Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 35
Filtrar
Más filtros

Banco de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(1): 328-333, 2019 01 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30559179

RESUMEN

As a part of social cognition, people automatically construct rich models of other people's vision. Here we show that when people judge the mechanical forces acting on an object, their judgments are biased by another person gazing at the object. The bias is consistent with an implicit perception that gaze adds a gentle force, pushing on the object. The bias was present even though the participants were not explicitly aware of it and claimed that they did not believe in an extramission view of vision (a common folk view of vision in which the eyes emit an invisible energy). A similar result was not obtained on control trials when participants saw a blindfolded face turned toward the object, or a face with open eyes turned away from the object. The findings suggest that people automatically and implicitly generate a model of other people's vision that uses the simplifying construct of beams coming out of the eyes. This implicit model of active gaze may be a hidden, yet fundamental, part of the rich process of social cognition, contributing to how we perceive visual agency. It may also help explain the extraordinary cultural persistence of the extramission myth of vision.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Ojo , Percepción Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Movimientos Oculares , Femenino , Fijación Ocular , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Modelos Psicológicos , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Teoría de la Mente , Visión Ocular , Adulto Joven
2.
Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol ; 28(4): 493-502, 2022 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34291982

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: The Ethnic Identity Scale (EIS) was developed to distinguish between process and content components of ethnic-racial identity (ERI). However, the affirmation subscale is composed entirely of negatively worded items, measuring negative feelings about one's ethnic-racial group, rather than positive feelings as widely conceptualized. Addressing this gap, the present study examined the psychometric validity of a revised EIS with positively and negatively worded items to determine whether affirmation is best represented as a unidimensional construct, a bidimensional construct, or a combination of the two. METHOD: The sample consisted of 280 college students (75.5% female; Mage = 20.95 years; SD = 1.98 years). The largest ethnic-racial group consisted of Black or African Americans (68.2%), followed by Asian/Asian Americans (12.1%), Hispanic/Latinos (9.6%), and other ethnic-racial groups (10%). RESULTS: Confirmatory factor analysis provided evidence for both unidimensionality and multidimensionality. Indeed, although positively worded and negatively worded items of "affirmation" loaded onto a general factor representing affirmation, there was still a significant amount of variance captured by the negative ERI affect specific factor, indicating the presence of multidimensionality. In addition, results indicated that negative ERI affect, over and above the general ERI affirmation factor, predicted psychosocial functioning. CONCLUSIONS: The present study expands our understanding of the multidimensionality of ERI, highlighting the need for examination of how we measure ERI affect at the very least, and possibly how we conceptualize it within the broader ERI literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Etnicidad , Identificación Social , Femenino , Humanos , Adulto Joven , Adulto , Masculino , Negro o Afroamericano/psicología , Grupos Raciales , Asiático
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(7): E1684-E1689, 2018 02 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29339513

RESUMEN

Many people show a left-right bias in visual processing. We measured spatial bias in neurotypical participants using a variant of the line bisection task. In the same participants, we measured performance in a social cognition task. This theory-of-mind task measured whether each participant had a processing-speed bias toward the right of, or left of, a cartoon agent about which the participant was thinking. Crucially, the cartoon was rotated such that what was left and right with respect to the cartoon was up and down with respect to the participant. Thus, a person's own left-right bias could not align directly onto left and right with respect to the cartoon head. Performance on the two tasks was significantly correlated. People who had a natural bias toward processing their own left side of space were quicker to process how the cartoon might think about objects to the left side of its face, and likewise for a rightward bias. One possible interpretation of these results is that the act of processing one's own personal space shares some of the same underlying mechanisms as the social cognitive act of reconstructing someone else's processing of their space.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Espacial , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Sesgo , Cognición , Femenino , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
4.
Mil Psychol ; 32(6): 408-416, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38536242

RESUMEN

Given over 2.77 million US service members have been deployed in the past 20 years and the intense process of reintegration to civilian life, understanding factors that contribute to Veterans' mental health and substance use is critical. This study sought to understand the effects of US identity exploration, US identity commitment, US identity affirmation, and US identity centrality on substance use and symptoms of depression and anxiety. The sample consisted of 195 US military Veterans (n = 184, 53.3% women; 73.3% White; Mage = 35.12 years, SD = 9.60 years). Bivariate correlations indicated US identity affirmation was negatively associated with substance use and symptoms of depression and anxiety whereas US identity centrality was positively correlated with alcohol use. Utilizing structural equation model, US identity affirmation and US identity centrality were, respectively, negatively and positively associated with alcohol use, substance use, and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Partially consistent with our hypothesis, US identity exploration was positively associated with symptoms of anxiety. In contrast to our hypothesis, US identity commitment was not significantly associated with any outcome. Results are discussed in terms of important directions for identity research in the transition to civilian life.

5.
J Magn Reson Imaging ; 50(5): 1526-1533, 2019 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30950554

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Thermal dosimetry during MR-guided focused ultrasound (MRgFUS) of bone tumors underpredicts ablation zone. Intraprocedural understanding of heat accumulation near bone is needed to prevent undesired treatment of nontargeted tissue. HYPOTHESIS: Temperature decay rates predict prolonged, spatially varying heating during MRgFUS bone treatments. STUDY TYPE: Prospective case series. PATIENTS: Nine patients with localized painful bone tumors (five bone metastasis, four osteoid osteomas), were compared with five patients with uterine fibroid tumors treated using MRgFUS. FIELD STRENGTH/SEQUENCE: Proton resonance frequency shift thermometry using 2D-GRE with echo-planar imaging at 3 T. ASSESSMENT: Tissue response was derived by fitting data from extended thermometry acquisitions to a decay model. Decay rates and time to peak temperature (TTP) were analyzed in segmented zones between the bone target and skin. Decay rates were used to calculate intersonication cooling times required to return to body temperature; these were compared against conventional system-mandated cooling times. STATISTICAL TESTS: Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests for normality, and Student's t-test was used to compare decay rates. Spatial TTP delay and predicted cooling times used Wilcoxon signed rank tests. P < 0.05 was significant. RESULTS: Tissue decay rates in bone tumor patients were 3.5 times slower than those in patients with fibroids (τbone = 0.037 ± 0.012 vs. τfibroid = 0.131 ± 0.010, P < 0.05). Spatial analysis showed slow decay rates effecting baseline temperature as far as 12 mm away from the bone surface, τ4 = 0.015 ± 0.026 (median ± interquartile range [IQR]). Tissue within 9 mm of bone experienced delayed TTP (P < 0.01). In the majority of bone tumor treatments, system-predicted intersonication cooling times were insufficient for nearby tissue to return to body temperature (P = 0.03 in zone 4). DATA CONCLUSION: MRgFUS near bone is susceptible to long tissue decay rates, and unwanted cumulative heating up to 1.2 cm from the surface of the bone. Knowledge of decay rates may be used to alter treatment planning and intraprocedural thermal monitoring protocols to account for prolonged heating by bone. LEVEL OF EVIDENCE: 4 Technical Efficacy: Stage 4 J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 2019;50:1526-1533.


Asunto(s)
Neoplasias Óseas/diagnóstico por imagen , Neoplasias Óseas/terapia , Calor , Osteoma Osteoide/diagnóstico por imagen , Osteoma Osteoide/terapia , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Huesos/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Leiomioma , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Metástasis de la Neoplasia , Estudios Prospectivos , Termometría , Terapia por Ultrasonido , Neoplasias Uterinas , Adulto Joven
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 113(48): 13923-13928, 2016 11 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27849616

RESUMEN

It is now well established that visual attention, as measured with standard spatial attention tasks, and visual awareness, as measured by report, can be dissociated. It is possible to attend to a stimulus with no reported awareness of the stimulus. We used a behavioral paradigm in which people were aware of a stimulus in one condition and unaware of it in another condition, but the stimulus drew a similar amount of spatial attention in both conditions. The paradigm allowed us to test for brain regions active in association with awareness independent of level of attention. Participants performed the task in an MRI scanner. We looked for brain regions that were more active in the aware than the unaware trials. The largest cluster of activity was obtained in the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) bilaterally. Local independent component analysis (ICA) revealed that this activity contained three distinct, but overlapping, components: a bilateral, anterior component; a left dorsal component; and a right dorsal component. These components had brain-wide functional connectivity that partially overlapped the ventral attention network and the frontoparietal control network. In contrast, no significant activity in association with awareness was found in the banks of the intraparietal sulcus, a region connected to the dorsal attention network and traditionally associated with attention control. These results show the importance of separating awareness and attention when testing for cortical substrates. They are also consistent with a recent proposal that awareness is associated with ventral attention areas, especially in the TPJ.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Concienciación/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lóbulo Parietal/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología
8.
Cereb Cortex ; 27(4): 2617-2627, 2017 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27073219

RESUMEN

The neural basis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is not yet understood. ASD is marked by social deficits and is strongly associated with cerebellar abnormalities. We studied the organization and cerebellar connectivity of the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), an area that plays a crucial role in social cognition. We applied localized independent component analysis to resting-state fMRI data from autistic and neurotypical adolescents to yield an unbiased parcellation of the bilateral TPJ into 11 independent components (ICs). A comparison between neurotypical and autistic adolescents showed that the organization of the TPJ was not significantly altered in ASD. Second, we used the time courses of the TPJ ICs as spatially unbiased "seeds" for a functional connectivity analysis applied to voxels within the cerebellum. We found that the cerebellum contained a fine-grained, lateralized map of the TPJ. The connectivity of the TPJ subdivisions with cerebellar zones showed one striking difference in ASD. The right dorsal TPJ showed markedly less connectivity with the left Crus II. Disturbed cerebellar input to this key region for cognition and multimodal integration may contribute to social deficits in ASD. The findings might also suggest that the right TPJ and/or left Crus II are potential targets for noninvasive brain stimulation therapies.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista/patología , Cerebelo/patología , Vías Nerviosas/patología , Lóbulo Parietal/patología , Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Adolescente , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Interpretación de Imagen Asistida por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino
9.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(13): 5012-7, 2014 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24639542

RESUMEN

This study tested the possible relationship between reported visual awareness ("I see a visual stimulus in front of me") and the social attribution of awareness to someone else ("That person is aware of an object next to him"). Subjects were tested in two steps. First, in an fMRI experiment, subjects were asked to attribute states of awareness to a cartoon face. Activity associated with this task was found bilaterally within the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) among other areas. Second, the TPJ was transiently disrupted using single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). When the TMS was targeted to the same cortical sites that had become active during the social attribution task, the subjects showed symptoms of visual neglect in that their detection of visual stimuli was significantly affected. In control trials, when TMS was targeted to nearby cortical sites that had not become active during the social attribution task, no significant effect on visual detection was found. These results suggest that there may be at least some partial overlap in brain mechanisms that participate in the social attribution of sensory awareness to other people and in attributing sensory awareness to oneself.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Factores de Tiempo , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
10.
J Neurosci ; 35(25): 9432-45, 2015 Jun 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26109666

RESUMEN

The human temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is a topic of intense research. Imaging studies have identified TPJ activation in association with many higher-order functions such as theory-of-mind, episodic memory, and attention, causing debate about the distribution of different processes. One major challenge is the lack of consensus about the anatomical location and extent of the TPJ. Here, we address this problem using data-driven analysis to test the hypothesis that the bilateral TPJ can be parcellated into subregions. We applied independent component analysis (ICA) to task-free fMRI data within a local region around the bilateral TPJ, iterating the ICA at multiple model orders and in several datasets. The localized analysis allowed finer separation of processes and the use of multiple dimensionalities provided qualitative information about lateralization. We identified four subdivisions that were bilaterally symmetrical and one that was right biased. To test whether the independent components (ICs) reflected true subdivisions, we performed functional connectivity analysis using the IC coordinates as seeds. This confirmed that the subdivisions belonged to distinct networks. The right-biased IC was connected with a network often associated with attentional processing. One bilateral subdivision was connected to sensorimotor regions and another was connected to auditory regions. One subdivision that presented as distinct left- and right-biased ICs was connected to frontoparietal regions. Another subdivision that also had left- and right-biased ICs was connected to social or default mode networks. Our results show that the TPJ in both hemispheres hosts multiple neural processes with connectivity patterns consistent with well developed specialization and lateralization.


Asunto(s)
Lóbulo Parietal/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Vías Nerviosas/anatomía & histología , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Adulto Joven
11.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 28(6): 842-51, 2016 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26836517

RESUMEN

Previous studies show that it is possible to attend to a stimulus without awareness of it. Whether attention and awareness are independent or have a specific relationship, however, remains debated. Here, we tested three aspects of visual attention with and without awareness of the visual stimulus. Metacontrast masking rendered participants either subjectively aware or not aware of the stimulus. Attention drawn to the stimulus was measured by using the stimulus as a cue in a spatial attention task. We found that attention was drawn to the stimulus regardless of whether or not people were aware of it. However, attention changed significantly in the absence of awareness in at least three ways. First, attention to a task-relevant stimulus was less stable over time. Second, inhibition of return, the automatic suppression of attention to a task-irrelevant stimulus, was reduced. Third, attention was more driven by the luminance contrast of the stimulus. These findings add to the growing information on the behavior of attention with and without awareness. The findings are also consistent with our recently proposed account of the relationship between attention and awareness. In the attention schema theory, awareness is the internal model of attention. Just as the brain contains a body schema that models the body and helps control the body, so it contains an attention schema that helps control attention. In that theory, in the absence of awareness, the control of attention should suffer in basic ways predictable from dynamical systems theory. The present results confirm some of those predictions.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Concienciación , Función Ejecutiva , Percepción Visual , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Pruebas Psicológicas , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven
12.
J Magn Reson Imaging ; 43(1): 181-9, 2016 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26119129

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To investigate thermal dose volume (TDV) and non-perfused volume (NPV) of magnetic resonance-guided focused ultrasound (MRgFUS) treatments in patients with soft tissue tumors, and describe a method for MR thermal dosimetry using a baseline reference. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Agreement between TDV and immediate post treatment NPV was evaluated from MRgFUS treatments of five patients with biopsy-proven desmoid tumors. Thermometry data (gradient echo, 3T) were analyzed over the entire course of the treatments to discern temperature errors in the standard approach. The technique searches previously acquired baseline images for a match using 2D normalized cross-correlation and a weighted mean of phase difference images. Thermal dose maps and TDVs were recalculated using the matched baseline and compared to NPV. RESULTS: TDV and NPV showed between 47%-91% disagreement, using the standard immediate baseline method for calculating TDV. Long-term thermometry showed a nonlinear local temperature accrual, where peak additional temperature varied between 4-13°C (mean = 7.8°C) across patients. The prior baseline method could be implemented by finding a previously acquired matching baseline 61% ± 8% (mean ± SD) of the time. We found 7%-42% of the disagreement between TDV and NPV was due to errors in thermometry caused by heat accrual. For all patients, the prior baseline method increased the estimated treatment volume and reduced the discrepancies between TDV and NPV (P = 0.023). CONCLUSION: This study presents a mismatch between in-treatment and post treatment efficacy measures. The prior baseline approach accounts for local heating and improves the accuracy of thermal dose-predicted volume.


Asunto(s)
Fibromatosis Agresiva/diagnóstico , Fibromatosis Agresiva/cirugía , Ultrasonido Enfocado de Alta Intensidad de Ablación/métodos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Cirugía Asistida por Computador/métodos , Termografía/métodos , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Fibromatosis Agresiva/fisiopatología , Ondas de Choque de Alta Energía/uso terapéutico , Calor , Humanos , Aumento de la Imagen/métodos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Dosis de Radiación , Radiometría/métodos , Valores de Referencia , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Sensibilidad y Especificidad , Resultado del Tratamiento , Adulto Joven
13.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Mar 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37873134

RESUMEN

Many areas of science and medicine would benefit from selective release of drugs in specific regions of interest. Nanoparticle drug carriers activated by focused ultrasound-remotely applied, depth-penetrating energy-may provide such selective interventions. Here, we developed stable, ultrasound-responsive nanoparticles that can be used to release drugs effectively and safely in non-human primates. The nanoparticles were used to release propofol in deep brain visual regions. The release reversibly modulated the subjects' visual choice behavior and was specific to the targeted region and to the released drug. Gadolinium-enhanced MRI imaging suggested an intact blood-brain barrier. Blood draws showed normal clinical chemistry and hematology. In summary, this study provides a safe and effective approach to release drugs on demand in selected deep brain regions at levels sufficient to modulate behavior.

14.
J Control Release ; 369: 775-785, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38604386

RESUMEN

Many areas of science and medicine would benefit from selective release of drugs in specific regions. Nanoparticle drug carriers activated by focused ultrasound-remotely applied, depth-penetrating energy-may provide such selective interventions. Here, we developed stable, ultrasound-responsive nanoparticles that can be used to release drugs effectively and safely in non-human primates. The nanoparticles were used to release propofol in deep brain visual regions. The release reversibly modulated the subjects' visual choice behavior and was specific to the targeted region and to the released drug. Gadolinium-enhanced MR imaging suggested an intact blood-brain barrier. Blood draws showed normal clinical chemistry and hematology. In summary, this study provides a safe and effective approach to release drugs on demand in selected deep brain regions at levels sufficient to modulate behavior.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Preparaciones de Acción Retardada , Propofol , Animales , Propofol/farmacocinética , Propofol/administración & dosificación , Propofol/sangre , Propofol/química , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Nanopartículas/administración & dosificación , Masculino , Liberación de Fármacos , Macaca mulatta , Portadores de Fármacos/química , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Barrera Hematoencefálica/metabolismo , Sistemas de Liberación de Medicamentos , Gadolinio/administración & dosificación , Gadolinio/química , Gadolinio/farmacocinética
15.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 2024 May 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38729852

RESUMEN

A central challenge for cognitive science is to explain how abstract concepts are acquired from limited experience. This has often been framed in terms of a dichotomy between connectionist and symbolic cognitive models. Here, we highlight a recently emerging line of work that suggests a novel reconciliation of these approaches, by exploiting an inductive bias that we term the relational bottleneck. In that approach, neural networks are constrained via their architecture to focus on relations between perceptual inputs, rather than the attributes of individual inputs. We review a family of models that employ this approach to induce abstractions in a data-efficient manner, emphasizing their potential as candidate models for the acquisition of abstract concepts in the human mind and brain.

16.
Nat Hum Behav ; 7(9): 1526-1541, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37524930

RESUMEN

The recent advent of large language models has reinvigorated debate over whether human cognitive capacities might emerge in such generic models given sufficient training data. Of particular interest is the ability of these models to reason about novel problems zero-shot, without any direct training. In human cognition, this capacity is closely tied to an ability to reason by analogy. Here we performed a direct comparison between human reasoners and a large language model (the text-davinci-003 variant of Generative Pre-trained Transformer (GPT)-3) on a range of analogical tasks, including a non-visual matrix reasoning task based on the rule structure of Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices. We found that GPT-3 displayed a surprisingly strong capacity for abstract pattern induction, matching or even surpassing human capabilities in most settings; preliminary tests of GPT-4 indicated even better performance. Our results indicate that large language models such as GPT-3 have acquired an emergent ability to find zero-shot solutions to a broad range of analogy problems.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Solución de Problemas , Humanos , Lenguaje
17.
J Neural Eng ; 20(3)2023 06 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37236172

RESUMEN

Objective:The ability to generate electric fields in specific targets remotely would transform manipulations of processes that rest on electrical signaling.Approach:This article shows that focal electric fields are generated from distance by combining two orthogonal, remotely applied energies-magnetic and focused ultrasonic fields. The effect derives from the Lorentz force equation applied to magnetic and ultrasonic fields.Main results:We elicited this effect using standard hardware and confirmed that the generated electric fields align with the Lorentz equation. The effect significantly and safely modulated human peripheral nerves and deep brain regions of non-human primates.Significance:This approach opens a new set of applications in which electric fields are generated at high spatiotemporal resolution within intact biological tissues or materials, thus circumventing the limitations of traditional electrode-based procedures.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Ultrasonido , Animales , Encéfalo/fisiología , Estimulación Eléctrica
18.
Front Integr Neurosci ; 17: 1232523, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38093826

RESUMEN

Introduction: A general trend in the psychological literature suggests that guilt contributes to morality more than shame does. Unlike shame-prone individuals, guilt-prone individuals internalize the causality of negative events, attribute responsibility in the first person, and engage in responsible behavior. However, it is not known how guilt- and shame-proneness interact with the attribution of responsibility to others. Methods: In two Web-based experiments, participants reported their attributions of moral culpability (i.e., responsibility, causality, punishment and decision-making) about morally ambiguous acts of killing in different conditions. In Study 1 the vignettes were presented in the 1st person, while in Study 2 in the 3rd person. To test proneness to guilt and shame, we utilized the GASP scale, which differentiates between the affective and behavioral components of each emotion. Statistical analyses were performed in Matlab and R. Results: We found that guilt- and shame-proneness were associated with the severity of attributions in both the first and the third person, but the effect was strong only in the guilt case (both subtypes) and shame-affect case, and not in the shame-behavior case. We call this the Moralizing Effect. Discussion: We wonder whether our finding that guilt-prone people tend to attribute a higher degree of culpability to others is really consistent with the view that guilt motivates people to choose the "moral paths in life". This echoes views about the harmful aspects of guilt, which have been expressed historically in philosophy, for example, by Friedrich Nietzsche.

19.
Nat Commun ; 14(1): 3992, 2023 07 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37414780

RESUMEN

Previous work has sought to understand decision confidence as a prediction of the probability that a decision will be correct, leading to debate over whether these predictions are optimal, and whether they rely on the same decision variable as decisions themselves. This work has generally relied on idealized, low-dimensional models, necessitating strong assumptions about the representations over which confidence is computed. To address this, we used deep neural networks to develop a model of decision confidence that operates directly over high-dimensional, naturalistic stimuli. The model accounts for a number of puzzling dissociations between decisions and confidence, reveals a rational explanation of these dissociations in terms of optimization for the statistics of sensory inputs, and makes the surprising prediction that, despite these dissociations, decisions and confidence depend on a common decision variable.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Toma de Decisiones , Probabilidad , Sesgo
20.
Brain Stimul ; 16(3): 798-805, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37080427

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Transcranial focused ultrasound has the potential to noninvasively modulate deep brain circuits and impart sustained, neuroplastic effects. OBJECTIVE: Bring the approach closer to translations by demonstrating sustained modulation of deep brain circuits and choice behavior in task-performing non-human primates. METHODS: Low-intensity transcranial ultrasound of 30 s in duration was delivered in a controlled manner into deep brain targets (left or right lateral geniculate nucleus; LGN) of non-human primates while the subjects decided whether a left or a right visual target appeared first. While the animals performed the task, we recorded intracranial EEG from occipital screws. The ultrasound was delivered into the deep brain targets daily for a period of more than 6 months. RESULTS: The brief stimulation induced effects on choice behavior that persisted up to 15 minutes and were specific to the sonicated target. Stimulation of the left/right LGN increased the proportion of rightward/leftward choices. These effects were accompanied by an increase in gamma activity over visual cortex. The contralateral effect on choice behavior and the increase in gamma, compared to sham stimulation, suggest that the stimulation excited the target neural circuits. There were no detrimental effects on the animals' discrimination performance over the months-long course of the stimulation. CONCLUSION: This study demonstrates that brief, 30-s ultrasonic stimulation induces neuroplastic effects specifically in the target deep brain circuits, and that the stimulation can be applied daily without detrimental effects. These findings encourage repeated applications of transcranial ultrasound to malfunctioning deep brain circuits in humans with the goal of providing a durable therapeutic reset.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Ondas Ultrasónicas , Humanos , Animales , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Primates
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA