Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 142
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Alzheimers Dement ; 17(4): 641-652, 2021 04.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33325121

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: The clinical relevance of brain atrophy subtypes categorization in non-demented persons without a priori knowledge regarding their amyloid status or clinical presentation is unknown. METHODS: A total of 2083 outpatients with either subjective cognitive complaint or mild cognitive impairment at study entry were followed during 4 years (MEMENTO cohort). Atrophy subtypes were defined using baseline magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and previously described algorithms. RESULTS: Typical/diffuse atrophy was associated with faster cognitive decline and the highest risk of developing dementia and Alzheimer's disease (AD) over time, both in the whole analytic sample and in amyloid-positive participants. Hippocampal-sparing and limbic-predominant atrophy were also associated with incident dementia, with faster cognitive decline in the limbic predominant atrophy group. Lewy body dementia was more frequent in the hippocampal-sparing and minimal/no atrophy groups. DISCUSSION: Atrophy subtypes categorization predicted different subsequent patterns of cognitive decline and rates of conversion to distinct etiologies of dementia in persons attending memory clinics.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer , Instituições de Assistência Ambulatorial , Atrofia/patologia , Encéfalo/patologia , Transtornos da Memória , Idoso , Doença de Alzheimer/classificação , Doença de Alzheimer/patologia , Estudos de Coortes , Feminino , Hipocampo/patologia , Humanos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação
2.
Annu Rev Neurosci ; 34: 259-88, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21456960

RESUMO

Work with patient H.M., beginning in the 1950s, established key principles about the organization of memory that inspired decades of experimental work. Since H.M., the study of human memory and its disorders has continued to yield new insights and to improve understanding of the structure and organization of memory. Here we review this work with emphasis on the neuroanatomy of medial temporal lobe and diencephalic structures important for memory, multiple memory systems, visual perception, immediate memory, memory consolidation, the locus of long-term memory storage, the concepts of recollection and familiarity, and the question of how different medial temporal lobe structures may contribute differently to memory functions.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Memória/fisiologia , Neurociências , Animais , Encéfalo/anatomia & histologia , Mapeamento Encefálico , História do Século XX , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/patologia , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Modelos Animais , Neurociências/história , Percepção Visual/fisiologia
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 236(7): 2037-2046, 2018 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29744565

RESUMO

Confabulating patients produce statements and actions that are unintentionally incongruous to their history, background, present and future situation. Here we present the very unusual case of a patient with right hemisphere damage and signs of left visual neglect, who, when presented with visual stimuli, confabulated both for consciously undetected and for consciously detected left-sided details. Advanced anatomical investigation suggested a disconnection between the parietal and the temporal lobes in the right hemisphere. A disconnection between the ventral cortical visual stream and the dorsal fronto-parietal networks in the right hemisphere may contribute to confabulatory behaviour by restricting processing of left-sided stimuli to pre-conscious stages in the ventral visual stream.


Assuntos
Lateralidade Funcional , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Vias Neurais/patologia , Transtornos da Percepção , Percepção Espacial/fisiologia , Idoso , Atenção , Mapeamento Encefálico , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Imagem de Difusão por Ressonância Magnética , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico por imagem , Vias Neurais/diagnóstico por imagem , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Transtornos da Percepção/complicações , Transtornos da Percepção/diagnóstico por imagem , Transtornos da Percepção/etiologia , Estimulação Luminosa , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações
4.
Alzheimers Dement ; 14(6): 734-742, 2018 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29352855

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: We compared risk of progression from subjective cognitive decline (SCD) to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in an academic memory clinic versus a population-based study. METHODS: Older adults presenting at a memory clinic were classified as SCD (n = 113) or as noncomplainers (n = 82). Participants from a population study were classified as SCD (n = 592) and noncomplainers (n = 589) based on a memory complaint score. Annual follow-up performed for a mean of 3 years. RESULTS: The adjusted hazard ratio for SCD was 15.97 (95% confidence interval: 6.08-42.02, P < .001) in the memory clinic versus 1.18 (95% confidence interval: 1.00-1.40, P = .047) in the population study, where reported "worry" about memory further increased SCD-associated risk for MCI. DISCUSSION: SCD is more likely to progress to MCI in a memory clinic than the general population; participants' characteristics vary across settings. Study setting should be considered when evaluating SCD as a risk state for MCI and dementia.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva/classificação , Progressão da Doença , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Idoso , Instituições de Assistência Ambulatorial , Disfunção Cognitiva/epidemiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Fatores de Risco , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia
5.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 23(5): 421-430, 2017 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28481196

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: In complex real life situations, memories for temporal and spatial information are naturally linked since sequential events coincide in time and space. Whether this connection is inseparable or instead whether these processes are functionally dissociable was investigated in this patient study. METHODS: Spatial object-location and temporal order memory tasks were administered to 36 stroke patients and 44 healthy control participants. RESULTS: On group level, patients with a stroke in the left hemisphere performed worse on temporal order memory, compared to the control participants. On individual level, using a multiple case-study approach, a clear pattern of dissociations was found between memory for temporal and for spatial features. CONCLUSIONS: These findings indicate that location and temporal order memory contain functionally separable processes. This adds to our understanding of how context information is processed in human memory. (JINS, 2017, 23, 421-430).


Assuntos
Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Rememoração Mental/fisiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Memória Espacial/fisiologia , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional/fisiologia , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Acidente Vascular Cerebral/complicações
6.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 52(2): 214-226, 2017 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27349587

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Word-retrieval difficulties are commonly experienced by people with aphasia (PwA) and also by typically ageing persons. Differentiation between true naming impairments and naming difficulties found in healthy persons may, therefore, be challenging. AIMS: To investigate the extent to which the Maltese adaptation of the Boston Naming Test (BNT) can identify people with lexical retrieval difficulties and to differentiate them from people with unimpaired word finding. METHODS & PROCEDURES: Naming performance of a group of PwA was compared with the performance of a control group. Performance on the Maltese adaptation of the BNT was investigated in terms of scores, range of scores and error profiles of the two groups. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: All PwA scored below the mean score of the controls, indicating that persons who scored above the mean score may be considered as unimpaired. However, a number of the controls obtained very low scores that overlapped with the scores obtained by the PwA. This indicated that scores alone cannot be used to differentiate between impaired and unimpaired people. Some types of errors were only produced by people with impaired naming, and did not appear at all in error profiles of unimpaired individuals. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: Mild-moderate anomic impairments may be missed if naming impairment is assessed and diagnosed using a cut-off score. In order to differentiate between people with impaired and unimpaired naming, it is necessary to look at error profiles, apart from the number of errors, as the presence of atypical errors may be an important indicator of naming impairments.


Assuntos
Anomia/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Adulto , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Anomia/classificação , Anomia/psicologia , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Malta , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Psicometria/estatística & dados numéricos , Valores de Referência , Adulto Jovem
7.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 173(7-8): 498-505, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28843413

RESUMO

Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is a type of epilepsy that often has a negative impact on patients' memory. Despite the importance of patients' complaints in this regard, the difficulties described by these patients are often not easy to demonstrate through a standard neuropsychological assessment. Accelerated long-term forgetting and autobiographical memory disorders are the two main memory impairments reported in the literature in patients with TLE. However, the methods used by different authors to evaluate long-term memory and autobiographical memory are heterogeneous. This heterogeneity can lead to differences in the observed results as well as how they are interpreted. Yet, despite the methodological differences, objectification of such memory deficits appears to be both specific and robust within this patient population. Analysis of the literature shows that accelerated long-term forgetting and autobiographical memory disorders share the same clinical characteristics. This leads to the assumption that they are, in fact, only one entity and that their evaluation may be done through a single procedure. Our proposal is to place this evaluation within the context of memory consolidation disorders. With such a perspective, evaluation of accelerated forgetting in autobiographical memory should consist of identifying a disorder in the formation and/or recovery of new memory traces.


Assuntos
Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/complicações , Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/psicologia , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Memória Episódica , Memória de Longo Prazo/fisiologia , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Testes Neuropsicológicos
8.
Clin Gerontol ; 40(4): 249-257, 2017.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28459304

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: To pilot two new cognitive screening tools for use in an urban Malaysian population and to compare their criterion validity against a gold standard, the well-established Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). METHODS: The IDEA cognitive screen, Picture-based Memory Impairment Scale (PMIS), and MMSE were administered to a convenience sample of elderly (≥ 65 years) from the community and outpatient clinics at an urban teaching hospital. Consensus diagnosis was performed by two geriatricians blinded to PMIS and IDEA cognitive screen scores using the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-V) clinical criteria. The MMSE performance was used as a reference. RESULTS: The study enrolled 66 participants, with a median age of 78.5 years (interquartile range [IQR], 72.5-83.0) years and 11.0 median years of education (IQR, 9.0-13.0). Forty-three (65.2%) were female, and 32 (48.4%) were Chinese. The area under the receiver operating characteristic (AUROC) curve values were .962 (IDEA cognitive screen), .970 (PMIS), and .935 (MMSE). The optimal cutoff values for sensitivity and specificity were: IDEA cognitive screen: ≤ 11, 90.9% and 89.7%; PMIS: ≤ 6, 97.3% and 69.0%; and MMSE: ≤ 23, 84.6% and 76.0%. Although the sample size was small, multivariable logistic regression modelling suggested that all three screen scores did not appear to be educationally biased. CONCLUSION: The IDEA and PMIS tools are potentially valid screening tools for dementia in urban Malaysia, and perform at least as well as the MMSE. Further work on larger representative, cohorts is needed to further assess the psychometric properties. CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS: Study provides alternative screening tools for dementia for both non-specialists and specialists.


Assuntos
Disfunção Cognitiva/diagnóstico , Demência/diagnóstico , Programas de Rastreamento/instrumentação , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Testes de Estado Mental e Demência/normas , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Cognição/fisiologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/etnologia , Demência/etnologia , Feminino , Humanos , Malásia/epidemiologia , Masculino , Programas de Rastreamento/métodos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Psicometria/instrumentação , Psicometria/métodos , Sensibilidade e Especificidade
9.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 29(2): 141-5, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25037029

RESUMO

BACKGROUND AND AIMS: Several studies have demonstrated that patients with essential tremor (ET) may also have mild cognitive impairments (MCIs), and cross-sectional and population-based studies have shown that ET is associated with prevalent dementia. Different presentations of MCI are suggested to be associated with different pathologies. For example, amnestic MCI may be associated with Alzheimer disease. Therefore, in this study, we explored whether the MCI subtype in patients with ET (MCI-ET+) is different from the MCI subtype in patients without ET attending a memory outpatient clinic (MCI-ET-). METHODS: Using a case-control study design, cognitive status in MCI patients with ET and without ET was assessed by neuropsychological testing. Patients with MCI were stratified into groups: amnestic and nonamnestic MCI, or single-domain and multidomain MCI. RESULTS: Of the 93 patients in the ET+ group and the 169 in the ET- group, 45 (48.4%) and 94 (55.6%) patients had MCI, respectively. The frequency of MCI subtypes between the 2 groups was different, such that 25 (55.6%) patients had nonamnestic MCI in the ET+ group and 68 (72.3%) patients had amnestic MCI in ET- group (χ=10.195, P=0.001). Compared with the MCI-ET+ group, patients in the MCI-ET- group showed more severe impairments in verbal and visuospatial memory functions. CONCLUSIONS: ET is associated with MCI, particularly the nonamnestic subtype. These results suggest that cognitive change in patients with ET may have a different pathogenesis from that of Alzheimer disease.


Assuntos
Amnésia/psicologia , Disfunção Cognitiva/psicologia , Tremor Essencial/psicologia , Idoso , Amnésia/complicações , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Disfunção Cognitiva/classificação , Disfunção Cognitiva/complicações , Tremor Essencial/complicações , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/complicações , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos
10.
J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol ; 27(4): 259-65, 2014 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24763068

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: The clinical significance of subjective memory complaints in the elderly participants, particularly regarding liability of subsequent progression to dementia, has been controversial. In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that severity or type of subjective memory complaints reported by patients in a clinical setting may predict future conversion to dementia. METHODS: A cohort of nondemented patients with cognitive complaints, followed up for at least 2 years or until conversion to dementia, underwent a neuropsychological evaluation and detailed assessment of memory difficulties with the Subjective Memory Complaints (SMC) Scale. RESULTS: At baseline, patients who converted to dementia (36.8%) had less years of formal education and generally a worse performance in the neuropsychological assessment. There were no differences in the total SMC score between nonconverters (9.5 ± 4.2) and converters (8.9 ± 4.0, a nonsignificant difference), but nonconverters scored higher in several items of the scale. CONCLUSION: For patients with cognitive complaints observed in a memory clinic setting, the severity of subjective memory complaints is not useful to predict future conversion to dementia.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Demência/diagnóstico , Progressão da Doença , Avaliação Geriátrica/métodos , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Demência/psicologia , Escolaridade , Feminino , Seguimentos , Avaliação Geriátrica/estatística & dados numéricos , Humanos , Modelos Logísticos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos/estatística & dados numéricos , Valor Preditivo dos Testes , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica/estatística & dados numéricos , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Fatores Socioeconômicos
11.
Epilepsy Behav ; 26(3): 335-42, 2013 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23146363

RESUMO

Transient Epileptic Amnesia (TEA) is a recently defined subtype of temporal lobe epilepsy, principally affecting people in middle age with a male predominance. Its key manifestation is the occurrence of recurring episodes of transient amnesia, usually lasting less than an hour and often occurring on waking. One-third of patients have exclusively amnestic attacks, while in two-thirds, at least some attacks are accompanied by other manifestations of epilepsy, especially olfactory hallucinations. Several lines of evidence point to a seizure focus in the medial temporal lobes. Transient Epileptic Amnesia is accompanied by a striking loss of autobiographical memories in two-thirds of sufferers, accelerated loss of memories which had been acquired successfully in around one half, and topographical amnesia in around one-third. This paper reviews the findings of the TIME project (The Impairment of Memory in Epilepsy - http://sites.pcmd.ac.uk/time/tea.php) in relation to TEA, accelerated long-term forgetting, and remote memory impairment.


Assuntos
Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/complicações , Transtornos da Memória/complicações , Epilepsia do Lobo Temporal/epidemiologia , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/epidemiologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos
12.
Psychiatr Pol ; 47(2): 313-24, 2013.
Artigo em Polonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23888764

RESUMO

In the last two decades of the last century there has been a shift in the studies on memory. In psychology of memory the criticism of the laboratory approach resulted in development of the ecological approach. One of the effects of this change was to initiate researches on memory that includes plans for the future, which has resulted in the distinction of the concept of prospective memory. Prospective memory is used in many aspects of everyday life. It deals with remembering intentions and plans, it is connected with remembering about specific task or activity in the future. There are three types of PM: event-based prospective memory, time-based prospective memory and activity-based prospective memory. Current research in this field have already established its own paradigm and tools measuring PM and there is still increasing scientific interest in this issue. Prospective memory assessment may be carried out in various ways. Among them, the most frequently used are: a) questionnaires, b) psychological tests, c) experimental procedures. Within the latter, the additional distinction can be introduced for: the experiments conducted under natural conditions and the laboratory procedures. In Polish literature, there are only a few articles on PM. The aim of this work is to review studies on assessment methods of PM. Its neuroanatomical bases and its functioning in different mental disorders are analyzed. The work is aimed to focus clinicians attention on prospective memory as an area which is important for complex diagnosis of cognitive processes.


Assuntos
Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Memória Episódica , Transtornos Mentais/complicações , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Testes Neuropsicológicos
13.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 17(1): 143-53, 2011 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21092388

RESUMO

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), especially amnestic, often represents pre-dementia Alzheimer's disease, characterized by medial temporal lobe atrophy, while white matter (WM) alterations are insufficiently described. We analyze both cortical morphometric and WM diffusivity differences in amnestic versus non-amnestic subtypes and ask if memory and WM tract affection are related independently of cortical atrophy. Forty-nine patients from a university-hospital based memory clinic with a score of 3 on the Global Deterioration Scale aged 43-77 years (45% female) were included. Two neuropsychologists have classified cases as amnestic (aMCI), non-amnestic (naMCI), or less advanced (laMCI), not satisfying criteria for aMCI/naMCI. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) WM tract and morphometric data of the temporal-parietal memory network were compared among patient subtypes and related to story, word list, and visual memory. WM radial and mean diffusivity (DR and MD), underlying the entorhinal cortex, were higher in aMCI compared with laMCI. WM DR and MD, underlying the entorhinal, parahippocampal, and middle temporal cortex, explained unique variance in word list and story memory, and this was not due to secondary effects of cortical thinning. DTI may thus potentially aid diagnosis in early disease stages. ).


Assuntos
Mapeamento Encefálico , Encéfalo/patologia , Transtornos da Memória/patologia , Fibras Nervosas Mielinizadas/patologia , Adulto , Idoso , Distribuição de Qui-Quadrado , Imagem de Tensor de Difusão/métodos , Feminino , Humanos , Processamento de Imagem Assistida por Computador/métodos , Imageamento por Ressonância Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Análise de Regressão
14.
Actas Esp Psiquiatr ; 39(4): 251-9, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês, Espanhol | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21769748

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: A working definition of confabulation could be that of describing them as false memories due to a retrieval problem, where the patient is unaware that he/she is confabulating, and has the belief that the memory is true. Several types of confabulations have been described, according to a broad variety of criteria. Confabulations can be seen in very different neurological conditions, which have lead to a controversy on their pathophysiological mechanisms. OBJECTIVE: To obtain an updated revision in Spanish of the definitions, types, brain regions involved and neuropsychological correlates of the confabulations. DEVELOPMENT: After reviewing the concept and several types of confabulations, the damaged brain regions associated to two conditions where confabulations occur, such as Korsakoff syndrome and patients with anterior communicating artery aneurysm, are described. The neuropsychological correlates associated to them are then reviewed. CONCLUSIONS: Confabulations are a difficult-to-define complex phenomenon. Probably, the most accepted classification, in accordance with how they appear, would be that which distinguishes spontaneous from provoked confabulations, although the validity of this distinction is not clear. Regarding to crucial cerebral regions involved in the confabulations, it seems that prefrontal cortex lesions, specifically in ventromedial and orbitofrontal areas, are necessary. Neuropsychological evidence suggests the presence in most of the cases of executive dysfunction and at least some degree of memory dysfunction as an underlying mechanism of confabulation. Nevertheless, the specific characteristics of these neuropsychological dysfunctions are not well-known.


Assuntos
Encefalopatias/complicações , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Transtornos Mentais/complicações , Encefalopatias/psicologia , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação
15.
Medicine (Baltimore) ; 100(30): e25442, 2021 Jul 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34397680

RESUMO

ABSTRACT: This study aimed to explore the efficacy of Baduanjin exercise on promoting memory function, executive function and general self-efficacy, decreasing the level of subjective memory complaints of older adults.In this randomized controlled trial, 80 patients were randomly allocated in a 1:1 ratio to 12-week Baduanjin exercise group or 12-week control group. Subjective memory complaint questionnaire, Auditory verbal learning test, Trail Making Test and General Self-Efficacy Scale was used to assess the subjective memory complaint level, memory function, executive function and general self-efficacy level at three times (baseline, after intervention and follow up at 3 months). One-way repeated measures analysis of variance was used to compare the outcome variables of the two groups.There were no significant differences between the Baduanjin exercise and the control group at baseline in demographic, SMCQ, MoCA, and GDS-15. Compared to participants in the control group, participants in the Baduanjin group had a significantly improvement in memory function (F = 46.93, P < .00), executive function (F = 317.83, P < .00) and general self-efficacy (F = 38.72, P < .00) at the end of 12-week intervention period and after 3months follow-up period (P < .01). At the same time, participants in the Baduanjin group had a significantly greater decrease in subjective memory complaint scores at the end of 12-week intervention period and after 3months follow-up period (F = 24.53, P < 0.00).Baduanjin exercise appears to be a feasible and acceptable intervention to improve subjective memory complaint among older adults.


Assuntos
Autoavaliação Diagnóstica , Terapia por Exercício/normas , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Idoso , Terapia por Exercício/psicologia , Terapia por Exercício/estatística & dados numéricos , Feminino , Geriatria/métodos , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Qualidade de Vida/psicologia , Inquéritos e Questionários , Resultado do Tratamento
16.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 16(6): 967-74, 2010 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20946707

RESUMO

Clinical and experimental observation have shown that patients who confabulate, especially but not exclusively when provoked by specific questions, retrieve personal habits, repeated events or over-learned information and mistake them for actually experienced, specific, unique events. Accordingly, the aim of this study is to characterize and quantify the relative contribution of this type of confabulation, which we refer to as Habits Confabulation (HC), to confabulations produced by 10 mild Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients and 8 confabulating amnesics (CA) of various etiologies. On the Confabulation Battery (Dalla Barba, 1993a, Dalla Barba & Decaix, 2009), a set of questions involving the retrieval of various kinds of semantic and episodic information, patients produced a total of 424 confabulation. HC accounted for 42% and 62% of confabulations in AD patients and CA, respectively. This result indicates that, regardless the clinical diagnosis, the brain pathology or their lesion's site, confabulation largely reflects the individuals' tendency to consider habits, routines, and over-learned information as unique episodes. These results are discussed in the framework of the Memory Consciousness and Temporality Theory (Dalla Barba, 2002).


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/complicações , Amnésia/complicações , Classificação , Confusão/etiologia , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Análise de Variância , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Entrevista Psiquiátrica Padronizada , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Inquéritos e Questionários
17.
Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord ; 24(3): 248-55, 2010.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20473134

RESUMO

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a transitional state between normal cognitive functioning and dementia. A proposed MCI typology classifies individuals by the type and extent of cognitive impairment, yet few studies have characterized or compared these subtypes. Four hundred forty-seven women 65 years of age and older from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study were classified into the 4 MCI subgroups and a "no impairment" group and compared on clinical, sociodemographic, and health variables. A cognitive deficit in at least 1 domain was present in 82.1% of participants, with most (74.3%) having deficits in multiple cognitive domains. Only 4.3% had an isolated memory deficit, whereas 21.3% had an isolated nonmemory deficit. Of the 112 women who met all MCI criteria examined, the most common subtype was amnestic multidomain MCI (42.8%), followed by nonamnestic multiple domain MCI (26.7%), nonamnestic single domain (24.1%), and amnestic single domain MCI (6.3%). Subtypes were similar with respect to education, health status, smoking, depression, and prestudy and onstudy use of hormone therapy. Despite the attention it receives in the literature, amnestic MCI is the least common type highlighting the importance of identifying and characterizing other nonamnestic and multidomain subtypes. Further research is needed on the epidemiology of MCI subtypes, clinical and biologic differences between them, and rates for conversion to dementia.


Assuntos
Transtornos Cognitivos/fisiopatologia , Demência/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/fisiopatologia , Pós-Menopausa/psicologia , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Algoritmos , Transtornos Cognitivos/classificação , Transtornos Cognitivos/epidemiologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Demência/diagnóstico , Demência/epidemiologia , Demência/fisiopatologia , Demência/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Transtornos da Memória/epidemiologia , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Pós-Menopausa/fisiologia , Estados Unidos/epidemiologia
18.
Epilepsy Behav ; 18(3): 218-22, 2010 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20494621

RESUMO

Persons with epilepsy experience déjà vu phenomena with or without seizure recognition. Déjà vu experiences are also common mental phenomena in nonclinical individuals. The purpose of this study was to clarify two forms of déjà vu experiences in persons with epilepsy. Déjà vu experiences of 312 patients with epilepsy and 402 nonclinical individuals were evaluated using the Inventory of Déjà vu Experiences Assessment. In the patients with epilepsy, characteristics of déjà vu experiences with seizure recognition (SR form) were compared with those experiences with no seizure recognition (NSR form). The incidence (63.1%) of déjà vu experiences in patients with epilepsy was significantly lower than that (76.1%) of nonclinical individuals (chi(2)=14.2, P=0.000). Among the patients with epilepsy, 55.6% had the NSR form and 24.0% had the SR form. Those with the NSR form manifested fewer psychopathological characteristics than did those with the SR form. Patients tended to view the SR form more negatively (i.e., frightened, uncomfortable, or disturbed) than the NSR form. The NSR form was significantly associated with idiopathic generalized epilepsies, less frequent antiepileptic drug administration, and no mesial temporal sclerosis. Although there was a significant association between the frequency of the SR form and patients' habitual seizures, the frequency of the NSR form was not associated with the frequency of the patients' habitual seizures. Persons with epilepsy experience two forms of déjà vu which are differently associated with their seizure recognition.


Assuntos
Déjà Vu , Epilepsia/complicações , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/etiologia , Adolescente , Adulto , Fatores Etários , Idoso , Anticonvulsivantes/uso terapêutico , Eletroencefalografia , Epilepsia/classificação , Epilepsia/tratamento farmacológico , Feminino , Lateralidade Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Transtornos da Memória/tratamento farmacológico , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Estatísticas não Paramétricas , Adulto Jovem
19.
Int Psychogeriatr ; 22(3): 395-402, 2010 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20105352

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The concept of benign senescent forgetfulness (BSF) was introduced in 1958 by V. A. Kral, consultant neuropsychiatrist to the Montreal Hebrew Old People's and Sheltering Home (hereafter the Home). BSF was criticized and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) was adopted years later. In this paper I argue that a notion of MCI also originated with Kral. METHODS: A historical review was undertaken of the Canadian Jewish Congress archives in Montreal, Kral's biography and his publications, as well as relevant literature. A discussion was held with one of Kral's collaborators. RESULTS: In the mid-1950s the administration of the Home sought research-derived recommendations to improve residents' conditions. In an initial survey, based on a meticulous neuropsychiatric examination, Kral departed from contemporaneous psychiatric nosology and suggested other criteria in order to classify the home's population. He classified one of the five groups, consisting of subjects with mild memory dysfunction, as having BSF. In his next survey, Kral included psychological tests in addition to clinical assessment and focused on the elderly people who were well preserved (good personal and social functioning). This sample was subdivided into four subgroups, including BSF and incipient amnestic syndrome (IAS). The findings revealed: well-preserved aged people; normal (dull) level of general intelligence; subnormal performance of specific memory and perceptual/organization tests; and no significant signs of malignant amnestic syndrome. This construct is very similar to that of MCI, which is widely used today. CONCLUSIONS: The interaction between Kral and a particular home for the elderly at a particular moment in its history gave rise to the concept of BSF, and a notion of what we now call MCI emerged.


Assuntos
Envelhecimento/psicologia , Transtornos Cognitivos/diagnóstico , Inteligência , Transtornos da Memória/diagnóstico , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Transtornos Cognitivos/classificação , Transtornos Cognitivos/psicologia , Avaliação Geriátrica , Humanos , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Escalas de Graduação Psiquiátrica , Índice de Gravidade de Doença , Terminologia como Assunto
20.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 15(1): 14-37, 2010 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19753493

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Different types of confabulation or false memory can arise from brain disease. There are competing explanatory theories for the mechanisms underlying confabulation. Recent literature has attempted to relate the notion of delusion to that of confabulation. METHOD: A brief review of the literature relating to these ideas. RESULTS: The varieties of confabulation or false memory that can arise from brain disease are considered. The varieties of delusion and the contexts in which they arise are considered. Comparisons are made between the characteristics of spontaneous confabulation and those of delusional memory. CONCLUSION: It is suggested that global theories purporting to account for both confabulation and delusions, in whatever circumstances they arise, can have only limited explanatory power. On the other hand, there are resemblances between confabulation and delusional memory, and the similarities and differences between these phenomena deserve further empirical investigation.


Assuntos
Delusões/classificação , Transtornos da Memória/classificação , Repressão Psicológica , Delusões/psicologia , Humanos , Memória , Transtornos da Memória/psicologia , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA