Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 69
Filtrar
Mais filtros

Base de dados
País/Região como assunto
Tipo de documento
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Aggress Behav ; 50(3): e22146, 2024 05.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38623794

RESUMO

This research examines the notion of defensive gun ownership using the Dualistic Model of Passion. We hypothesized that an obsessive (vs. harmonious) passion for guns would be associated with a belief in a dangerous world (BDW). We expected this relationship to intensify in threatening contexts, leading to a more expansive view on defensive gun ownership. We tested this hypothesis across three threat contexts: a gun-control message (Study 1, N = 342), a live shooting simulation (Study 2, N = 398), and the aftermath of the Christchurch mass shootings (Study 3, N = 314). In the experimental Study 1, exposure to a gun-control message increased the intention to purchase guns among those with an obsessive passion (OP) for guns. Study 2 revealed that BDW mediated the relationship between OP and assertive modes of protection, the desire to purchase high-stopping-power guns, and anti-Black racial bias in a shooting task. Study 3 showed that knowledge of the Christchurch attack intensified the link between OP and BDW, leading to increased support for gun access, a willingness to act as a citizen-protector, and prejudice against Muslims. Comprehending these dynamics can assist policymakers in crafting messaging campaigns for firearm regulation and public safety measures that are more effective.


Assuntos
Armas de Fogo , Humanos , Propriedade , Intenção , Emoções , Comportamento Perigoso
2.
Appetite ; 186: 106508, 2023 07 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36924801

RESUMO

Although behavioral weight loss treatment can result in health benefits, the effects on weight loss are generally disappointing, with lost weight regained within a few years. Because these problems appear to be due to a failure to execute the strong weight control intentions formed during treatment, the use of implementation intentions is suggested to bridge that intention-behavior gap. In contrast to behavioral intentions, implementation intentions specify the situation in which the behavior should be enacted. Although implementation intentions have generally been very effective in increasing the likelihood of goal attainment for a wide range of behaviors, results with regard to eating have been mixed. Especially disappointing have been the findings of a large study that added implementation intentions to a clinical weight loss treatment and found no effects either on weight loss or on weight loss maintenance (Knäuper et al., 2018). I argue that this failure is due to the use of implementation intentions that target specific eating behaviors. According to the goal conflict model of eating, implementation intentions are most effective if they prime the weight control goal; implementation intentions targeting specific behaviors do not reliably do that. I review evidence that the implementation intention to think of dieting when confronted with palatable food items primes weight control thoughts. It also reduces eating of primed foods and even result in weight loss.


Assuntos
Comportamento Alimentar , Intenção , Humanos , Redução de Peso , Motivação , Alimentos
3.
Appetite ; 188: 106614, 2023 09 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37271254

RESUMO

There is a striking discrepancy in both U.S and the U.K data between obesity rates, which are increasing, and self-reported food consumption rates, which are decreasing. There are two possible explanations for this discrepancy, namely that the widely accepted energy balance interpretation of obesity is wrong or that food consumption data are somehow biased. In a comment entitled "Obesity-an unexplained epidemic", Mozaffarian (2022) challenged the Energy Balance Model (EBM) and argued for a need to replace it with a novel biological theory. This challenge is premature, because there are psychological explanations for this discrepancy, namely that individuals with overweight and obesity underreport their food consumption and that this tendency has increased in recent years. To support these hypotheses, U.S and U.K data are reviewed that used the Doubly Labelled Water method (DLW), which is the gold standard for estimating energy expenditure. Such studies find not only consistent evidence of underreporting, but also that the discrepancy between measured energy expenditure and reported calorie consumption increased over time. Two psychological explanations for this pattern are discussed.


Assuntos
Obesidade , Sobrepeso , Humanos , Obesidade/epidemiologia , Ingestão de Energia , Água , Metabolismo Energético
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(32): 19072-19079, 2020 08 11.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32719127

RESUMO

People may be sympathetic to violent extremism when it serves their own interests. Such support may manifest itself via biased recognition of hate crimes. Psychological surveys were conducted in the wakes of mass shootings in the United States, New Zealand, and the Netherlands (total n = 2,332), to test whether factors that typically predict endorsement of violent extremism also predict biased hate crime perceptions. Path analyses indicated a consistent pattern of motivated judgment: hate crime perceptions were directly biased by prejudicial attitudes and indirectly biased by an aggrieved sense of disempowerment and White/Christian nationalism. After the shooting at a synagogue in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, disempowerment-fueled anti-Semitism predicted lower perceptions that the gunman was motivated by hatred and prejudice (study 1). After the shootings that occurred at two mosques in Christchurch, New Zealand, disempowerment-fueled Islamoprejudice similarly predicted lower hate crime perceptions (study 2a). Conversely, after the tram shooting in Utrecht, Netherlands (which was perpetrated by a Turkish-born immigrant), disempowerment-fueled Islamoprejudice predicted higher hate crime perceptions (study 2b). Finally, after the Walmart shooting in El Paso, Texas, hate crime perceptions were specifically biased by an ethnonationalist view of Hispanic immigrants as a symbolic (rather than realistic) threat to America; that is, disempowered individuals deemphasized likely hate crimes due to symbolic concerns about cultural supremacy rather than material concerns about jobs or crime (study 3). Altogether, biased hate crime perceptions can be purposive and reveal supremacist sympathies.


Assuntos
Crime/psicologia , Emoções , Percepção Social , Atitude , Crime/estatística & dados numéricos , Ódio , Humanos , Preconceito/psicologia , Relações Raciais , Violência/psicologia
5.
Behav Brain Sci ; 41: e151, 2018 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31064559

RESUMO

We advocate that replications should be an integral part of the scientific discourse and provide insights about the conditions under which an effect occurs. By themselves, mere nonreplications are not informative about the "truth" of an effect. As a consequence, the mechanistic continuation of multilab replications should be replaced by diagnostic studies providing insights about the underlying causes and mechanisms.

6.
Br J Clin Psychol ; 54(2): 163-80, 2015 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25229192

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Rumination is a risk factor after bereavement, predicting higher concurrent and prospective symptom levels of complicated grief and depression in mourners. Research has shown that rumination may consist of adaptive and maladaptive subtypes, but there has been a paucity of research in this topic in the bereavement area. Therefore, we aimed to clarify whether functional and dysfunctional forms of rumination can be distinguished after loss. DESIGN: Two-hundred and forty-two adults, who lost a first-degree family member on average 10 months previously, filled out questionnaires at three time points with 6 months between each time point. METHODS: Multiple regression analyses, controlled for loss-related variables, neuroticism, and baseline symptoms, were run to examine associations of subtypes of depressive rumination (brooding, reflection) and grief rumination (rumination about injustice, meaning, reactions, relationships and counterfactual thinking) with concurrent and prospective symptom levels of complicated grief and depression. RESULTS: Overall, grief rumination explained more variance in symptom levels than depressive rumination. Other major findings were that grief rumination about injustice predicted higher concurrent and prospective symptom levels of complicated grief and higher prospective symptom levels of depression. In contrast, grief rumination about emotional reactions was related to prospective reductions in symptoms of complicated grief. Reflection was also associated with prospective reductions of complicated grief and depressive symptom levels. CONCLUSIONS: Results indicate that adaptive and maladaptive forms of ruminative thinking can be distinguished in bereaved individuals. Therapeutic interventions for complicated grief could potentially be improved by including techniques aimed at reducing maladaptive rumination and increasing adaptive rumination. PRACTITIONER POINTS: Clinical implications: Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination after loss can be distinguished. They are differentially associated with concurrent and prospective symptom levels of complicated grief and depression in mourners. Adaptive rumination after bereavement is characterized by repetitive, self-focused thinking aimed at understanding one's depressive and loss-related emotional reactions. Maladaptive rumination is characterized by repetitive, self-focused thinking about injustice to the self and making passive comparisons between the current situation (in which one has experienced a loss) and unrealized alternatives. Psychological interventions for complicated grief may be improved by adding therapeutic techniques aimed at reducing maladaptive rumination and increasing adaptive rumination. Cautions and limitations: This investigation relied exclusively on self-report measures. Conjugally bereaved women were overrepresented in the current sample. Complicated grief and depression levels in the current sample ranged from non-clinical to clinical. Effects may be more pronounced in a clinical sample.


Assuntos
Adaptação Psicológica , Luto , Depressão/psicologia , Transtorno Depressivo/psicologia , Resiliência Psicológica , Pensamento , Adulto , Idoso , Feminino , Pesar , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Análise de Regressão , Fatores de Risco , Autorrelato , Inquéritos e Questionários
7.
Appetite ; 78: 102-9, 2014 Jul.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24675683

RESUMO

Because eating behavior can take on an impulsive nature many people experience difficulty with dieting to lose weight. Therefore, an experiment was conducted to test the effectiveness of two interventions targeting impulsive processes of eating behavior to facilitate weight loss: Implementation intentions to remind people about dieting versus a go/no-go task to change impulses toward palatable foods. Dieters performed an online training program (four times in 4 weeks) in which they were randomly assigned to a 2 (implementation intention condition: dieting versus control) × 2 (go/no-go task condition: food versus control) design. They formed either dieting implementation intentions (e.g., If I open the fridge I will think of dieting!) or control implementation intentions. Furthermore, they received either a go/no-go task in which behavioral stop signals were presented upon presentation of palatable foods (food go/no-go task), or upon control stimuli. Participants' weight was measured in the laboratory before and after the intervention. Strength of participants' dieting goal and their Body Mass Index (BMI; as a proxy for impulsiveness toward food) were examined as moderators. Results showed that both dieting implementation intentions and the food go/no-go task facilitated weight loss. Moreover, dieting implementation intentions facilitated weight loss particularly among people with a strong current dieting goal, whereas the food go/no-go task facilitated weight loss independent of this factor. Instead, the food go/no-go task, but not formation of dieting implementation intentions, was primarily effective among dieters with a relatively high BMI. These results provide the first preliminary evidence that interventions aimed at targeting impulsive eating-related processes via the internet can facilitate weight loss.


Assuntos
Ingestão de Energia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Comportamento Impulsivo , Intenção , Internet , Sobrepeso/psicologia , Redução de Peso , Adolescente , Adulto , Terapia Comportamental , Índice de Massa Corporal , Peso Corporal , Sinais (Psicologia) , Dieta Redutora/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Sobrepeso/dietoterapia , Adulto Jovem
8.
Perspect Psychol Sci ; 19(3): 576-579, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38652782

RESUMO

Roberts and colleagues focus on two aspects of racial inequality in psychological research, namely an alleged underrepresentation of racial minorities and the effects attributed to this state of affairs. My comment focuses only on one aspect, namely the assumed consequences of the lack of diversity in subject populations. Representativeness of samples is essential in survey research or applied research that examines whether a particular intervention will work for a particular population. Representativeness or diversity is not necessary in theory-testing research, where we attempt to establish laws of causality. Because theories typically apply to all of humanity, all members of humanity (even American undergraduates) are suitable for assessing the validity of theoretical hypotheses. Admittedly, the assumption that a theory applies to all of humanity is also a hypothesis that can be tested. However, to test it, we need theoretical hypotheses about specific moderating variables. Supporting a theory with a racially diverse sample does not make conclusions more valid than support from a nondiverse sample. In fact, cause-effect conclusions based on a diverse sample might not be valid for any member of that sample.


Assuntos
Diversidade Cultural , Humanos , Teoria Psicológica , Minorias Étnicas e Raciais , Racismo , Psicologia , Projetos de Pesquisa
10.
Psychol Sci ; 24(4): 395-402, 2013 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23406609

RESUMO

Bereavement research has focused on individual rather than interdependent processes in coping with loss. Yet bereavement takes place in a social context, and relationship partners are likely to influence each other's grieving process. We examined the impact of a dynamic, interpersonal phenomenon, partner-oriented self-regulation (POSR): the avoidance of talking about loss and remaining strong in the partner's presence to protect the partner. Two hundred nineteen couples who had lost a child participated 6, 13, and 20 months after their loss. Consistent with predictions, results showed that one partner's POSR was associated not only with an increase in his or her own grief, but also with an increase in the other partner's grief. These relationships persisted over time: Self-reported and partner-reported POSR predicted later grief. These results are paradoxical: Although parents try to protect their partners through POSR, this effort has the opposite of the desired outcome. These findings underline the importance of further investigating interpersonal dynamics of coping with bereavement.


Assuntos
Pesar , Pais/psicologia , Cônjuges/psicologia , Adaptação Psicológica , Adulto , Idoso , Luto , Conflito Familiar/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
11.
Appetite ; 60(1): 81-84, 2013 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23017465

RESUMO

Restrained eaters with high scores on the Perceived Self-Regulatory Success in Dieting Scale (PSRS) are more successful than low scorers in regulating their food intake. According to the theory of temptation-elicited goal activation (Fishbach, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2003), they have become successful because, due to earlier repeated instances of successful self-control, they formed an associative link between temptations and thoughts of dieting. It is unclear, however, why they should have been more successful in earlier attempts at self-control than their unsuccessful counterparts. We examined whether trait impulsiveness plays a role by investigating the associations between dietary restraint, trait impulsiveness, and PSRS. Results showed that the interaction between dietary restraint and impulsiveness predicted dieting success: A lower level of impulsiveness was associated with greater dieting success among restrained eaters. These results suggest that restrained eaters who are less impulsive are more likely to become successful restrained eaters as identified with the PSRS.


Assuntos
Dieta Redutora/psicologia , Ingestão de Energia/fisiologia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Comportamento Impulsivo/psicologia , Adolescente , Índice de Massa Corporal , Peso Corporal , Feminino , Humanos , Comportamento Impulsivo/metabolismo , Masculino , Motivação , Análise de Regressão , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
12.
Swiss Med Wkly ; 153: 40026, 2023 01 06.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36652694

RESUMO

There is a great deal of empirical evidence that owning a firearm increases the risk of dying from suicide. Most suicides are impulsive. Nearly 50% of survivors of suicide attempts report that they took less than 10 minutes between the decision to die and their suicide attempt. The great majority of these suicide survivors never make another attempt and die of natural causes. Because nearly 90% of firearm suicide attempts have a deadly outcome, gun owners are unlikely to have such a second chance. These impulsive suicide attempts are typically carried out with the means at hand. Swiss men have much higher firearm suicide rates than men in other European countries and this excess is likely to be due to their easy access to guns, because army conscripts have to keep their guns at home. When the number of conscripts was nearly halved in 2003/4 as a result of the Swiss Army Reform XXI, the number of army-issued firearms was reduced by an estimated 20%. An analysis of suicide rates before and after the reform indicated that male (but not female) suicide rates decreased by 8%, with no evidence of substitution with other means of suicide. If the army would require that the remaining half of conscripts had to keep their weapons at their barracks rather than at home, a further decrease in male suicide rates could be expected.


Assuntos
Armas de Fogo , Humanos , Masculino , Suíça/epidemiologia , Propriedade , Tentativa de Suicídio , Europa (Continente)
13.
Appl Psychol Health Well Being ; 15(3): 825-845, 2023 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36345695

RESUMO

It is puzzling that a sizeable percentage of people refuse to get vaccinated against COVID-19. This study aimed to examine social psychological factors influencing their vaccine hesitancy. This longitudinal study traced a cohort of 2663 individuals in 25 countries from the time before COVID-19 vaccines became available (March 2020) to July 2021, when vaccination was widely available. Multilevel logistic regressions were used to examine determinants of actual COVID-19 vaccination behavior by July 2021, with country-level intercept as random effect. Of the 2663 participants, 2186 (82.1%) had been vaccinated by July 2021. Participants' attitude toward COVID-19 vaccines was the strongest predictor of both vaccination intention and subsequent vaccination behavior (p < .001). Perceived risk of getting infected and perceived personal disturbance of infection were also associated with higher likelihood of getting vaccinated (p < .001). However, religiosity, right-wing political orientation, conspiracy beliefs, and low trust in government regarding COVID-19 were negative predictors of vaccination intention and behavior (p < .05). Our findings highlight the importance of attitude toward COVID-19 vaccines and also suggest that certain life-long held convictions that predate the pandemic make people distrustful of their government and likely to accept conspiracy beliefs and therefore less likely to adopt the vaccination behavior.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Humanos , COVID-19/prevenção & controle , Vacinas contra COVID-19 , Estudos Longitudinais , Intenção , Vacinação
14.
Appetite ; 58(2): 593-6, 2012 Apr.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22138114

RESUMO

We examined whether exposure to cues of attractive food reduces effortful behavior toward healthy foods for restrained eaters. After manipulating food pre-exposure, we recorded handgrip force while presenting participants with pictures of healthy food objects. Because participants were led to expect that they could obtain each object (not specified beforehand) by squeezing the handgrip as forcefully as possible while the object was displayed on the screen, the recorded handgrip force constitutes a measure of spontaneous effortful behavior. Results show that restrained eaters, but not unrestrained eaters, displayed less forceful action toward healthy food objects (i.e., lower exertion of force) when pre-exposed to tempting food cues. No effects were found on palatability perceptions of the healthy foods. The results provide further insight into why restrained eaters have difficulties in maintaining a low-calorie diet in food-rich environments.


Assuntos
Dieta/psicologia , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Preferências Alimentares/fisiologia , Preferências Alimentares/psicologia , Alimentos , Adulto , Restrição Calórica , Dieta Redutora , Ingestão de Energia , Feminino , Força da Mão/fisiologia , Humanos , Comportamento Impulsivo/fisiopatologia , Masculino , Motivação , Percepção Visual
16.
Patterns (N Y) ; 3(4): 100482, 2022 Apr 08.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35282654

RESUMO

Before vaccines for coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) became available, a set of infection-prevention behaviors constituted the primary means to mitigate the virus spread. Our study aimed to identify important predictors of this set of behaviors. Whereas social and health psychological theories suggest a limited set of predictors, machine-learning analyses can identify correlates from a larger pool of candidate predictors. We used random forests to rank 115 candidate correlates of infection-prevention behavior in 56,072 participants across 28 countries, administered in March to May 2020. The machine-learning model predicted 52% of the variance in infection-prevention behavior in a separate test sample-exceeding the performance of psychological models of health behavior. Results indicated the two most important predictors related to individual-level injunctive norms. Illustrating how data-driven methods can complement theory, some of the most important predictors were not derived from theories of health behavior-and some theoretically derived predictors were relatively unimportant.

17.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 3824, 2022 03 09.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35264597

RESUMO

The present paper examines longitudinally how subjective perceptions about COVID-19, one's community, and the government predict adherence to public health measures to reduce the spread of the virus. Using an international survey (N = 3040), we test how infection risk perception, trust in the governmental response and communications about COVID-19, conspiracy beliefs, social norms on distancing, tightness of culture, and community punishment predict various containment-related attitudes and behavior. Autoregressive analyses indicate that, at the personal level, personal hygiene behavior was predicted by personal infection risk perception. At social level, social distancing behaviors such as abstaining from face-to-face contact were predicted by perceived social norms. Support for behavioral mandates was predicted by confidence in the government and cultural tightness, whereas support for anti-lockdown protests was predicted by (lower) perceived clarity of communication about the virus. Results are discussed in light of policy implications and creating effective interventions.


Assuntos
COVID-19/prevenção & controle , Fidelidade a Diretrizes , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Saúde Pública , Atitude , COVID-19/virologia , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , SARS-CoV-2 , Normas Sociais , Inquéritos e Questionários
18.
Appetite ; 56(3): 643-8, 2011 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21354452

RESUMO

We examined whether presentation of environmental cues that are associated with motor inhibition, i.e., fearful facial expressions, can be effective in controlling unintentionally evoked impulses toward rewarding food objects. Participants were presented with palatable foods or control objects. During presentation of the objects, facial expressions displaying fear, disgust, or neutral emotion were shortly presented. Results show that presentation of fearful facial expressions together with palatable foods slowed down subsequent responding to action probes, but only for participants who perceive palatable foods as highly rewarding and impulse-evoking, i.e., restrained eaters. Facial expressions of disgust did not show this effect. This finding suggests that unintentionally evoked motor impulses toward rewarding objects are inhibited upon presentation of a fear signal. The present research provides new insight on how emotional signals may be used to control impulsive responses toward palatable foods by the environment.


Assuntos
Medo/psicologia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Alimentos , Comportamento Impulsivo/psicologia , Inibição Psicológica , Recompensa , Sinais (Psicologia) , Expressão Facial , Medo/fisiologia , Comportamento Alimentar/fisiologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Estudantes/psicologia
19.
PLoS One ; 16(10): e0256740, 2021.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34669724

RESUMO

During the initial phase of the COVID-19 pandemic, U.S. conservative politicians and the media downplayed the risk of both contracting COVID-19 and the effectiveness of recommended health behaviors. Health behavior theories suggest perceived vulnerability to a health threat and perceived effectiveness of recommended health-protective behaviors determine motivation to follow recommendations. Accordingly, we predicted that-as a result of politicization of the pandemic-politically conservative Americans would be less likely to enact recommended health-protective behaviors. In two longitudinal studies of U.S. residents, political conservatism was inversely associated with perceived health risk and adoption of health-protective behaviors over time. The effects of political orientation on health-protective behaviors were mediated by perceived risk of infection, perceived severity of infection, and perceived effectiveness of the health-protective behaviors. In a global cross-national analysis, effects were stronger in the U.S. (N = 10,923) than in an international sample (total N = 51,986), highlighting the increased and overt politicization of health behaviors in the U.S.


Assuntos
COVID-19 , Comportamentos Relacionados com a Saúde , Motivação , Pandemias/prevenção & controle , Política , SARS-CoV-2 , Adolescente , Adulto , Idoso , COVID-19/epidemiologia , COVID-19/prevenção & controle , Estudos Transversais , Humanos , Estudos Longitudinais , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
20.
Psychol Sci ; 21(12): 1863-70, 2010 Dec.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21106885

RESUMO

Why do chronic dieters often violate their dieting goals? One possibility is that they experience stronger hedonic responses to tempting food than normal eaters do. We scrutinized hedonic processing in dieters and normal eaters (a) by manipulating food preexposure and (b) by assessing both immediate and delayed hedonic responses to tempting food with an adapted affect-misattribution procedure. Without food preexposure, dieters showed less positive hedonic responses than normal eaters (Study 1). When preexposed to tempting-food stimuli, however, dieters exhibited more positive delayed hedonic responses than normal eaters (Studies 1 and 2). Furthermore, delayed hedonic responding was meaningfully related to self-reported power of food and state cravings (Study 2). These findings suggest that dieters experience difficulties in down-regulating hedonic affect when in a "hot" state and that self-regulation research may benefit from a greater emphasis on temporal dynamics rather than static differences.


Assuntos
Alimentos , Prazer , Dieta Redutora/psicologia , Comportamento Alimentar/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Adulto Jovem
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA