and luminance axes under a variety of background colors. Both qualitative and quantitative assessments in modeling showed that nonlinearity is required in both the cone and the cone-opponent stages to interpret adaptation effects of both color and luminance on color discrimination. This fact suggests that the nonlinear properties at each stage have different roles in color perception.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Dinámicas no Lineales
16.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 3995, 2021 02 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33597567

RESUMEN

Two different motion mechanisms have been identified with motion aftereffect (MAE). (1) A slow motion mechanism, accessed by a static MAE, is sensitive to high-spatial and low-temporal frequency; (2) a fast motion mechanism, accessed by a flicker MAE, is sensitive to low-spatial and high-temporal frequency. We examined their respective responses to global motion after adapting to a global motion pattern constructed of multiple compound Gabor patches arranged circularly. Each compound Gabor patch contained two gratings at different spatial frequencies (0.53 and 2.13 cpd) drifting in opposite directions. The participants reported the direction and duration of the MAE for a variety of global motion patterns. We discovered that static MAE durations depended on the global motion patterns, e.g., longer MAE duration to patches arranged to see rotation than to random motion (Exp 1), and increase with global motion strength (patch number in Exp 2). In contrast, flicker MAEs durations are similar across different patterns and adaptation strength. Further, the global integration occurred at the adaptation stage, rather than at the test stage (Exp 3). These results suggest that slow motion mechanism, assessed by static MAE, integrate motion signals over space while fast motion mechanisms do not, at least under the conditions used.

17.
Foods ; 10(8)2021 Jul 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34441502

RESUMEN

Crispness is among the most important food textures that contribute significantly to palatability. This study investigated the association between the perceived crispness and palatability of five types of Japanese rice crackers known as "kakinotane." Two experiments were conducted using the temporal dominance of sensations (TDS) and temporal drivers of liking (TDL) methods. As descriptors for the TDS evaluation, we used 10 Japanese onomatopoeias to indicate various attributes of crispness. We also measured the mastication sounds and electromyography (EMG) activity during mastication. Principal component analysis data revealed that principal component 1, representing moisture characteristics, contributed more than 60% in both experiments. The palatability of the stimulus, which was described as having a very soft, moist, and sticky texture, BETA-BETA, was significantly lower than the others. However, there was no significant relationship between the amplitude of mastication sound or EMG activity and palatability. We demonstrated that naïve university students can discriminate the fine nuances of the crispness of "kakinotane" using the TDS and TDL methods. Our findings also suggested that the onomatopoeias used as descriptors in the TDS method had a greater influence on describing the nuances of food texture than the physiological data.

18.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 11507, 2021 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34075138

RESUMEN

Collinear search impairment (CSI) is a phenomenon where a task-irrelevant collinear structure impairs a target search in a visual display. It has been suggested that CSI is monocular, occurs without the participants' access to consciousness and is possibly processed at an early visual site (e.g. V1). This effect has frequently been compared with a well-documented opposite effect called attentional capture (AC), in which salient and task-irrelevant basic features (e.g. color, orientation) enhance target detection. However, whether this phenomenon can be attributed to non-attentional factors such as collinear facilitation (CF) has not yet been formally tested. Here we used one well-established property of CF, i.e. that target contrast modulates its effect direction (facilitation vs suppression), to examine whether CSI shared similar signature profiles along different contrast levels. In other words, we tested whether CSI previously observed at the supra-threshold level was reduced or reversed at near-threshold contrast levels. Our results showed that, regardless of the luminance contrast levels, participants spent a longer time searching for targets displayed on the salient singleton collinear structure than those displayed off the structure. Contrast invariance suggests that it is unlikely that CSI is exclusively sub-served by an early vision mechanism (e.g. CF).

19.
J Vis ; 10(10): 10, 2010 Aug 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20884475

RESUMEN

We investigated the effect of attention on the flash-lag effect (FLE) in order to determine whether the FLE can be used to estimate the effect of visual attention. The FLE is the effect that a flash aligned with a moving object is perceived to lag the moving object, and several studies have shown that attention reduces its magnitude. We measured the FLE as a function of the number or speed of moving objects. The results showed that the effect of cueing, which we attributed the effect of attention, on the FLE increased monotonically with the number or the speed of the objects. This suggests that the amount of attention can be estimated by measuring the FLE, assuming that more amount of attention is required for a larger number or faster speed of objects to attend. On the basis of this presumption, we attempted to measure the spatial spread of visual attention by FLE measurements. The estimated spatial spreads were similar to those estimated by other experimental methods.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Humanos
20.
Vision Res ; 172: 11-26, 2020 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32388210

RESUMEN

Perception of motion in depth is one of the most important visual functions for living in the three-dimensional world. Two binocular cues have been investigated for motion in depth: inter-ocular velocity difference (IOVD) and changing disparity (CD). IOVD provides direction information directly by comparing velocity signals from the two retinas. In this study, we propose for the first time a motion-in-depth model of IOVD that predicts motion-in-depth direction. The model is based on a psychophysical assumption that there are four channels tuned to different directions in depth (Journal of Physiology 235 (1973) 17-29). We modeled these channels by combining outputs of low-level motion detectors that are sensitive to left and right retinal stimulation. Using these channels, we constructed a model of motion in depth that successfully predicted a variety of psychophysical results including direction discrimination, perceived direction, spatial frequency tuning, effect of speed on rotation in depth, effect of lateral motion direction, and effect of binocular and temporal correlations.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Modelos Teóricos , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Psicofísica , Retina/fisiología , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA