Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 1.179
Filtrar
Más filtros

Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Nature ; 618(7967): 1000-1005, 2023 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37258667

RESUMEN

A hallmark of human intelligence is the ability to plan multiple steps into the future1,2. Despite decades of research3-5, it is still debated whether skilled decision-makers plan more steps ahead than novices6-8. Traditionally, the study of expertise in planning has used board games such as chess, but the complexity of these games poses a barrier to quantitative estimates of planning depth. Conversely, common planning tasks in cognitive science often have a lower complexity9,10 and impose a ceiling for the depth to which any player can plan. Here we investigate expertise in a complex board game that offers ample opportunity for skilled players to plan deeply. We use model fitting methods to show that human behaviour can be captured using a computational cognitive model based on heuristic search. To validate this model, we predict human choices, response times and eye movements. We also perform a Turing test and a reconstruction experiment. Using the model, we find robust evidence for increased planning depth with expertise in both laboratory and large-scale mobile data. Experts memorize and reconstruct board features more accurately. Using complex tasks combined with precise behavioural modelling might expand our understanding of human planning and help to bridge the gap with progress in artificial intelligence.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Teoría del Juego , Juegos Experimentales , Inteligencia , Modelos Psicológicos , Humanos , Inteligencia Artificial , Cognición , Movimientos Oculares , Heurística , Memoria , Tiempo de Reacción , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
2.
Nature ; 606(7912): 129-136, 2022 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35589843

RESUMEN

One of the most striking features of human cognition is the ability to plan. Two aspects of human planning stand out-its efficiency and flexibility. Efficiency is especially impressive because plans must often be made in complex environments, and yet people successfully plan solutions to many everyday problems despite having limited cognitive resources1-3. Standard accounts in psychology, economics and artificial intelligence have suggested that human planning succeeds because people have a complete representation of a task and then use heuristics to plan future actions in that representation4-11. However, this approach generally assumes that task representations are fixed. Here we propose that task representations can be controlled and that such control provides opportunities to quickly simplify problems and more easily reason about them. We propose a computational account of this simplification process and, in a series of preregistered behavioural experiments, show that it is subject to online cognitive control12-14 and that people optimally balance the complexity of a task representation and its utility for planning and acting. These results demonstrate how strategically perceiving and conceiving problems facilitates the effective use of limited cognitive resources.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Función Ejecutiva , Eficiencia , Heurística , Humanos , Modelos Psicológicos
3.
Genome Res ; 33(7): 1175-1187, 2023 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36990779

RESUMEN

Seed-chain-extend with k-mer seeds is a powerful heuristic technique for sequence alignment used by modern sequence aligners. Although effective in practice for both runtime and accuracy, theoretical guarantees on the resulting alignment do not exist for seed-chain-extend. In this work, we give the first rigorous bounds for the efficacy of seed-chain-extend with k-mers in expectation Assume we are given a random nucleotide sequence of length ∼n that is indexed (or seeded) and a mutated substring of length ∼m ≤ n with mutation rate θ < 0.206. We prove that we can find a k = Θ(log n) for the k-mer size such that the expected runtime of seed-chain-extend under optimal linear-gap cost chaining and quadratic time gap extension is O(mn f (θ) log n), where f(θ) < 2.43 · θ holds as a loose bound. The alignment also turns out to be good; we prove that more than [Formula: see text] fraction of the homologous bases is recoverable under an optimal chain. We also show that our bounds work when k-mers are sketched, that is, only a subset of all k-mers is selected, and that sketching reduces chaining time without increasing alignment time or decreasing accuracy too much, justifying the effectiveness of sketching as a practical speedup in sequence alignment. We verify our results in simulation and on real noisy long-read data and show that our theoretical runtimes can predict real runtimes accurately. We conjecture that our bounds can be improved further, and in particular, f(θ) can be further reduced.


Asunto(s)
Algoritmos , Heurística , Simulación por Computador , Alineación de Secuencia , Análisis de Secuencia de ADN/métodos
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(11): e2214211120, 2023 03 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36881625

RESUMEN

During the biofilm life cycle, bacteria attach to a surface and then reproduce, forming crowded, growing communities. Many theoretical models of biofilm growth dynamics have been proposed; however, difficulties in accurately measuring biofilm height across relevant time and length scales have prevented testing these models, or their biophysical underpinnings, empirically. Using white light interferometry, we measure the heights of microbial colonies with nanometer precision from inoculation to their final equilibrium height, producing a detailed empirical characterization of vertical growth dynamics. We propose a heuristic model for vertical growth dynamics based on basic biophysical processes inside a biofilm: diffusion and consumption of nutrients and growth and decay of the colony. This model captures the vertical growth dynamics from short to long time scales (10 min to 14 d) of diverse microorganisms, including bacteria and fungi.


Asunto(s)
Biopelículas , Heurística , Biofisica , Difusión , Interferometría
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(11): e2208839120, 2023 03 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36881628

RESUMEN

Human communication is increasingly intermixed with language generated by AI. Across chat, email, and social media, AI systems suggest words, complete sentences, or produce entire conversations. AI-generated language is often not identified as such but presented as language written by humans, raising concerns about novel forms of deception and manipulation. Here, we study how humans discern whether verbal self-presentations, one of the most personal and consequential forms of language, were generated by AI. In six experiments, participants (N = 4,600) were unable to detect self-presentations generated by state-of-the-art AI language models in professional, hospitality, and dating contexts. A computational analysis of language features shows that human judgments of AI-generated language are hindered by intuitive but flawed heuristics such as associating first-person pronouns, use of contractions, or family topics with human-written language. We experimentally demonstrate that these heuristics make human judgment of AI-generated language predictable and manipulable, allowing AI systems to produce text perceived as "more human than human." We discuss solutions, such as AI accents, to reduce the deceptive potential of language generated by AI, limiting the subversion of human intuition.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Lenguaje , Humanos , Comunicación , Colina O-Acetiltransferasa , Inteligencia Artificial
6.
Bioinformatics ; 40(3)2024 Mar 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38265119

RESUMEN

MOTIVATION: Sequence alignment has been at the core of computational biology for half a century. Still, it is an open problem to design a practical algorithm for exact alignment of a pair of related sequences in linear-like time. RESULTS: We solve exact global pairwise alignment with respect to edit distance by using the A* shortest path algorithm. In order to efficiently align long sequences with high divergence, we extend the recently proposed seed heuristic with match chaining, gap costs, and inexact matches. We additionally integrate the novel match pruning technique and diagonal transition to improve the A* search. We prove the correctness of our algorithm, implement it in the A*PA aligner, and justify our extensions intuitively and empirically.On random sequences of divergence d=4% and length n, the empirical runtime of A*PA scales near-linearly with length (best fit n1.06, n≤107 bp). A similar scaling remains up to d=12% (best fit n1.24, n≤107 bp). For n=107 bp and d=4%, A*PA reaches >500× speedup compared to the leading exact aligners Edlib and BiWFA. The performance of A*PA is highly influenced by long gaps. On long (n>500kb) ONT reads of a human sample it efficiently aligns sequences with d<10%, leading to 3× median speedup compared to Edlib and BiWFA. When the sequences come from different human samples, A*PA performs 1.7× faster than Edlib and BiWFA. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION: github.com/RagnarGrootKoerkamp/astar-pairwise-aligner.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Programas Informáticos , Humanos , Análisis de Secuencia de ADN/métodos , Algoritmos , Semillas
7.
Mol Biol Evol ; 40(7)2023 07 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37395787

RESUMEN

Inference and interpretation of evolutionary processes, in particular of the types and targets of natural selection affecting coding sequences, are critically influenced by the assumptions built into statistical models and tests. If certain aspects of the substitution process (even when they are not of direct interest) are presumed absent or are modeled with too crude of a simplification, estimates of key model parameters can become biased, often systematically, and lead to poor statistical performance. Previous work established that failing to accommodate multinucleotide (or multihit, MH) substitutions strongly biases dN/dS-based inference towards false-positive inferences of diversifying episodic selection, as does failing to model variation in the rate of synonymous substitution (SRV) among sites. Here, we develop an integrated analytical framework and software tools to simultaneously incorporate these sources of evolutionary complexity into selection analyses. We found that both MH and SRV are ubiquitous in empirical alignments, and incorporating them has a strong effect on whether or not positive selection is detected (1.4-fold reduction) and on the distributions of inferred evolutionary rates. With simulation studies, we show that this effect is not attributable to reduced statistical power caused by using a more complex model. After a detailed examination of 21 benchmark alignments and a new high-resolution analysis showing which parts of the alignment provide support for positive selection, we show that MH substitutions occurring along shorter branches in the tree explain a significant fraction of discrepant results in selection detection. Our results add to the growing body of literature which examines decades-old modeling assumptions (including MH) and finds them to be problematic for comparative genomic data analysis. Because multinucleotide substitutions have a significant impact on natural selection detection even at the level of an entire gene, we recommend that selection analyses of this type consider their inclusion as a matter of routine. To facilitate this procedure, we developed, implemented, and benchmarked a simple and well-performing model testing selection detection framework able to screen an alignment for positive selection with two biologically important confounding processes: site-to-site synonymous rate variation, and multinucleotide instantaneous substitutions.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Molecular , Modelos Genéticos , Genómica , Evolución Biológica , Selección Genética , Sesgo , Humanos , Animales , Heurística , Simulación por Computador , Polimorfismo de Nucleótido Simple , Sustitución de Aminoácidos , Polimorfismo Genético , Virus/genética
8.
Bioinformatics ; 39(1)2023 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36453849

RESUMEN

MOTIVATION: The neighbor-joining (NJ) algorithm is a widely used method to perform iterative clustering and forms the basis for phylogenetic reconstruction in several bioinformatic pipelines. Although NJ is considered to be a computationally efficient algorithm, it does not scale well for datasets exceeding several thousand taxa (>100 000). Optimizations to the canonical NJ algorithm have been proposed; these optimizations are, however, achieved through approximations or extensive memory usage, which is not feasible for large datasets. RESULTS: In this article, two new algorithms, dynamic neighbor joining (DNJ) and heuristic neighbor joining (HNJ), are presented, which optimize the canonical NJ method to scale to millions of taxa without increasing the memory requirements. Both DNJ and HNJ outperform the current gold standard methods to construct NJ trees, while DNJ is guaranteed to produce exact NJ trees. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION: https://bitbucket.org/genomicepidemiology/ccphylo.git. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: Supplementary data are available at Bioinformatics online.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Modelos Genéticos , Filogenia , Algoritmos , Análisis por Conglomerados
9.
Bioinformatics ; 39(5)2023 05 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37166444

RESUMEN

MOTIVATION: Tertiary structure alignment is one of the main challenges in the computer-aided comparative study of molecular structures. Its aim is to optimally overlay the 3D shapes of two or more molecules in space to find the correspondence between their nucleotides. Alignment is the starting point for most algorithms that assess structural similarity or find common substructures. Thus, it has applications in solving a variety of bioinformatics problems, e.g. in the search for structural patterns, structure clustering, identifying structural redundancy, and evaluating the prediction accuracy of 3D models. To date, several tools have been developed to align 3D structures of RNA. However, most of them are not applicable to arbitrarily large structures and do not allow users to parameterize the optimization algorithm. RESULTS: We present two customizable heuristics for flexible alignment of 3D RNA structures, geometric search (GEOS), and genetic algorithm (GENS). They work in sequence-dependent/independent mode and find the suboptimal alignment of expected quality (below a predefined RMSD threshold). We compare their performance with those of state-of-the-art methods for aligning RNA structures. We show the results of quantitative and qualitative tests run for all of these algorithms on benchmark sets of RNA structures. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION: Source codes for both heuristics are hosted at https://github.com/RNApolis/rnahugs.


Asunto(s)
ARN , Programas Informáticos , ARN/química , Heurística , Algoritmos , Conformación de Ácido Nucleico
10.
Bioinformatics ; 39(1)2023 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36655786

RESUMEN

MOTIVATION: Folding during transcription can have an important influence on the structure and function of RNA molecules, as regions closer to the 5' end can fold into metastable structures before potentially stronger interactions with the 3' end become available. Thermodynamic RNA folding models are not suitable to predict structures that result from cotranscriptional folding, as they can only calculate properties of the equilibrium distribution. Other software packages that simulate the kinetic process of RNA folding during transcription exist, but they are mostly applicable for short sequences. RESULTS: We present a new algorithm that tracks changes to the RNA secondary structure ensemble during transcription. At every transcription step, new representative local minima are identified, a neighborhood relation is defined and transition rates are estimated for kinetic simulations. After every simulation, a part of the ensemble is removed and the remainder is used to search for new representative structures. The presented algorithm is deterministic (up to numeric instabilities of simulations), fast (in comparison with existing methods), and it is capable of folding RNAs much longer than 200 nucleotides. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION: This software is open-source and available at https://github.com/ViennaRNA/drtransformer. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: Supplementary data are available at Bioinformatics online.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Pliegue del ARN , Conformación de Ácido Nucleico , ARN/química , Programas Informáticos , Algoritmos
11.
Bioinformatics ; 39(1)2023 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36669133

RESUMEN

MOTIVATION: Binary (or Boolean) matrices provide a common effective data representation adopted in several domains of computational biology, especially for investigating cancer and other human diseases. For instance, they are used to summarize genetic aberrations-copy number alterations or mutations-observed in cancer patient cohorts, effectively highlighting combinatorial relations among them. One of these is the tendency for two or more genes not to be co-mutated in the same sample or patient, i.e. a mutual-exclusivity trend. Exploiting this principle has allowed identifying new cancer driver protein-interaction networks and has been proposed to design effective combinatorial anti-cancer therapies rationally. Several tools exist to identify and statistically assess mutual-exclusive cancer-driver genomic events. However, these tools need to be equipped with robust/efficient methods to sort rows and columns of a binary matrix to visually highlight possible mutual-exclusivity trends. RESULTS: Here, we formalize the mutual-exclusivity-sorting problem and present MutExMatSorting: an R package implementing a computationally efficient algorithm able to sort rows and columns of a binary matrix to highlight mutual-exclusivity patterns. Particularly, our algorithm minimizes the extent of collective vertical overlap between consecutive non-zero entries across rows while maximizing the number of adjacent non-zero entries in the same row. Here, we demonstrate that existing tools for mutual-exclusivity analysis are suboptimal according to these criteria and are outperformed by MutExMatSorting. AVAILABILITY AND IMPLEMENTATION: https://github.com/AleVin1995/MutExMatSorting. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: Supplementary data are available at Bioinformatics online.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Neoplasias , Humanos , Algoritmos , Neoplasias/genética , Genómica , Biología Computacional/métodos , Mutación
12.
Syst Biol ; 72(2): 446-465, 2023 Jun 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36504374

RESUMEN

In the past two decades, genomic data have been widely used to detect historical gene flow between species in a variety of plants and animals. The Tamias quadrivittatus group of North America chipmunks, which originated through a series of rapid speciation events, are known to undergo massive amounts of mitochondrial introgression. Yet in a recent analysis of targeted nuclear loci from the group, no evidence for cross-species introgression was detected, indicating widespread cytonuclear discordance. The study used the heuristic method HYDE to detect gene flow, which may suffer from low power. Here we use the Bayesian method implemented in the program BPP to re-analyze these data. We develop a Bayesian test of introgression, calculating the Bayes factor via the Savage-Dickey density ratio using the Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sample under the model of introgression. We take a stepwise approach to constructing an introgression model by adding introgression events onto a well-supported binary species tree. The analysis detected robust evidence for multiple ancient introgression events affecting the nuclear genome, with introgression probabilities reaching 63%. We estimate population parameters and highlight the fact that species divergence times may be seriously underestimated if ancient cross-species gene flow is ignored in the analysis. We examine the assumptions and performance of HYDE and demonstrate that it lacks power if gene flow occurs between sister lineages or if the mode of gene flow does not match the assumed hybrid-speciation model with symmetrical population sizes. Our analyses highlight the power of likelihood-based inference of cross-species gene flow using genomic sequence data. [Bayesian test; BPP; chipmunks; introgression; MSci; multispecies coalescent; Savage-Dickey density ratio.].


Asunto(s)
Flujo Génico , Sciuridae , Animales , Filogenia , Teorema de Bayes , Sciuridae/genética , Funciones de Verosimilitud , Heurística , América del Norte , ADN Mitocondrial/genética
13.
Anim Cogn ; 27(1): 20, 2024 Mar 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38429612

RESUMEN

While foraging, animals have to find potential food sites, remember these sites, and plan the best navigation route. To deal with problems associated with foraging for multiple and patchy resources, primates may employ heuristic strategies to improve foraging success. Until now, no study has attempted to investigate experimentally the use of such strategies by a primate in a context involving foraging in large-scale space. Thus, we carried out an experimental field study that aimed to test if wild common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) employ heuristic strategies to efficiently navigate through multiple feeding sites distributed in a large-scale space. In our experiment, we arranged four feeding platforms in a trapezoid configuration with up to 60 possible routes and observe marmosets' decisions under two experimental conditions. In experimental condition I, all platforms contained the same amount of food; in experimental condition II, the platforms had different amounts of food. According to the number and arrangement of the platforms, we tested two heuristic strategies: the Nearest Neighbor Rule and the Gravity Rule. Our results revealed that wild common marmosets prefer to use routes consistent with a heuristic strategy more than expected by chance, regardless of food distribution. The findings also demonstrate that common marmosets seem to integrate different factors such as distance and quantity of food across multiple sites distributed over a large-scale space, employing a combination of heuristic strategies to select the most efficient routes available. In summary, our findings confirm our expectations and provide important insights into the spatial cognition of these small neotropical primates.


Asunto(s)
Callithrix , Cognición , Animales , Alimentos , Heurística , Recuerdo Mental
14.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 19(6): e1011087, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37262023

RESUMEN

Human behavior emerges from planning over elaborate decompositions of tasks into goals, subgoals, and low-level actions. How are these decompositions created and used? Here, we propose and evaluate a normative framework for task decomposition based on the simple idea that people decompose tasks to reduce the overall cost of planning while maintaining task performance. Analyzing 11,117 distinct graph-structured planning tasks, we find that our framework justifies several existing heuristics for task decomposition and makes predictions that can be distinguished from two alternative normative accounts. We report a behavioral study of task decomposition (N = 806) that uses 30 randomly sampled graphs, a larger and more diverse set than that of any previous behavioral study on this topic. We find that human responses are more consistent with our framework for task decomposition than alternative normative accounts and are most consistent with a heuristic-betweenness centrality-that is justified by our approach. Taken together, our results suggest the computational cost of planning is a key principle guiding the intelligent structuring of goal-directed behavior.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Humanos , Objetivos , Conducta
15.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 19(9): e1011458, 2023 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37669314

RESUMEN

Food webs are complex ecological networks whose structure is both ecologically and statistically constrained, with many network properties being correlated with each other. Despite the recognition of these invariable relationships in food webs, the use of the principle of maximum entropy (MaxEnt) in network ecology is still rare. This is surprising considering that MaxEnt is a statistical tool precisely designed for understanding and predicting many types of constrained systems. This principle asserts that the least-biased probability distribution of a system's property, constrained by prior knowledge about that system, is the one with maximum information entropy. MaxEnt has been proven useful in many ecological modeling problems, but its application in food webs and other ecological networks is limited. Here we show how MaxEnt can be used to derive many food-web properties both analytically and heuristically. First, we show how the joint degree distribution (the joint probability distribution of the numbers of prey and predators for each species in the network) can be derived analytically using the number of species and the number of interactions in food webs. Second, we present a heuristic and flexible approach of finding a network's adjacency matrix (the network's representation in matrix format) based on simulated annealing and SVD entropy. We built two heuristic models using the connectance and the joint degree sequence as statistical constraints, respectively. We compared both models' predictions against corresponding null and neutral models commonly used in network ecology using open access data of terrestrial and aquatic food webs sampled globally (N = 257). We found that the heuristic model constrained by the joint degree sequence was a good predictor of many measures of food-web structure, especially the nestedness and motifs distribution. Specifically, our results suggest that the structure of terrestrial and aquatic food webs is mainly driven by their joint degree distribution.


Asunto(s)
Cadena Alimentaria , Heurística , Entropía , Sesgo , Conocimiento
16.
J Chem Inf Model ; 64(4): 1277-1289, 2024 Feb 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38359461

RESUMEN

Predicting the synthesizability of a new molecule remains an unsolved challenge that chemists have long tackled with heuristic approaches. Here, we report a new method for predicting synthesizability using a simple yet accurate thermochemical descriptor. We introduce Emin, the energy difference between a molecule and its lowest energy constitutional isomer, as a synthesizability predictor that is accurate, physically meaningful, and first-principles based. We apply Emin to 134,000 molecules in the QM9 data set and find that Emin is accurate when used alone and reduces incorrect predictions of "synthesizable" by up to 52% when used to augment commonly used prediction methods. Our work illustrates how first-principles thermochemistry and heuristic approximations for molecular stability are complementary, opening a new direction for synthesizability prediction methods.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Isomerismo
17.
J Chem Inf Model ; 64(12): 4928-4937, 2024 Jun 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38837744

RESUMEN

Drug repositioning is a strategy of repurposing approved drugs for treating new indications, which can accelerate the drug discovery process, reduce development costs, and lower the safety risk. The advancement of biotechnology has significantly accelerated the speed and scale of biological data generation, offering significant potential for drug repositioning through biomedical knowledge graphs that integrate diverse entities and relations from various biomedical sources. To fully learn the semantic information and topological structure information from the biological knowledge graph, we propose a knowledge graph convolutional network with a heuristic search, named KGCNH, which can effectively utilize the diversity of entities and relationships in biological knowledge graphs, as well as topological structure information, to predict the associations between drugs and diseases. Specifically, we design a relation-aware attention mechanism to compute the attention scores for each neighboring entity of a given entity under different relations. To address the challenge of randomness of the initial attention scores potentially impacting model performance and to expand the search scope of the model, we designed a heuristic search module based on Gumbel-Softmax, which uses attention scores as heuristic information and introduces randomness to assist the model in exploring more optimal embeddings of drugs and diseases. Following this module, we derive the relation weights, obtain the embeddings of drugs and diseases through neighborhood aggregation, and then predict drug-disease associations. Additionally, we employ feature-based augmented views to enhance model robustness and mitigate overfitting issues. We have implemented our method and conducted experiments on two data sets. The results demonstrate that KGCNH outperforms competing methods. In particular, case studies on lithium and quetiapine confirm that KGCNH can retrieve more actual drug-disease associations in the top prediction results.


Asunto(s)
Reposicionamiento de Medicamentos , Humanos , Heurística , Redes Neurales de la Computación
18.
Biomed Eng Online ; 23(1): 21, 2024 Feb 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38368358

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Human activity Recognition (HAR) using smartphone sensors suffers from two major problems: sensor orientation and placement. Sensor orientation and sensor placement problems refer to the variation in sensor signal for a particular activity due to sensors' altering orientation and placement. Extracting orientation and position invariant features from raw sensor signals is a simple solution for tackling these problems. Using few heuristic features rather than numerous time-domain and frequency-domain features offers more simplicity in this approach. The heuristic features are features which have very minimal effects of sensor orientation and placement. In this study, we evaluated the effectiveness of four simple heuristic features in solving the sensor orientation and placement problems using a 1D-CNN-LSTM model for a data set consisting of over 12 million samples. METHODS: We accumulated data from 42 participants for six common daily activities: Lying, Sitting, Walking, and Running at 3-Metabolic Equivalent of Tasks (METs), 5-METs and 7-METs from a single accelerometer sensor of a smartphone. We conducted our study for three smartphone positions: Pocket, Backpack and Hand. We extracted simple heuristic features from the accelerometer data and used them to train and test a 1D-CNN-LSTM model to evaluate their effectiveness in solving sensor orientation and placement problems. RESULTS: We performed intra-position and inter-position evaluations. In intra-position evaluation, we trained and tested the model using data from the same smartphone position, whereas, in inter-position evaluation, the training and test data was from different smartphone positions. For intra-position evaluation, we acquired 70-73% accuracy; for inter-position cases, the accuracies ranged between 59 and 69%. Moreover, we performed participant-specific and activity-specific analyses. CONCLUSIONS: We found that the simple heuristic features are considerably effective in solving orientation problems. With further development, such as fusing the heuristic features with other methods that eliminate placement issues, we can also achieve a better result than the outcome we achieved using the heuristic features for the sensor placement problem. In addition, we found the heuristic features to be more effective in recognizing high-intensity activities.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Teléfono Inteligente , Humanos , Actividades Humanas , Caminata , Acelerometría/métodos
19.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 242: 105907, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38513328

RESUMEN

Intuitive statistical inferences refer to making inferences about uncertain events based on limited probabilistic information, which is crucial for both human and non-human species' survival and reproduction. Previous research found that 7- and 8-year-old children failed in intuitive statistical inference tasks after heuristic strategies had been controlled. However, few studies systematically explored children's heuristic strategies of intuitive statistical inferences and their potential numerical underpinnings. In the current research, Experiment 1 (N = 81) examined 7- to 10-year-olds' use of different types of heuristic strategies; results revealed that children relied more on focusing on the absolute number strategy. Experiment 2 (N = 99) and Experiment 3 (N = 94) added continuous-format stimuli to examine whether 7- and 8-year-olds could make genuine intuitive statistical inferences instead of heuristics. Results revealed that both 7- and 8-year-olds and 9- and 10-year-olds performed better in intuitive statistical inference tasks with continuous-format stimuli, even after focusing on the absolute number strategy had been controlled. The results across the three experiments preliminarily hinted that the ratio processing system might rely on the approximate number system. Future research could clarify what specific numerical processing mechanism may be used and how it might support children's statistical intuitions.


Asunto(s)
Heurística , Intuición , Humanos , Incertidumbre
20.
BMC Public Health ; 24(1): 207, 2024 01 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38233842

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Schools are a key setting for supporting youth physical activity, given their broad reach and diverse student populations. Organizational readiness is a precursor to the successful implementation of school-based physical activity opportunities. The R = MC2 heuristic (Readiness = Motivation x Innovation-Specific Capacity x General Capacity) describes readiness as a function of an organization's motivation and capacity to implement an innovation and can be applied to better understand the implementation process. The purpose of this study was to explore the barriers to and facilitators of implementing school-based physical activity opportunities in the context of organizational readiness. METHODS: We analyzed interview data from 15 elementary school staff (principals, assistant principals, physical education teachers, and classroom teachers) from a school district in Texas. We focused on factors related to adopting, implementing, and sustaining a variety of school-based physical activity opportunities. We used the Framework Method to guide the analysis and coded data using deductive (informed by the R = MC2 heuristic) and inductive approaches. Themes were generated using the frequency, depth, and richness of participant responses. RESULTS: Four themes emerged from the data: (1) implementation is aided by the presence of internal and external relationships; (2) physical activity opportunities compete with other school priorities; (3) seeing the benefits of physical activity opportunities motivates school staff toward implementation; and (4) staff buy-in is critical to the implementation process. Themes 1-3 aligned with subcomponents of the R = MC2 heuristic (intra- and inter-organizational relationships, priority, and observability), whereas Theme 4 (staff buy-in) related to multiple subcomponents within the Motivation component but was ultimately viewed as a distinct construct. CONCLUSION: Our results highlight and explain how key readiness constructs impact the implementation of school-based physical activity opportunities. They also highlight the importance of obtaining staff buy-in when implementing in the school setting. This information is critical to developing readiness-building strategies that help schools improve their capacity to deliver physical activity opportunities effectively. TRIAL REGISTRATION: Not applicable.


Asunto(s)
Ejercicio Físico , Heurística , Adolescente , Humanos , Investigación Cualitativa , Estudiantes , Motivación
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA