Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 30
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
País/Región como asunto
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 231(4): 425-32, 2013 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24071925

RESUMEN

In prism adaptation experiments, the effect on throwing to a target is reduced (primary aftereffect is smaller) when the throwing condition with prisms removed (first test phase) is different from the throwing condition with prisms (the training phase). The missing adaptation, however, can be revealed through further testing (second test phase) in which the throwing condition during training is fully reinstated. We studied throwing underhand to a target flush with the floor. During training, participants wore left-shifting prism glasses while standing on the floor (Group 1) or on a balance board (Groups 2 and 3). Tests 1 and 2 following training involved the same underhand throwing. For Group 2, Test 1 was on the balance board and Test 2 on the ground; for Group 3, the order was reversed; and for Group 1, both tests were on the ground. The Group 3 Test 1 aftereffect was smaller, and the Test 2 aftereffect was larger than the respective tests for Groups 1 and 2, with the aftereffect sum the same for all three groups. A parallel was noted between prism adaptation and implicit memory: whether given training (study) conditions lead to better or poorer persistence of adaptation (memory performance) at test depends on the fit between the conditions at test relative to the conditions at training (study). In the general memory case, those conditions will involve nonobvious contributors to memory performance, analogous to the support for upright standing in the adaptation of the visual system to prismatic distortion investigated in the present research.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Efecto Tardío Figurativo/fisiología , Equilibrio Postural/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
2.
J Exp Biol ; 213(Pt 9): 1436-42, 2010 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20400627

RESUMEN

For some animals (e.g. the night-active wandering spider) the encounters with the habitat that result in place learning are predominantly mechanical. We asked whether place learning limited to mechanical contact, like place learning in general, entails vectors tied to individual landmarks and relations between landmarks. We constructed minimal environments for blindfolded human participants. Landmarks were raised steps. 'Home' was a mechanically indistinct location. Travel was linear. The mechanical contacts were those of walking, stepping, and probing with a soft-tipped cane. Home-orienting activities preceded tests of finding home from a given location with landmarks unchanged or (unbeknown to participants) shifted. In a one-landmark environment, perceived home shifted in the same direction, with the same magnitude, as the shifted landmark. In an environment of two landmarks located in the same direction from home, shifting the further landmark toward home resulted in a change in home's perceived location that preserved the original ratio of distances separating home, nearer landmark, and further landmark. Both findings were invariant over the travel route to the test location and repetitions of testing. It seems, therefore, that for humans (and, perhaps, for wandering spiders), mechanical contact can reveal the vectors and relations specifying places.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Percepción Espacial , Percepción del Tacto , Bastones , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Caminata
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 276(1677): 4309-14, 2009 Dec 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19740881

RESUMEN

In 1709, Berkeley hypothesized of the human that distance is measurable by 'the motion of his body, which is perceivable by touch'. To be sufficiently general and reliable, Berkeley's hypothesis must imply that distance measured by legged locomotion approximates actual distance, with the measure invariant to gait, speed and number of steps. We studied blindfolded human participants in a task in which they travelled by legged locomotion from a fixed starting point A to a variable terminus B, and then reproduced, by legged locomotion from B, the A-B distance. The outbound ('measure') and return ('report') gait could be the same or different, with similar or dissimilar step sizes and step frequencies. In five experiments we manipulated bipedal gait according to the primary versus secondary distinction revealed in symmetry group analyses of locomotion patterns. Berkeley's hypothesis held only when the measure and report gaits were of the same symmetry class, indicating that idiothetic distance measurement is gait-symmetry specific. Results suggest that human odometry (and perhaps animal odometry more generally) entails variables that encompass the limbs in coordination, such as global phase, and not variables at the level of the single limb, such as step length and step number, as traditionally assumed.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Marcha/fisiología , Locomoción/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Connecticut , Humanos , Adulto Joven
4.
Psychol Rev ; 116(2): 318-42, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19348544

RESUMEN

Trial-to-trial variation in word-pronunciation times exhibits 1/f scaling. One explanation is that human performances are consequent on multiplicative interactions among interdependent processes-interaction dominant dynamics. This article describes simulated distributions of pronunciation times in a further test for multiplicative interactions and interdependence. Individual participant distributions of approximately 1,100 word-pronunciation times were successfully mimicked for each participant in combinations of lognormal and power-law behavior. Successful hazard function simulations generalized these results to establish interaction dominant dynamics, in contrast with component dominant dynamics, as a likely mechanism for cognitive activity.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Dinámicas no Lineales , Tiempo de Reacción , Distribuciones Estadísticas , California , Fractales , Humanos , Modelos de Riesgos Proporcionales
5.
Adv Exp Med Biol ; 629: 93-123, 2009.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19227497

RESUMEN

Four perspectives on motor control provide the framework for developing a comprehensive theory of motor control in biological systems. The four perspectives, of decreasing orthodoxy, are distinguished by their sources of inspiration: neuroanatomy, robotics, self-organization, and ecological realities. Twelve major issues that commonly constrain (either explicitly or implicitly) the understanding of the control and coordination of movement are identified and evaluated within the framework of the four perspectives. The issues are as follows: (1) Is control strictly neural? (2) Is there a divide between planning and execution? (3) Does control entail a frequently involved knowledgeable executive? (4) Do analytical internal models mediate control? (5) Is anticipation necessarily model dependent? (6) Are movements preassembled? (7) Are the participating components context independent? (8) Is force transmission strictly myotendinous? (9) Is afference a matter of local linear signaling? (10) Is neural noise an impediment? (11) Do standard variables (of mechanics and physiology) suffice? (12) Is the organization of control hierarchical?


Asunto(s)
Modelos Biológicos , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Fractales , Humanos , Modelos Neurológicos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Transducción de Señal
6.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 81(7): 2330-2342, 2019 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31650520

RESUMEN

An animal's environment is rich with affordances. Different possible actions are specified by visual information while competing for dominance over neural dynamics. Affordance competition models account for this in terms of winner-takes-all cross-inhibition dynamics. Multistable phenomena also reveal how the visual system deals with ambiguity. Their key property is spontaneous instability, in forms such as alternating dominance in binocular rivalry. Theoretical models of self-inhibition or self-organized instability posit that the instability is tied to some kind of neural adaptation and that its functional significance is to enable flexible perceptual transitions. We hypothesized that the two perspectives are interlinked. Spontaneous instability is an intrinsic property of perceptual systems, but it is revealed when they are stripped from the constraints of possibilities for action. To test this, we compared a multistable gestalt phenomenon against its embodied version and estimated the neural adaptation and competition parameters of an affordance transition dynamic model. Wertheimer's (Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 61, 161-265, 1912) optimal (ß) and pure (φ) forms of apparent motion from a stroboscopic point-light display were endowed with action relevance by embedding the display in a visual object-tracking task. Thus, each mode was complemented by its action, because each perceptual mode uniquely enabled different ways of tracking the target. Perceptual judgment of the traditional apparent motion exhibited spontaneous instabilities, in the form of earlier switching when the frame rate was changed stepwise. In contrast, the embodied version exhibited hysteresis, consistent with affordance transition studies. Consistent with our predictions, the parameter for competition between modes in the affordance transition model increased, and the parameter for self-inhibition vanished.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Conducta Competitiva/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Femenino , Teoría Gestáltica , Humanos , Masculino , Visión Binocular/fisiología , Adulto Joven
7.
Biosystems ; 91(2): 320-30, 2008 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17709173

RESUMEN

This paper has two primary aims. The first is to provide an introductory discussion of hyperset theory and its usefulness for modeling complex systems. The second aim is to provide a hyperset analysis of several perspectives on autonomy: Robert Rosen's metabolism-repair systems and his claim that living things are closed to efficient cause, Maturana and Varela's autopoietic systems, and Kauffman's cataytically closed systems. Consequences of the hyperset models for Rosen's claim that autonomous systems have non-computable models are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Cognición/fisiología , Intención , Vida , Modelos Biológicos , Autonomía Personal , Volición/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Terminología como Asunto
8.
Hum Mov Sci ; 57: 111-133, 2018 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29202312

RESUMEN

Explanation of how goal-directed movements are made manifest is the ultimate aim of the field classically referred to as "motor control". Essential to the sought-after explanation is comprehension of the supporting functional architecture. Seven decades ago, the Russian physiologist and movement scientist Nikolai A. Bernstein proposed a hierarchical model to explain the construction of movements. In his model, the levels of the hierarchy share a common language (i.e., they are commensurate) and perform complementing functions to bring about dexterous movements. The science of the control and coordination of movement in the phylum Craniata has made considerable progress in the intervening seven decades. The contemporary body of knowledge about each of Bernstein's hypothesized functional levels is both more detailed and more sophisticated. A natural consequence of this progress, however, is the relatively independent theoretical development of a given level from the other levels. In this essay, we revisit each level of Bernstein's hierarchy from the joint perspectives of (a) the ecological approach to perception-action and (b) dynamical systems theory. We review a substantial and relevant body of literature produced in different areas of study that are accommodated by this ecological-dynamical version of Bernstein's levels. Implications for the control and coordination of movement and the challenges to producing a unified theory are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Invertebrados/fisiología , Movimiento , Teoría de Sistemas , Animales , Biología/historia , Ecología , Matriz Extracelular/fisiología , Fractales , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Historia del Siglo XX , Humanos , Destreza Motora , Miofibrillas/fisiología , Estrés Mecánico
9.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 43(5): 914-925, 2017 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28230398

RESUMEN

Two experiments are reported showing that behavior exhibited in manual tracking is consistent with behavior predicted by a dynamical systems phenomenon known as anticipating synchronization (Voss, 2000). They extend a prior investigation of the effect of delay on anticipatory manual tracking (Stepp, 2009) by also manipulating coupling strength. The coupling scheme in Experiment 1 and that in Experiment 2 go beyond the single delayed feedback coupling used in previous research and articulations of anticipating synchronization. These advanced coupling arrangements are addressed using an extended formulation which allows for multiple feedback delays, a continuous range of delay, or even coupling to real future values. The latter case is specifically investigated in Experiment 2, which utilizes a navigation task that provides a natural way to speak about coupling to future values. (PsycINFO Database Record


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Retroalimentación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
10.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 24(5): 1597-1603, 2017 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28188562

RESUMEN

The sciences of development and learning have been slow to acknowledge that absence of an identifiable experience that relates straightforwardly to a given perception-action ability need not mean that experience per se is irrelevant to the emergence of that ability. A recent study reveals that a difference in diet (plain vs. energy rich) leads to a difference in how rats navigate (use of geometry vs. use of features, respectively). It is a good example of how a seemingly unrelated experience (e.g., what the rats eat) can be a non-obvious yet crucial determiner of perception-action modes. We situate this finding in the broader context of the related conceptions of Schneirla's and Lehrman's Developmental Systems Theory, Gottlieb's Probabilistic Epigenesis, and Bolles's Structure of Learning (see article for references). In doing so we highlight that such phenomena may be the norm, both in development and learning, rather than the exception.


Asunto(s)
Crecimiento y Desarrollo/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Percepción/fisiología , Animales , Humanos
11.
Hum Mov Sci ; 54: 13-23, 2017 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28323219

RESUMEN

Perceptual guidance of movement with simple visual or temporal information can facilitate performance of difficult coordination patterns. Guidance may override coordination constraints that usually limit stability of bimanual coordination to only in-phase and anti-phase. Movement dynamics, however, might not have the same characteristics with and without perceptual guidance. Do visual and auditory guidance produce qualitatively different dynamical organization of movement? An anti-phase wrist flexion and extension coordination task was performed under no specific perceptual guidance, under temporal guidance with a metronome, and under visual guidance with a Lissajous plot. For the time series of amplitudes, periods and relative phases, temporal correlations were measured with Detrended Fluctuation Analysis and complexity levels were measured with multiscale entropy. Temporal correlations of amplitudes and relative phases deviated from the typical 1/f variation towards more random variation under visual guidance. The same was observed for the series of periods under temporal guidance. Complexity levels for all time series were lower in visual guidance, but higher for periods under temporal guidance. Perceptual simplification of the task's goal may produce enhancement of performance, but it is accompanied by changes in the details of movement organization that may be relevant to explain dependence and poor retention after practice under guidance.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Desempeño Psicomotor , Visión Ocular , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Voluntarios Sanos , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
12.
J Mot Behav ; 38(4): 251-64, 2006 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16801318

RESUMEN

The authors manipulated the circumstances in which individuals are typically embedded when standing upright by manipulating the intensity of light and the stationary structure of the environment. They expected that the manipulations would affect 12 older participants (aged 65-82 years) more than it would 12 younger participants (aged 22-24 years). Linear (e.g., total path length) and nonlinear (e.g., maximum line length of recurrent points in phase space) measures of the center of pressure time series confirmed that expectation. Moreover, for some measures, there was a suggestion that participants' visual contrast sensitivity (an index of neurophysiological age) was a more important contributing factor overall than was their chronological age. In the Discussion, the authors highlight the significance of interactive effects of environmental, organismic, and task constraints on quiet standing.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Ambiente , Iluminación , Postura/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Algoritmos , Humanos , Modelos Biológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Valores de Referencia
13.
Psychol Rev ; 123(3): 305-23, 2016 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26881694

RESUMEN

Behavioral dynamics is a framework for understanding adaptive behavior as arising from the self-organizing interaction between animal and environment. The methods of nonlinear dynamics provide a language for describing behavior that is both stable and flexible. Behavioral dynamics has been criticized for ignoring the animal's sensitivity to its own capabilities, leading to the development of an alternative framework: affordance-based control. Although it is theoretically sound and empirically motivated, affordance-based control has resisted characterization in terms of nonlinear dynamics. Here, we provide a dynamical description of affordance-based control, extending behavioral dynamics to meet its criticisms. We propose a general modeling strategy consistent with both theories. We use visually guided braking as a representative behavior and construct a novel dynamical model. This model demonstrates the possibility of understanding visually guided action as respecting the limits of the actor's capabilities, while still being guided by informational variables associated with desired states of affairs. In addition to such "hard" constraints on behavior, our framework allows for the influence of "soft" constraints such as preference and comfort, opening a new area of inquiry in perception-action dynamics.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal , Modelos Teóricos , Animales
14.
Vision Res ; 125: 1-11, 2016 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27210039

RESUMEN

The historical but questionable size-distance invariance hypothesis (SDIH) features computation over geometric, oculomotor, and binocular cues and the coupling of percepts-perceived size, S', is mediated by perceived distance, D'. A contemporary non-mediational hypothesis holds that S' and D' are specific to distinct optical variables. We report two experiments with an optical tunnel, an arrangement of alternating black and white concentric rings, that allows systematic manipulation of the optic array at a point of observation while controlling a variety of size and depth cues. Participants viewed targets of different sizes at different distances monocularly, reporting S' and D' via magnitude production. In Experiment 1, the target was either placed in a continuous tunnel (extending 164cm) or in a tunnel that truncated at the target's location. Experiment 2 included a third tunnel, one that was truncated with a flat depiction of the posterior surface structure that would have been visible in the continuous tunnel. In both experiments, S' decreased with D but D' was unaffected by S. Partial correlation analyses showed that the relationship between S' and D' was not significant when the contributions of other variables were removed. Importantly, S' and D' were affected differently by manipulations of the optical tunnel's continuity while computationally obvious visual cues were controlled. These outcomes suggest that D' is not a mediator of S'. Rather S' and D' are independently determined with correlated but different optical bases, results that support the direct model.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Distancia/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Adolescente , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos , Adulto Joven
15.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 134(1): 117-23, 2005 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15702967

RESUMEN

Ubiquitous 1/f scaling in human cognition and physiology suggests a mind-body interaction that contradicts commonly held assumptions. The intrinsic dynamics of psychological phenomena are interaction dominant (rather than component dominant), and the origin of purposive behavior lies with a general principle of self-organization (rather than a special neurocognitive mechanism). E.-J. Wagenmakers, S. Farrell, and R. Ratcliff (2005) raised concerns about the kinds of data and analyses that support generic 1/f scaling. This reply is a defense that furthermore questions the model that Wagenmakers and colleagues endorse and their strategy for addressing complexity.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Psicofisiología , Humanos , Teoría Psicológica , Autoimagen
16.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 31(5): 980-90, 2005 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16248746

RESUMEN

In 2 experiments, bimanual 1:1 rhythmic coordination was performed concurrently with encoding or retrieval of word lists. Effects of divided attention (DA) on coordination were indexed by changes in mean relative phase and recurrence measures of shared activity between the 2 limbs. Effects of DA on memory were indexed by deficits in recall relative to baseline. For DA at both encoding and retrieval, the equilibrium values of relative phase were shifted and the degree of shared activity between left and right rhythmic motions was reduced. Recall was reduced, however, only for DA at encoding. The results corroborate and extend those obtained with more conventional secondary tasks (e.g., visual reaction time), suggesting attention dissimilarities between episodic encoding and retrieval.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Mano/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Periodicidad , Desempeño Psicomotor , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental , Tiempo de Reacción
17.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 132(3): 331-50, 2003 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-13678372

RESUMEN

Background noise is the irregular variation across repeated measurements of human performance. Background noise remains after task and treatment effects are minimized. Background noise refers to intrinsic sources of variability, the intrinsic dynamics of mind and body, and the internal workings of a living being. Two experiments demonstrate 1/f scaling (pink noise) in simple reaction times and speeded word naming times, which round out a catalog of laboratory task demonstrations that background noise is pink noise. Ubiquitous pink noise suggests processes of mind and body that change each other's dynamics. Such interaction-dominant dynamics are found in systems that self-organize their behavior. Self-organization provides an unconventional perspective on cognition, but this perspective closely parallels a contemporary interdisciplinary view of living systems.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto , Fractales , Humanos , Intención , Tiempo de Reacción , Estadística como Asunto , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Conducta Verbal
19.
J Mot Behav ; 36(4): 371-2, 402-7; discussion 408-17, 2004 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15695215

RESUMEN

F. Mechsner (2004) bases his argument for the primacy of perception on a simplified interpretation of phase transition findings. The authors show that attention to the details of phase transition analysis, as well as consideration of findings from steady-state experiments and the tools of symmetry theory, necessitate a theory of bimanual coordination that includes both perceptual and motor processes.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Espacial , Humanos , Movimiento , Teoría Psicológica
20.
J Sport Exerc Psychol ; 16(s1): S128-S153, 1994 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29513128

RESUMEN

Recent developments in the study of action and perception have their roots in the contemplations of Giovanni Borelli, a 17th-century Italian mathematician and physicist, and Sir Charles Bell, and 18th-century English physiologist and neuroanatomist. When Borelli looked at muscle and its functional achievements, he saw dynamics with its attendant laws and principles; when Bell looked at muscle, he saw information about muscular states and a smart mechanism for its measurement. Research and theory on the dynamics of coordination and locomotion, and on the perceptual achievements of the haptic subsystem of dynamic touch, are providing affirmation of these visions of Borelli and Bell.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA