Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 71
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Proc Biol Sci ; 289(1968): 20212244, 2022 02 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35105243

RESUMEN

Social bonds enhance fitness in many group-living animals, generating interest in the processes that create individual variation in sociality. Previous work on female baboons shows that early life adversity and temperament both influence social connectedness in adulthood. Early life adversity might shape sociality by reducing ability to invest in social relationships or through effects on attractiveness as a social partner. We examine how females' early life adversity predicts sociality and temperament in wild olive baboons, and evaluate whether temperament mediates the relationship between early life adversity and sociality. We use behavioural data on 31 females to quantify sociality. We measure interaction style as the tendency to produce grunts (signals of benign intent) in contexts in which the vocalization does not produce immediate benefits to the actor. Early life adversity was negatively correlated with overall sociality, but was a stronger predictor of social behaviours received than behaviours initiated. Females who experienced less early life adversity had more benign interaction styles and benign interaction styles were associated with receiving more social behaviours. Interaction style may partially mediate the association between early life adversity and sociality. These analyses add to our growing understanding of the processes connecting early life experiences to adult sociality.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Social , Estrés Psicológico , Animales , Femenino , Papio anubis , Comunicación Animal
2.
Evol Anthropol ; 31(5): 245-262, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35289027

RESUMEN

Natural selection will favor male care when males have limited alternative mating opportunities, can invest in their own offspring, and when care enhances males' fitness. These conditions are easiest to fulfill in pair-bonded species, but neither male care nor stable "breeding bonds" that facilitate it are limited to pair-bonded species. We review evidence of paternal care and extended breeding bonds in owl monkeys, baboons, Assamese macaques, mountain gorillas, and chimpanzees. The data, which span social/mating systems and ecologies, suggest that there are multiple pathways by which conditions conducive to male care can arise. This diversity highlights the difficulty of making inferences about the emergence of male care in early hominins based on single traits visible in the fossil record. We discuss what types of data are most needed and the questions yet to be answered about the evolution of male care and extended breeding bonds in the primate order.


Asunto(s)
Primates , Conducta Sexual Animal , Animales , Masculino , Aotidae , Reproducción , Gorilla gorilla , Papio
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 287(1925): 20192794, 2020 04 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32315587

RESUMEN

Human cooperation is probably supported by our tendency to punish selfishness in others. Social norms play an important role in motivating third-party punishment (TPP), and also in explaining societal differences in prosocial behaviour. However, there has been little work directly linking social norms to the development of TPP across societies. In this study, we explored the impact of normative information on the development of TPP in 603 children aged 4-14, across six diverse societies. Children began to perform TPP during middle childhood, and the developmental trajectories of this behaviour were similar across societies. We also found that social norms began to influence the likelihood of performing TPP during middle childhood in some of these societies. Norms specifying the punishment of selfishness were generally more influential than norms specifying the punishment of prosocial behaviour. These findings support the view that TPP of selfishness is important in all societies, and its development is shaped by a shared psychology for responding to normative information. Yet, the results also highlight the important role that children's prior knowledge of local norms may play in explaining societal variation in the development of both TPP and prosociality.


Asunto(s)
Diversidad Cultural , Normas Sociales , Adolescente , Altruismo , Niño , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Motivación , Probabilidad , Castigo/psicología
4.
J Hum Evol ; 127: 81-92, 2019 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30777360

RESUMEN

Long-term male-female bonds and bi-parental investment in offspring are hallmarks of human society. A key question is how these traits evolved from the polygynandrously mating multimale multifemale society that likely characterized the Pan-Homo ancestor. In all three species of savanna baboons, lactating females form strong ties (sometimes called "friendships") with one or more adult males. For yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) and chacma baboons (Papio ursinus), several lines of evidence suggest that these relationships are a form of male parenting effort. In olive baboons (Papio anubis), females are thought to preferentially mate with their "friends", and male-female bonds may thus function as a form of mating effort. Here, we draw on behavioral and genetic data to evaluate the factors that shape male-female relationships in a well-studied population of olive baboons. We find support for the parenting effort hypothesis in that sires have stronger bonds with their infants' mothers than do other males. These bonds sometimes persist past weaning age and, in many cases, the sire of the previous infant is still a close partner of the female when she nurses her subsequent offspring. We find that males who have the strongest bonds with females that have resumed cycling, but are not currently sexually receptive, are more likely to sire the female's next offspring but the estimate is associated with large statistical uncertainty. We also find that in over one third of the cases, a female's successive infants were sired by the same male. Thus, in olive baboons, the development of stable breeding bonds and paternal investment seem to be grounded in the formation of close ties between males and anestrous females. However, other factors such as male dominance rank also influence paternity success and may preclude stability of these bonds to the extent found in human societies.


Asunto(s)
Papio anubis/psicología , Responsabilidad Parental , Conducta Sexual Animal , Animales , Femenino , Masculino
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(18): 8644-8645, 2019 04 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30910986
7.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 110(36): 14586-91, 2013 Sep 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23959869

RESUMEN

Humans are an exceptionally cooperative species, but there is substantial variation in the extent of cooperation across societies. Understanding the sources of this variability may provide insights about the forces that sustain cooperation. We examined the ontogeny of prosocial behavior by studying 326 children 3-14 y of age and 120 adults from six societies (age distributions varied across societies). These six societies span a wide range of extant human variation in culture, geography, and subsistence strategies, including foragers, herders, horticulturalists, and urban dwellers across the Americas, Oceania, and Africa. When delivering benefits to others was personally costly, rates of prosocial behavior dropped across all six societies as children approached middle childhood and then rates of prosociality diverged as children tracked toward the behavior of adults in their own societies. When prosocial acts did not require personal sacrifice, prosocial responses increased steadily as children matured with little variation in behavior across societies. Our results are consistent with theories emphasizing the importance of acquired cultural norms in shaping costly forms of cooperation and creating cross-cultural diversity.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Diversidad Cultural , Relaciones Interpersonales , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Australia , República Centroafricana , Niño , Preescolar , Ecuador , Femenino , Fiji , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Namibia , Estados Unidos
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 109(42): 16980-5, 2012 Oct 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23027933

RESUMEN

Studies of personality in nonhuman primates have usually relied on assessments by humans and seldom considered the function of the resulting "trait" classifications. In contrast, we applied exploratory principal component analysis to seven behaviors among 45 wild female baboons over 7 y to determine whether the personality dimensions that emerged were associated with measures of reproductive success. We identified three relatively stable personality dimensions, each characterized by a distinct suite of behaviors that were not redundant with dominance rank or the availability of kin. Females scoring high on the "Nice" dimension were friendly to all females and often grunted to lower-ranking females to signal benign intent. "Aloof" females were aggressive, less friendly, and grunted primarily to higher-ranking females. "Loner" females were often alone, relatively unfriendly, and also grunted most often to higher-ranking females. Aloof and Loner females were rarely approached by others. Personality dimensions were correlated in different ways with three measures previously shown to be associated with fitness: stress levels and two behavioral indices reflecting the closeness of dyadic bonds formed by individuals. Females who scored high on Nice had high composite sociality indices (CSI) and stable partner preferences, whereas females who scored high on Aloof had lower CSI scores but significantly more stable partner preferences. Loner females had significantly lower CSI scores, less stable partner preferences, and significantly higher glucocorticoid levels. It remains to be determined which of the Nice or Aloof personality dimensions is more adaptive, or whether variation is maintained by contrasting effects on fitness.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Papio/fisiología , Personalidad/fisiología , Aptitud Física/fisiología , Conducta Social , Animales , Botswana , Heces/química , Femenino , Hormonas Esteroides Gonadales/análisis , Individualidad , Observación , Análisis de Componente Principal
10.
Evol Anthropol ; 28(2): 50-51, 2019 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30889306
11.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108 Suppl 2: 10910-7, 2011 Jun 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21690372

RESUMEN

A growing body of evidence shows that humans are remarkably altruistic primates. Food sharing and division of labor play an important role in all human societies, and cooperation extends beyond the bounds of close kinship and networks of reciprocating partners. In humans, altruism is motivated at least in part by empathy and concern for the welfare of others. Although altruistic behavior is well-documented in other primates, the range of altruistic behaviors in other primate species, including the great apes, is much more limited than it is in humans. Moreover, when altruism does occur among other primates, it is typically limited to familiar group members--close kin, mates, and reciprocating partners. This suggests that there may be fundamental differences in the social preferences that motivate altruism across the primate order, and there is currently considerable interest in how we came to be such unusual apes. A body of experimental studies designed to examine the phylogenetic range of prosocial sentiments and behavior is beginning to shed some light on this issue. In experimental settings, chimpanzees and tamarins do not consistently take advantage of opportunities to deliver food rewards to others, although capuchins and marmosets do deliver food rewards to others in similar kinds of tasks. Although chimpanzees do not satisfy experimental criteria for prosociality in food delivery tasks, they help others complete tasks to obtain a goal. Differences in performance across species and differences in performance across tasks are not yet fully understood and raise new questions for further study.


Asunto(s)
Altruismo , Evolución Biológica , Adaptación Fisiológica , Animales , Conducta Cooperativa , Humanos , Pan troglodytes , Medio Social
12.
Curr Biol ; 34(9): R353-R355, 2024 05 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714164

RESUMEN

A new paper shows that rates of aggression are higher, and rates of coalition formation are lower, among male bonobos than among male chimpanzees. These findings are noteworthy because they challenge the view that female bonobos' preferences for less aggressive males favored a reduction in male aggression and an increase in social tolerance.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Conducta Animal , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan paniscus/psicología , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Conducta Social
13.
Evol Anthropol ; 22(5): 213-25, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24166922

RESUMEN

Behavioral ecologists have devoted considerable effort to identifying the sources of variation in individual reproductive success. Much of this work has focused on the characteristics of individuals, such as their sex and rank. However, many animals live in stable social groups and the fitness of individuals depends at least in part on the outcome of their interactions with other group members. For example, in many primate species, high dominance rank enhances access to resources and reproductive success. The ability to acquire and maintain high rank often depends on the availability and effectiveness of coalitionary support. Allies may be cultivated and coalitions may be reinforced by affiliative interactions such as grooming, food sharing, and tolerance. These findings suggest that if we want to understand the selective pressures that shape the social behavior of primates, it will be profitable to broaden our focus from the characteristics of individuals to the properties of the relationships that they form with others. The goal of this paper is to discuss a set of methods that can be used to quantify the properties of social relationships.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Ciencias de la Conducta/métodos , Modelos Estadísticos , Proyectos de Investigación , Conducta Social , Animales , Heces/química , Femenino , Hormonas/análisis , Masculino
14.
Science ; 382(6672): 760-761, 2023 11 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37972176

RESUMEN

Bonobos provide insight into the origins of partner-specific cooperation in human groups.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Conducta de Masa , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Humanos , Pan paniscus/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Aseo Animal
15.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 378(1868): 20210439, 2023 01 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36440561

RESUMEN

Human cooperation varies both across and within societies, and developmental studies can inform our understanding of the sources of both kinds of variation. One key candidate for explaining within-society variation in cooperative behaviour is gender, but we know little about whether gender differences in cooperation take root early in ontogeny or emerge similarly across diverse societies. Here, we explore two existing cross-cultural datasets of 4- to 15-year-old children's preferences for equality in experimental tasks measuring prosociality (14 societies) and fairness (seven societies), and we look for evidence of (i) widespread gender differences in the development of cooperation, and (ii) substantial societal variation in gender differences. This cross-cultural approach is crucial for revealing universal human gender differences in the development of cooperation, and it helps answer recent calls for greater cultural diversity in the study of human development. We find that gender has little impact on the development of prosociality and fairness within these datasets, and we do not find much evidence for substantial societal variation in gender differences. We discuss the implications of these findings for our knowledge about the nature and origin of gender differences in cooperation, and for future research attempting to study human development using diverse cultural samples. This article is part of the theme issue 'Cooperation among women: evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives'.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Diversidad Cultural , Niño , Humanos , Femenino , Preescolar , Adolescente , Factores Sexuales , Evolución Biológica
16.
iScience ; 26(7): 106991, 2023 Jul 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37534148

RESUMEN

Reproductive strategies are defined by expenditures of time and energy devoted to mating effort, which increases mating opportunities, and parenting effort, which enhances the survival of offspring. We examine tradeoffs between mating effort and parenting effort in male olive baboons, Papio anubis, a species in which males compete for mating opportunities, but also form ties to lactating females (primary associations) that represent a form of parenting effort. Males that are involved in more primary associations invest less in mating effort than males who are involved in fewer primary associations. Males that are involved in more primary associations play a smaller role in establishing proximity to their primary associates than other males, suggesting that males operate under temporal constraints. There is also some evidence that involvement in primary associations negatively affects paternity success. Taken together, the data suggest that males face tradeoffs between mating effort and parenting effort.

17.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 378(1868): 20210426, 2023 01 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36440559

RESUMEN

In group-living species, cooperative tactics can offset asymmetries in resource-holding potential between individuals and alter the outcome of intragroup conflicts. Differences in the kinds of competitive pressures that males and females face might influence the benefits they gain from forming intragroup coalitions. We predicted that there would be a female bias in intragroup coalitions because females (1) are more like to live with kin than males are, and (2) compete over resources that are more readily shared than resources males compete over. We tested this main prediction using information about coalition formation across mammalian species and phylogenetic comparative analyses. We found that for nearly all species in which intragroup coalitions occur, members of both sexes participate, making this the typical mammalian pattern. The presence and frequency of female or male coalitions were not strongly associated with key socio-ecological factors like resource defensibility, sexual dimorphism or philopatry. This suggests that once the ability to form intragroup coalitions emerges in one sex, it is likely to emerge in the other sex as well and that there is no strong phylogenetic legacy of sex differences in this form of cooperation. This article is part of the theme issue 'Cooperation among women: evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives'.


Asunto(s)
Mamíferos , Caracteres Sexuales , Animales , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino , Filogenia
18.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 378(1868): 20210424, 2023 01 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36440567

RESUMEN

A holistic, evolutionary framework about human cooperation must incorporate information about women's cooperative behaviour. Yet, most empirical research on human cooperation has centered on men's behaviour or been derived from experimental studies conducted in western, industrialized populations. These bodies of data are unlikely to accurately represent human behavioural diversity. To address this gap and provide a more balanced view of human cooperation, this issue presents substantial new data and multi-disciplinary perspectives to document the complexity of women's cooperative behaviour. Research in this issue 1) challenges narratives about universal gender differences in cooperation, 2) reconsiders patrilocality and access to kin as constraints on women's cooperation, 3) reviews evidence for a connection between social support and women's health and 4) examines the phylogenetic roots of female cooperation. Here, we discuss the steps taken in this issue toward a more complete and evidence-based understanding of the role that cooperation plays in women's and girls' lives and in building human sociality. This article is part of the theme issue 'Cooperation among women: evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives'.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Cooperativa , Conducta Social , Masculino , Humanos , Femenino , Filogenia , Apoyo Social , Factores Sexuales
19.
Nature ; 479(7372): 182-3, 2011 Nov 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22071758
20.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(51): 18106-7, 2014 Dec 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25512536
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA