Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 73
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Laterality ; 27(3): 257-272, 2022 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34396912

RESUMO

Twenty six empirical investigations have now established that children and adults with DSM-defined Attention deficit/Hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) of the Hyperactive type (ADHD-H) or, more commonly, Combined Hyperactive/Inattentive types (ADHD-C) manifest a small but significant visual attentional bias to the right side (left subclinical neglect), consistently suggesting the existence of a subtle right hemisphere dysfunction or hemispheric imbalance in hyperactive people. Only one research team has investigated and compared the DSM-defined Inattentive subtype (ADHD-I) to the Hyperactive subtype (ADHD-H), confirming that line bisection is biased to the right in ADHD-H and discovering that it is biased to the left in ADHD-I. We aimed to test whether a similar crossed double dissociation would extend to Rey's Complex Figure Copy Task (RCF-CT), a simple visuospatial-constructive task. Clinical files of 205 juvenile clients from 6 to 16 years of age from a neuropsychological private clinic specialized in ADHD were analysed. Extreme scores on the Connors-3 Hyperactivity vs Inattention Parent Rating scales associated, respectively, with significant rightward and significant leftward emplacement of the drawing on the page on the RCF-CT. These results replicate previous findings and extend the "energetics" model of hemispheric specialization.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade , Viés de Atenção , Adulto , Criança , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos
2.
Eur J Clin Invest ; 51(9): e13641, 2021 Sep.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34146339

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Clinical research is becoming increasingly popular in Europe at a growth rate much higher than expected, especially in Benelux. Although traditionally thought to be the purview of academic health centres, clinical research to evaluate new drugs, devices and medical practices is being done more and more in healthcare organizations with little or no academic affiliation. METHODS: By managing a new infrastructure and centralizing resources and demands, clinical research unit (CRU) has become an effective mechanism for hospital research. The 'infrastructure' or CRU refers to the necessary resources and how the CRU is organized and communicates operationally to conduct clinical research within the institution. The creation of a new CRU within the Robert Schuman Hospital in Luxembourg is described in this article. RESULTS: This article discusses the concrete steps and basic elements such as patient-centric and hospital approaches needed to create and structure a CRU to provide academic or industry-sponsored research support in clinical research. CONCLUSIONS: Some infrastructure challenges (insufficient engagement, regulatory and administrative barriers) and possible courses of action (standardized procedures, training and centralization) will be discussed.


Assuntos
Pesquisa Biomédica/organização & administração , Ensaios Clínicos como Assunto/organização & administração , Unidades Hospitalares/organização & administração , Pesquisa Biomédica/economia , Pesquisa Biomédica/legislação & jurisprudência , Mão de Obra em Saúde , Humanos , Tecnologia da Informação , Luxemburgo , Seleção de Pacientes , Apoio à Pesquisa como Assunto
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(6): 1593-1602, 2018 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29582107

RESUMO

Two behavioral estimates of interhemispheric transfer time, the crossed-uncrossed difference (CUD) and the unilateral field advantage (UFA), are thought to, respectively, index transfer of premotor and visual information across the corpus callosum in neurotypical participants. However, no attempt to manipulate visual and motor contingencies in a set of tasks while measuring the CUD and the UFA has yet been reported. In two go/no-go comparison experiments, stimulus pair orientations were manipulated. The hand of response changed after each correct response in the second, but not the first experiment. No correlation was found between the CUD and the UFA, supporting the hypothesis that these two measures index different types of information transfer across hemispheres. An effect of manipulation of stimulus pair orientation on UFAs was attributed to the homotopy of callosal fibers transferring visual information, while an effect of hand switching on CUDs was attributed mostly to spatial compatibility.


Assuntos
Corpo Caloso/fisiologia , Função Executiva/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Transferência de Experiência , Adulto Jovem
4.
Epilepsy Behav ; 85: 129-140, 2018 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29940376

RESUMO

In patients with cortical lesions, the structure of intelligence has never been studied as a function of age at lesion onset and presence vs absence of lesional epilepsy over the life span. METHOD: Two thousand one hundred eighty-six cases were assembled bearing unilateral cortical lesions occurring at all ages (1301 with seizures) with postlesion verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) (VIQ) and performance IQ (PIQ). RESULTS: Global IQ significantly and constantly decreased as a function of age at lesion onset in the cases without epilepsy, and increased in the cases with epilepsy. Beyond the lesion onset age of 12 years, VIQ was significantly higher than PIQ in the cases without epilepsy, and lower in the cases with epilepsy. The VIQ/PIQ × lesion-side interaction indicative of hemispheric specialization increased significantly linearly with age at lesion onset in the patients without epilepsy but ceased to progress at the lesion-onset age of 30 years and beyond in the cases with epilepsy. CONCLUSION: Postlesion global IQ, the difference between VIQ and PIQ, and the laterality index all vary significantly as a function of age at lesion onset. In addition, these changes over the life span are all quite different between cases with and without epilepsy.


Assuntos
Córtex Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagem , Córtex Cerebral/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Epilepsia/diagnóstico por imagem , Testes de Inteligência , Inteligência/fisiologia , Longevidade/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Epilepsia/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Adulto Jovem
5.
Laterality ; 23(4): 422-440, 2018 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28876165

RESUMO

Disparity of verbal and performance intelligence (VIQ, PIQ) on the Wechsler scales of intelligence is a conceptually cluttered and empirically weak measure of hemispheric specialization (HS). However, in the context of life span research, it is the only measure that can be exploited meta-analytically with the lesion method from prenatal life to late senescence. We assembled 1917 cases with a unilateral cortical focal brain lesion occurring at all ages and a post-lesion VIQ and PIQ. Lesion locus, volume and side were documented for each case, as well as age at lesion onset, age at first symptoms and age at the IQ test, presence/absence of epilepsy, lesion aetiology, gender, date of publication or of transfer of medical file. With and without covariate adjustment, HS was significant across the life span though its pattern changed. HS increased linearly and highly significantly until late senescence. Only in early adulthood did VIQ appear to vacate the right temporal lobe and occupy the left and PIQ vacate the left parietal lobe and occupy the right until late senescence. Biomaturational factors are more important in the ontogeny of material-specific HS over the whole life span than previously established.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/fisiologia , Envelhecimento/psicologia , Encéfalo/crescimento & desenvolvimento , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Inteligência/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idade de Início , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Encefalopatias/fisiopatologia , Encefalopatias/psicologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Adulto Jovem
6.
Am J Psychol ; 128(3): 355-65, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26442342

RESUMO

To study local-global relationships in interhemispheric interactions, tachistoscopically presented pairs of lines (1.15 degrees) were compared for their relative orientation by 48 neurotypical adults. Orientations of line stimuli (local aspect of the task) were vertical, horizontal, forward slash or backslash, as were those of the interstimulus axes. The latter created a global context that could influence line discrimination. Stimulus pairs were presented within a field (not requiring callosal participation for line orientation comparison) or one on each side of the visual field meridian (requiring callosal participation). The primary purpose of the design was to determine whether local or global violations of stimulus "homotopy" across the meridian would impose costs of interhemispheric integration. The rationale for this expectation is that the fiber projection of the corpus callosum is highly symmetric across the midsagittal plane (i.e., homotopic). The expected "callosal homotopy" effect was significantly upheld as a whole but broke down or became extravagant in certain specific conditions, with specific costs of interhemispheric integration varying from null to a highly significant 20-ms as a function of interactions of interstimulus and stimulus orientations. The corpus callosum seems to be particularly sensitive to local stimulus orientation in interaction with long-range stimulus context orientation.


Assuntos
Corpo Caloso/fisiologia , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Orientação/fisiologia , Reconhecimento Visual de Modelos/fisiologia , Atenção/fisiologia , Tomada de Decisões , Discriminação Psicológica/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Fibras Nervosas/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Estudantes/psicologia
7.
Epilepsy Behav ; 29(1): 53-62, 2013 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23933913

RESUMO

Validity of two models of hemispheric specialization was compared. The "material-specific impairment" model was radicalized as postulating that left hemisphere (LH) lesions impair processing of verbal material and that right hemisphere (RH) lesions impair processing of visuospatial material, independently of response-bias distortions. The "response-bias distortion" model was radicalized as postulating that LH lesions distort response style toward omissiveness and that RH lesions distort response style toward commissiveness, regardless of material-specific impairments. Participants had comparable left (N=27) or right (N=24) hemisphere cortical lesions having occurred between birth and early adolescence. Four cognitive neuropsychological tests were adjusted to optimize applicability and comparability of the two theoretical models: Rey Complex Figure, Kimura's Recurring Figures, the Story Recall subtest of the Children's Memory Scale, and the California Verbal Learning Test. Both models significantly, independently, and equally distinguished the LH from the RH patients. Both these forms of hemispheric specialization seemed to be implemented very early in life and very rigidly. Intrahemispheric lesion sites, e.g., frontal vs nonfrontal, held no significant relation to the effects described above.


Assuntos
Viés , Encéfalo/patologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/etiologia , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Epilepsia/complicações , Epilepsia/patologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Aprendizagem por Associação , Criança , Transtornos Cognitivos/patologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Modelos Biológicos , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Aprendizagem Verbal , Percepção Visual , Adulto Jovem
8.
Laterality ; 18(4): 385-406, 2013.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22757589

RESUMO

Significant left subclinical hemi-neglect or "hemi-unawareness" in juvenile and adult ADHD has been reported many times. However, this literature has never been thoroughly reviewed, and is generally ignored in neuropsychological accounts of ADHD. The purposes of the present report were (1) to introduce a systematic review of this literature and (2) to prospectively test whether adults with ADHD (combined type) would present left hemi-unawareness measurable on a test of executive function commonly used with children and adults with ADHD, the Colour-Word Interference Test of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System. This test requires participants to discriminate unusual stimulus characteristics by stating words aloud while visually scanning plates comprising either 50 colour patches or 50 words. A total of 28 ADHD adults (combined type) and 30 IQ-, age-, and gender-matched controls completed the task. The ADHD group manifested a significantly higher proportion of errors on the left than right sides of the plates. This atypical left-right balance was related to severity of the ADHD according to the Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS) as well as to several other indicators from the Continuous Performance Test-II.


Assuntos
Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/complicações , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/fisiopatologia , Função Executiva , Lateralidade Funcional , Transtornos da Percepção/complicações , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico , Desempenho Psicomotor , Adulto , Transtorno do Deficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade/diagnóstico , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Transtornos da Percepção/fisiopatologia , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
9.
Clin Pract ; 13(5): 1266-1285, 2023 Oct 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37887090

RESUMO

Prehospital care plays a critical role in improving patient outcomes, particularly in cases of time-sensitive emergencies such as trauma, cardiac failure, stroke, bleeding, breathing difficulties, systemic infections, etc. In recent years, there has been a growing interest in clinical research in prehospital care, and several challenges and opportunities have emerged. There is an urgent need to adapt clinical research methodology to a context of prehospital care. At the same time, there are many barriers in prehospital research due to the complex context, posing unique challenges for research, development, and evaluation. Among these, this review allows the highlighting of limited resources and infrastructure, ethical and regulatory considerations, time constraints, privacy, safety concerns, data collection and analysis, selection of a homogeneous study group, etc. The analysis of the literature also highlights solutions such as strong collaboration between emergency medical services (EMS) and hospital care, use of (mobile) health technologies and artificial intelligence, use of standardized protocols and guidelines, etc. Overall, the purpose of this narrative review is to examine the current state of clinical research in prehospital care and identify gaps in knowledge, including the challenges and opportunities for future research.

10.
Healthcare (Basel) ; 11(16)2023 Aug 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37628452

RESUMO

The design of a clinical research protocol to evaluate new therapies, devices, patient quality of life, and medical practices from scratch is probably one of the greatest challenges for the majority of novice researchers. This is especially true since a high-quality methodology is required to achieve success and effectiveness in academic and hospital research centers. This review discusses the concrete steps and necessary guidelines needed to create and structure a research protocol. Along with the methodology, some administrative challenges (ethics, regulatory and people-management barriers) and possible time-saving recommendations (standardized procedures, collaborative training, and centralization) are discussed.

11.
Epilepsy Behav ; 21(1): 12-9, 2011 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21482195

RESUMO

With a multiple case report analysis we demonstrate that hypersexuality more often results from right hemisphere (RH) (n=26) than left hemisphere (LH) (n=7) lesions, possibly because of LH release after the RH lesion, and that ictal orgasm more often occurs in patients with right-sided (n=23) than left-sided (n=8) seizure foci, with the symptom probably resulting from RH activation. The LH may be specialized for increasing sexual tension, whereas the RH may be specialized for release of this tension (orgasm), the former being catabolic and the latter anabolic. Several other interpretations of the findings are also discussed.


Assuntos
Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Orgasmo/fisiologia , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/patologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Epilepsia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Disfunções Sexuais Psicogênicas/fisiopatologia , Adulto Jovem
12.
Kidney Blood Press Res ; 34(2): 125-34, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21335974

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: N-acetylcysteine (NAC) has been proposed to prevent radiocontrast nephropathy in high-risk patients. METHODS: The effect of single-dose and prolonged administration of NAC before application of either the ionic, high-osmolar radiocontrast agent diatrizoate sodium (DTZ) or the nonionic, low-osmolar radiocontrast agent iohexol (IOH) in a rat model combining uninephrectomy, salt depletion, and administration of indomethacin was explored. Arterial blood pressure and total, cortical, and medullary blood flow were continuously recorded in anesthetized Sprague-Dawley rats. RESULTS: NAC had no effect on renal hemodynamics in control rats. Both DTZ and IOH induced biphasic changes in renal blood flow and cortical renal blood flux and persistently reduced medullary blood flux. Neither single-dose nor prolonged administration of NAC prevented the hemodynamic changes following administration of DTZ or IOH, respectively. Acute prophylactic administration of NAC prevented increased urinary ET excretion after injection of IOH and, to a smaller degree, of DTZ. Both an ionic, high-osmolar (DTZ) and a nonionic, low-osmolar (IOH) radiocontrast agent induce marked changes in renal hemodynamics in salt-depleted rats treated with indomethacin. CONCLUSIONS: Renal perfusion is not affected by NAC application in a model of experimental contrast nephropathy in rats. Other effects of NAC might thus account for the presumed renoprotective properties.


Assuntos
Acetilcisteína/uso terapêutico , Meios de Contraste/efeitos adversos , Nefropatias/prevenção & controle , Circulação Renal/efeitos dos fármacos , Acetilcisteína/farmacologia , Animais , Hemodinâmica/efeitos dos fármacos , Rim/irrigação sanguínea , Nefropatias/induzido quimicamente , Ratos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley
13.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 16(1): 1-39, 2011 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20198522

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Neurocognitive accounts of delusion have traditionally highlighted perceptual misrepresentation, as the primary trigger in addition to other cognitive deficits that maintain the delusion. Here, a general neurocognitive model of delusional disorder (DSM-IV) is proposed, not so much based on perceptual or cognitive deficits after right hemisphere damage as on cognitive propensities, specifically excessive inferencing (especially jumping to conclusions) and excessive reference to the self, due to left hemisphere overactivity. METHOD: The functional imaging, topographic EEG, and experimental imaging literatures on delusional disorder are reviewed, and 37 previously published cases of postunilateral lesion delusion (DSM-IV type, grandeur, persecution, jealousy, erotomania, or somatic), are reviewed and analysed multivariately. RESULTS: Functional imaging and EEG topography data were slightly more indicative of left hemisphere overactivity in delusional disorder. In addition, 73% of the postunilateral lesion cases (χ(2)=7.8, p=.005) of delusional disorder (DSM-IV type) had a right hemisphere lesion, whereas only 27% had a left hemisphere lesion. CONCLUSION: Left hemisphere release appears to be a more primary cause of delusional disorder than right hemisphere impairment, the latter merely entailing loss of inhibition of delusional beliefs. We propose that most patients with DSM-IV diagnoses of delusional disorder could be afflicted by excessive left hemisphere activity, but further research is necessary.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Delusões/fisiopatologia , Delusões/psicologia , Lateralidade Funcional , Modelos Neurológicos , Modelos Psicológicos , Delusões/diagnóstico , Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais , Eletroencefalografia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética
14.
Am J Psychol ; 124(2): 163-76, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21834402

RESUMO

We report 2 experiments designed to demonstrate that unilateral tachistoscopic stimulation would yield a response time (RT) advantage over bilateral stimulation in a simple experiment, whereas the opposite pattern would occur in a complex version of the same task, as predicted by the intrahemispheric resource limitation model of Banich and colleagues. Experiment 1 was a go/no-go task in which participants had to press a key when two shapes (circles or squares) were identical on the computer screen. A unilateral field advantage was obtained that was accentuated in several task conditions that yielded overall longer RTs, mostly in the bilateral condition. Experiment 2 was similar but required a more complex judgment: The go trials were to 2 stimuli identical on 1 dimension (shape or color) but not both or neither. The RTs were significantly and substantially longer than in Experiment 1 and exhibited a nonsignificant bilateral field advantage, which differed significantly from the unilateral field advantage obtained in Experiment 1. These results support the intrahemispheric resource limitation model of Banich and colleagues. However, several within-experiment effects are in direct opposition to this model and are best explained as limitations of commissural relay of perceptual information.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Tempo de Reação/fisiologia , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
15.
Brain Cogn ; 71(1): 46-51, 2009 Oct.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19446944

RESUMO

Tachistoscopically presented bilateral stimulus pairs not parallel to the meridian produced significantly longer RTs on a task requiring discrimination of shapes (Go/no-Go) than pairs emplaced symmetrically on each side of the meridian in Desjardins and Braun [Desjardins, S., & Braun, C. M. J. (2006). Homotopy and heterotopy and the bilateral field advantage in the Dimond paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 121, 125-136]. This was explained by the fact that there are more homotopic than heterotopic fibers in the corpus callosum. However: (1) different parts of the visual field were not equiprobably stimulated, possibly causing subtle biases, (2) the predicted cost of vertical asymmetry was tested only with bilateral stimuli, and (3) interstimulus distance was at the outer limit of callosal midline fusion (10.6 degrees ). Here, a tachistoscopic experiment with 24 normal participants replicated the between-field vertical symmetry advantage [Desjardins, S., & Braun, C. M. J. (2006). Homotopy and heterotopy and the bilateral field advantage in the Dimond paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 121,125-136.], but without irrelevant stimulation conditions and with more proximal stimuli. In addition, a significant specific cost of vertical asymmetry of 7ms was found for between-field integration over within-field integration. As far as we know, this is the first demonstration of an effect of callosal anatomical homotopy with reaction time.


Assuntos
Corpo Caloso/anatomia & histologia , Desempenho Psicomotor , Tempo de Reação , Percepção Visual , Adulto , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Estimulação Luminosa , Análise e Desempenho de Tarefas , Vias Visuais/anatomia & histologia , Adulto Jovem
16.
Laterality ; 14(2): 122-64, 2009 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18991140

RESUMO

We propose that what appears to be hemispheric specialisation in the memory domain, as indexed by effects of unilateral brain lesions, is to a great extent explainable as response bias: left hemisphere lesions result in an omissive response bias or error pattern whereas right hemisphere lesions result in a commissive response bias or error pattern. To test this prediction a group of 40 non-confabulatory cases with a verbal and non-verbal retention deficit (hypomnesia), subsequent to a unilateral lesion, was assembled from the literature. A group of non-amnesic cases with confabulation, paramnesia, false memories or memory-laden hallucination (dysfunctional hypermnesia), due to a unilateral lesion, was also assembled from the literature (N=72). Most of the hypomnesic patients had left hemisphere lesions (73%, p<.005, two tailed) while most of the hypermnesic patients had right hemisphere lesions (78%, p<.0005, two tailed). This crossed double dissociation held good despite statistical control of the lesion's locus within the hemisphere, its size or its aetiology, presence of aphasic symptoms, psychiatric comorbidity, the patient's age, gender, or hand preference, and several other potentially confounding variables.


Assuntos
Encefalopatias , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Encefalopatias/complicações , Encefalopatias/patologia , Encefalopatias/fisiopatologia , Humanos , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Índice de Gravidade de Doença
17.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 38(4): 634-43, 2008 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17682931

RESUMO

Dream questionnaires were completed by 28 young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) participants. Seventy-nine typically developed individual served as the control group. In a subset of 17 persons with ASD and 11 controls matched for verbal IQ, dream narratives were obtained following REM sleep awakenings in a sleep laboratory. Questionnaires revealed that participants with ASD, compared to controls, had fewer recollections of dreaming, fewer bad dreams and fewer emotions. In the sleep laboratory, dream content narratives following REM sleep awakenings were shorter in ASD participants than in controls. ASD participants also reported fewer settings, objects, characters, social interactions, activities, and emotions. It is concluded that these characteristics of dreaming in ASD may reflect neurocognitive dimensions specific to this condition.


Assuntos
Transtorno Autístico/psicologia , Sonhos , Semântica , Adolescente , Adulto , Afeto , Síndrome de Asperger/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Rememoração Mental , Polissonografia , Sono REM/fisiologia , Inquéritos e Questionários
18.
J Child Neurol ; 23(6): 663-8, 2008 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18539991

RESUMO

A growing literature suggests that early lesions are associated with poorer IQ outcome. Those studies covered a restricted age range in pediatric populations only and did not control for important moderator variables. The present investigation studied IQ change in brain-lesioned children and adults (age 0 to 84 years). Altogether, 725 cases with a documented unilateral focal lesion were gathered from hospital charts and from published cases in the literature, including 240 with repeated IQ testing. Multiple regression analyses isolated the contribution of age at lesion onset to IQ change. Important mediator variables included were lesion side, site, volume, etiology, and so on. An early lesion was significantly associated with poorer postlesion IQ in time and with decline of IQ in time. Later onset lesions were associated with better postlesion IQ and recovery in time. The so-called Kennard principle is refuted, with regard to IQ.


Assuntos
Dano Encefálico Crônico/fisiopatologia , Inteligência/fisiologia , Plasticidade Neuronal/fisiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Dano Encefálico Crônico/diagnóstico , Dano Encefálico Crônico/psicologia , Causalidade , Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Dominância Cerebral/fisiologia , Feminino , Seguimentos , Humanos , Lactente , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Prognóstico
19.
Compr Psychiatry ; 49(5): 441-59, 2008.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18702930

RESUMO

Previously published single case reports of patients with a unilateral lesion were assembled. After the lesion, each of the 244 cases presented at least one of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) symptoms of a manic episode, namely, elated or irritable mood, grandiosity, talkativeness, flight of ideas, hyperhedonism, reduced need for sleep, agitation, or distractibility (all optional in DSM-IV). As expected, the subgroup of 59 manic patients had a right hemisphere lesion far more often than a left one. However, this was also true of various sets of the nonmanic cases. Furthermore, elation was not the symptom most strongly associated with lesion side. Elation without mania was not significantly predicted by lesion side. However, talkativeness was strongly predicted by right lesion side whether in manic or nonmanic patients or even when the symptom was the only symptom observed. Agitation was consistently and robustly associated with right lesion side, but not completely distinctly so (it fell short of significance when not accompanied by elation or other symptoms). It is proposed that prevalence of right hemisphere lesions causing mania is primarily related to mental and behavioral disinhibition rather than a shift of mood and that it consists of release of left hemisphere influence.


Assuntos
Transtorno Bipolar/psicologia , Infarto Encefálico/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais , Ego , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Lactente , Recém-Nascido , Humor Irritável , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Agitação Psicomotora/psicologia , Autoavaliação (Psicologia) , Comportamento Sexual/psicologia , Sono , Fala
20.
Epileptic Disord ; 10(2): 101-12, 2008 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18539560

RESUMO

Glascher and Adolph (2003) proposed that both amydalae are specialized for fear, but that the right one is a fast, short, and relatively automatic fear processor, whereas the left one is more detail-oriented and perceptual-cognitive. According to this model, early ictal fear should occur more often in cases with a right temporal lobe epileptic focus. Several authors have tried to find a hemispheric specialization for ictal fear, but have not reached the power to attain a statistically significant effect of focus side. In this study, using previously published cases of unilateral epileptic focus causing early ictal symptoms of fear, we found 144 cases, of which 98 had a right hemisphere focus (68%) and 46 having left hemisphere focus (32%, p < 0.0005). Several control variables were assembled to verify possible alternative explanations of the main effect.


Assuntos
Epilepsia/psicologia , Medo , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Convulsões/psicologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Criança , Pré-Escolar , Interpretação Estatística de Dados , Resistência a Medicamentos , Eletroencefalografia , Epilepsia/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Lactente , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtorno de Pânico/etiologia , Transtorno de Pânico/psicologia , Convulsões/fisiopatologia
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA