Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 28
Filtrar
1.
Nat Rev Neurosci ; 22(9): 573-583, 2021 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34345018

RESUMEN

How does the brain encode information about the environment? Decades of research have led to the pervasive notion that the object-processing pathway in primate cortex consists of multiple areas that are each specialized to process different object categories (such as faces, bodies, hands, non-face objects and scenes). The anatomical consistency and modularity of these regions have been interpreted as evidence that these regions are innately specialized. Here, we propose that ventral-stream modules do not represent clusters of circuits that each evolved to process some specific object category particularly important for survival, but instead reflect the effects of experience on a domain-general architecture that evolved to be able to adapt, within a lifetime, to its particular environment. Furthermore, we propose that the mechanisms underlying the development of domains are both evolutionarily old and universal across cortex. Topographic maps are fundamental, governing the development of specializations across systems, providing a framework for brain organization.


Asunto(s)
Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Humanos , Macaca mulatta , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual/fisiología
2.
J Neurosci ; 44(40)2024 Oct 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39358020

RESUMEN

Most studies on the development of the visual system have focused on the mechanisms shaping early visual stages up to the level of primary visual cortex (V1). Much less is known about the development of the stages after V1 that handle the higher visual functions fundamental to everyday life. The standard model for the maturation of these areas is that it occurs sequentially, according to the positions of areas in the adult hierarchy. Yet, the existing literature reviewed here paints a different picture, one in which the adult configuration emerges through a sequence of unique network configurations that are not mere partial versions of the adult hierarchy. In addition to studying higher visual development per se to fill major gaps in knowledge, it will be crucial to adopt a network-level perspective in future investigations to unravel normal developmental mechanisms, identify vulnerabilities to developmental disorders, and eventually devise treatments for these disorders.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Visual , Vías Visuales , Humanos , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Corteza Visual/crecimiento & desarrollo , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Corteza Visual Primaria/fisiología
3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(51): 32667-32678, 2020 12 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33277435

RESUMEN

Primate brains typically have regions within the ventral visual stream that are selectively responsive to faces. In macaques, these face patches are located in similar parts of inferotemporal cortex across individuals although correspondence with particular anatomical features has not been reported previously. Here, using high-resolution functional and anatomical imaging, we show that small "bumps," or buried gyri, along the lower bank of the superior temporal sulcus are predictive of the location of face-selective regions. Recordings from implanted multielectrode arrays verified that these bumps contain face-selective neurons. These bumps were present in monkeys raised without seeing faces and that lack face patches, indicating that these anatomical landmarks are predictive of, but not sufficient for, the presence of face selectivity. These bumps are found across primate species that span taxonomy lines, indicating common evolutionary developmental mechanisms. The bumps emerge during fetal development in macaques, indicating that they arise from general developmental mechanisms that result in the regularity of cortical folding of the entire brain.


Asunto(s)
Cara/anatomía & histología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Animales , Electrodos Implantados , Femenino , Macaca mulatta , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Neuroimagen/métodos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Lóbulo Temporal/embriología , Tomografía Computarizada por Rayos X
4.
Cereb Cortex ; 31(1): 48-61, 2021 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32954410

RESUMEN

The evolution and development of anatomical-functional relationships in the cerebral cortex is of major interest in neuroscience. Here, we leveraged the fact that a functional region selective for visual scenes is located within a sulcus in the medial ventral temporal cortex (VTC) in both humans and macaques to examine the relationship between sulcal depth and place selectivity in the medial VTC across species and age groups. To do so, we acquired anatomical and functional magnetic resonance imaging scans in 9 macaques, 26 human children, and 28 human adults. Our results revealed a strong structural-functional coupling between sulcal depth and place selectivity across age groups and species in which selectivity was strongest near the deepest sulcal point (the sulcal pit). Interestingly, this coupling between sulcal depth and place selectivity strengthens from childhood to adulthood in humans. Morphological analyses suggest that the stabilization of sulcal-functional coupling in adulthood may be due to sulcal deepening and areal expansion with age as well as developmental differences in cortical curvature at the pial, but not the white matter surfaces. Our results implicate sulcal features as functional landmarks in high-level visual cortex and highlight that sulcal-functional relationships in the medial VTC are preserved between macaques and humans despite differences in cortical folding.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/anatomía & histología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/anatomía & histología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Envejecimiento/psicología , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Macaca mulatta , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
5.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 116(49): 24861-24871, 2019 12 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31732670

RESUMEN

Topographic sensory maps are a prominent feature of the adult primate brain. Here, we asked whether topographic representations of the body are present at birth. Using functional MRI (fMRI), we find that the newborn somatomotor system, spanning frontoparietal cortex and subcortex, comprises multiple topographic representations of the body. The organization of these large-scale body maps was indistinguishable from those in older monkeys. Finer-scale differentiation of individual fingers increased over the first 2 y, suggesting that topographic representations are refined during early development. Last, we found that somatomotor representations were unchanged in 2 visually impaired monkeys who relied on touch for interacting with their environment, demonstrating that massive shifts in early sensory experience in an otherwise anatomically intact brain are insufficient for driving cross-modal plasticity. We propose that a topographic scaffolding is present at birth that both directs and constrains experience-driven modifications throughout somatosensory and motor systems.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral/anatomía & histología , Macaca mulatta/anatomía & histología , Corteza Somatosensorial/anatomía & histología , Animales , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Cerebral/crecimiento & desarrollo , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Femenino , Dedos/fisiología , Macaca mulatta/crecimiento & desarrollo , Macaca mulatta/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Neuronas Motoras , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Plasticidad Neuronal , Células Receptoras Sensoriales , Corteza Somatosensorial/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Somatosensorial/crecimiento & desarrollo , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología
6.
J Neurosci ; 37(31): 7373-7389, 2017 08 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28674177

RESUMEN

Primates have specialized domains in inferior temporal (IT) cortex that are responsive to particular image categories. Though IT traditionally has been regarded as lacking retinotopy, several recent studies in monkeys have shown that retinotopic maps extend to face patches along the lower bank of the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and neighboring regions of IT cortex. Here, we used fMRI to map the retinotopic organization of medial ventral temporal cortex in four monkeys (2 male and 2 female). We confirm the presence of visual field maps within and around the lower bank of the STS and extend these prior findings to scene-selective cortex in the ventral-most regions of IT. Within the occipitotemporal sulcus (OTS), we identified two retinotopic areas, OTS1 and OTS2. The polar angle representation of OTS2 was a mirror reversal of the OTS1 representation. These regions contained representations of the contralateral periphery and were selectively active for scene versus face, body, or object images. The extent of this retinotopy parallels that in humans and shows that the organization of the scene network is preserved across primate species. In addition retinotopic maps were identified in dorsal extrastriate, posterior parietal, and frontal cortex as well as the thalamus, including both the lateral geniculate nucleus and pulvinar. Together, it appears that most, if not all, of the macaque visual system contains organized representations of visual space.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Primates have specialized domains in inferior temporal (IT) cortex that are responsive to particular image categories. Though retinotopic maps are considered a fundamental organizing principle of posterior visual cortex, IT traditionally has been regarded as lacking retinotopy. Recent imaging studies have demonstrated the presence of several visual field maps within the lateral IT. Using neuroimaging, we found multiple representations of visual space within ventral IT cortex of macaques that included scene-selective cortex. Scene domains were biased toward the peripheral visual field. These data demonstrate the prevalence of visual field maps throughout the primate visual system, including late stages in the ventral visual hierarchy, and support the idea that domains representing different categories are biased toward different parts of the visual field.


Asunto(s)
Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Neuronas Retinianas/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología
7.
J Vis ; 18(13): 23, 2018 12 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30593068

RESUMEN

About a quarter of human cerebral cortex is dedicated mainly to visual processing. The large-scale spatial organization of visual cortex can be measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while subjects view spatially modulated visual stimuli, also known as "retinotopic mapping." One of the datasets collected by the Human Connectome Project involved ultrahigh-field (7 Tesla) fMRI retinotopic mapping in 181 healthy young adults (1.6-mm resolution), yielding the largest freely available collection of retinotopy data. Here, we describe the experimental paradigm and the results of model-based analysis of the fMRI data. These results provide estimates of population receptive field position and size. Our analyses include both results from individual subjects as well as results obtained by averaging fMRI time series across subjects at each cortical and subcortical location and then fitting models. Both the group-average and individual-subject results reveal robust signals across much of the brain, including occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal cortex as well as subcortical areas. The group-average results agree well with previously published parcellations of visual areas. In addition, split-half analyses show strong within-subject reliability, further demonstrating the high quality of the data. We make publicly available the analysis results for individual subjects and the group average, as well as associated stimuli and analysis code. These resources provide an opportunity for studying fine-scale individual variability in cortical and subcortical organization and the properties of high-resolution fMRI. In addition, they provide a set of observations that can be compared with other Human Connectome Project measures acquired in these same participants.


Asunto(s)
Conectoma , Conjuntos de Datos como Asunto , Retina/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Adulto Joven
8.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 111(13): 5012-7, 2014 Apr 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24639542

RESUMEN

This study tested the possible relationship between reported visual awareness ("I see a visual stimulus in front of me") and the social attribution of awareness to someone else ("That person is aware of an object next to him"). Subjects were tested in two steps. First, in an fMRI experiment, subjects were asked to attribute states of awareness to a cartoon face. Activity associated with this task was found bilaterally within the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) among other areas. Second, the TPJ was transiently disrupted using single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). When the TMS was targeted to the same cortical sites that had become active during the social attribution task, the subjects showed symptoms of visual neglect in that their detection of visual stimuli was significantly affected. In control trials, when TMS was targeted to nearby cortical sites that had not become active during the social attribution task, no significant effect on visual detection was found. These results suggest that there may be at least some partial overlap in brain mechanisms that participate in the social attribution of sensory awareness to other people and in attributing sensory awareness to oneself.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Conducta Social , Adolescente , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Factores de Tiempo , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
J Neurosci ; 35(27): 9848-71, 2015 Jul 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26156987

RESUMEN

The pulvinar is the largest nucleus in the primate thalamus and contains extensive, reciprocal connections with visual cortex. Although the anatomical and functional organization of the pulvinar has been extensively studied in old and new world monkeys, little is known about the organization of the human pulvinar. Using high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging at 3 T, we identified two visual field maps within the ventral pulvinar, referred to as vPul1 and vPul2. Both maps contain an inversion of contralateral visual space with the upper visual field represented ventrally and the lower visual field represented dorsally. vPul1 and vPul2 border each other at the vertical meridian and share a representation of foveal space with iso-eccentricity lines extending across areal borders. Additional, coarse representations of contralateral visual space were identified within ventral medial and dorsal lateral portions of the pulvinar. Connectivity analyses on functional and diffusion imaging data revealed a strong distinction in thalamocortical connectivity between the dorsal and ventral pulvinar. The two maps in the ventral pulvinar were most strongly connected with early and extrastriate visual areas. Given the shared eccentricity representation and similarity in cortical connectivity, we propose that these two maps form a distinct visual field map cluster and perform related functions. The dorsal pulvinar was most strongly connected with parietal and frontal areas. The functional and anatomical organization observed within the human pulvinar was similar to the organization of the pulvinar in other primate species. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: The anatomical organization and basic response properties of the visual pulvinar have been extensively studied in nonhuman primates. Yet, relatively little is known about the functional and anatomical organization of the human pulvinar. Using neuroimaging, we found multiple representations of visual space within the ventral human pulvinar and extensive topographically organized connectivity with visual cortex. This organization is similar to other nonhuman primates and provides additional support that the general organization of the pulvinar is consistent across the primate phylogenetic tree. These results suggest that the human pulvinar, like other primates, is well positioned to regulate corticocortical communication.


Asunto(s)
Pulvinar/irrigación sanguínea , Pulvinar/fisiología , Corteza Visual/irrigación sanguínea , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/irrigación sanguínea , Adulto , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Imagen de Difusión por Resonancia Magnética , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa , Filogenia , Psicofísica , Descanso , Movimientos Sacádicos , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 25(10): 3911-31, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25452571

RESUMEN

The human visual system contains an array of topographically organized regions. Identifying these regions in individual subjects is a powerful approach to group-level statistical analysis, but this is not always feasible. We addressed this limitation by generating probabilistic maps of visual topographic areas in 2 standardized spaces suitable for use with adult human brains. Using standard fMRI paradigms, we identified 25 topographic maps in a large population of individual subjects (N = 53) and transformed them into either a surface- or volume-based standardized space. Here, we provide a quantitative characterization of the inter-subject variability within and across visual regions, including the likelihood that a given point would be classified as a part of any region (full probability map) and the most probable region for any given point (maximum probability map). By evaluating the topographic organization across the whole of visual cortex, we provide new information about the organization of individual visual field maps and large-scale biases in visual field coverage. Finally, we validate each atlas for use with independent subjects. Overall, the probabilistic atlases quantify the variability of topographic representations in human cortex and provide a useful reference for comparing data across studies that can be transformed into these standard spaces.


Asunto(s)
Atlas como Asunto , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Funciones de Verosimilitud , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Probabilidad , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
11.
Cell Rep ; 43(9): 114732, 2024 Sep 24.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39269905

RESUMEN

An interconnected group of cortical regions distributed across the primate inferotemporal cortex forms a network critical for face perception. Understanding the microarchitecture of this face network can refine mechanistic accounts of how individual areas function and interact to support visual perception. To address this, we acquire a unique dataset in macaque monkeys combining fMRI to localize face patches in vivo and then ex vivo histology to resolve their histo-architecture across cortical depths in the same individuals. Our findings reveal that face patches differ based on cytochrome oxidase (CO) and, to a lesser extent, myelin staining, with the middle lateral (ML) face patch exhibiting pronounced CO staining. Histo-architectonic differences are less pronounced when using probabilistic definitions of face patches, underscoring the importance of precision mapping integrating in vivo and ex vivo measurements in the same individuals. This study indicates that the macaque face patch network is composed of architectonically distinct components.


Asunto(s)
Macaca mulatta , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Lóbulo Temporal , Animales , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Masculino , Cara , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Femenino , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Complejo IV de Transporte de Electrones/metabolismo
12.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Jun 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38915591

RESUMEN

Human cortical development follows a sensorimotor-to-association sequence during childhood and adolescence1-6. The brain's capacity to enact this sequence over decades indicates that it relies on intrinsic mechanisms to regulate inter-regional differences in the timing of cortical maturation, yet regulators of human developmental chronology are not well understood. Given evidence from animal models that thalamic axons modulate windows of cortical plasticity7-12, here we evaluate the overarching hypothesis that structural connections between the thalamus and cortex help to coordinate cortical maturational heterochronicity during youth. We first introduce, cortically annotate, and anatomically validate a new atlas of human thalamocortical connections using diffusion tractography. By applying this atlas to three independent youth datasets (ages 8-23 years; total N = 2,676), we reproducibly demonstrate that thalamocortical connections develop along a maturational gradient that aligns with the cortex's sensorimotor-association axis. Associative cortical regions with thalamic connections that take longest to mature exhibit protracted expression of neurochemical, structural, and functional markers indicative of higher circuit plasticity as well as heightened environmental sensitivity. This work highlights a central role for the thalamus in the orchestration of hierarchically organized and environmentally sensitive windows of cortical developmental malleability.

13.
J Neurosci ; 31(6): 2064-78, 2011 Feb 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21307244

RESUMEN

Macaque anatomy and physiology studies have revealed multiple visual areas in posterior parietal cortex (PPC). While many response properties of PPC neurons have been probed, little is known about PPC's large-scale functional topography-specifically related to visuotopic organization. Using high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging at 3 T with a phase-encoded retinotopic mapping paradigm in the awake macaque, a large-scale visuotopic organization along lateral portions of PPC anterior to area V3a and extending into the lateral intraparietal sulcus (LIP) was found. We identify two new visual field maps anterior to V3a within caudal PPC, referred to as caudal intraparietal-1 (CIP-1) and CIP-2. The polar angle representation in CIP-1 extends from regions near the upper vertical meridian (that is the shared border with V3a and dorsal prelunate) to those within the lower visual field (that is the shared border with CIP-2). The polar angle representation in CIP-2 is a mirror reversal of the CIP-1 representation. CIP-1 and CIP-2 share a representation of central space on the lateral border. Anterior to CIP-2, a third polar angle representation was found within LIP, referred to as visuotopic LIP. The polar angle representation in LIP extends from regions near the upper vertical meridian (that is the shared border with CIP-2) to those near the lower vertical meridian. Representations of central visual space were identified within dorsal portions of LIP with peripheral representations in ventral portions. We also consider the topographic large-scale organization found within macaque PPC relative to that observed in human PPC.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Lóbulo Parietal/irrigación sanguínea , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Lateralidad Funcional , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador/métodos , Macaca fascicularis , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Lóbulo Parietal/anatomía & histología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Vías Visuales/irrigación sanguínea , Vías Visuales/fisiología
14.
Brain Struct Funct ; 227(4): 1227-1245, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34921348

RESUMEN

Primate cerebral cortex is highly convoluted with much of the cortical surface buried in sulcal folds. The origins of cortical folding and its functional relevance have been a major focus of systems and cognitive neuroscience, especially when considering stereotyped patterns of cortical folding that are shared across individuals within a primate species and across multiple species. However, foundational questions regarding organizing principles shared across species remain unanswered. Taking a cross-species comparative approach with a careful consideration of historical observations, we investigate cortical folding relative to primary visual cortex (area V1). We identify two macroanatomical structures-the retrocalcarine and external calcarine sulci-in 24 humans and 6 macaque monkeys. We show that within species, these sulci are identifiable in all individuals, fall on a similar part of the V1 retinotopic map, and thus, serve as anatomical landmarks predictive of functional organization. Yet, across species, the underlying eccentricity representations corresponding to these macroanatomical structures differ strikingly across humans and macaques. Thus, the correspondence between retinotopic representation and cortical folding for an evolutionarily old structure like V1 is species-specific and suggests potential differences in developmental and experiential constraints across primates.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Visual , Animales , Mapeo Encefálico , Humanos , Macaca
15.
Neuron ; 109(16): 2616-2626.e6, 2021 08 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34228960

RESUMEN

Vision develops rapidly during infancy, yet how visual cortex is organized during this period is unclear. In particular, it is unknown whether functional maps that organize the mature adult visual cortex are present in the infant striate and extrastriate cortex. Here, we test the functional maturity of infant visual cortex by performing retinotopic mapping with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Infants aged 5-23 months had retinotopic maps, with alternating preferences for vertical and horizontal meridians indicating the boundaries of visual areas V1 to V4 and an orthogonal gradient of preferences from high to low spatial frequencies. The presence of multiple visual maps throughout visual cortex in infants indicates a greater maturity of extrastriate cortex than previously appreciated. The areas showed subtle age-related fine-tuning, suggesting that early maturation undergoes continued refinement. This early maturation of area boundaries and tuning may scaffold subsequent developmental changes.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Corteza Visual/crecimiento & desarrollo , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Vías Visuales/crecimiento & desarrollo , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
16.
J Neurosci ; 29(34): 10638-52, 2009 Aug 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19710316

RESUMEN

Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies have shown that human ventral visual cortex anterior to human visual area V4 contains two visual field maps, VO-1 and VO-2, that together form the ventral occipital (VO) cluster (Brewer et al., 2005). This cluster is characterized by common functional response properties and responds preferentially to color and object stimuli. Here, we confirm the topographic and functional characteristics of the VO cluster and describe two new visual field maps that are located anterior to VO-2 extending across the collateral sulcus into the posterior parahippocampal cortex (PHC). We refer to these visual field maps as parahippocampal areas PHC-1 and PHC-2. Each PHC map contains a topographic representation of contralateral visual space. The polar angle representation in PHC-1 extends from regions near the lower vertical meridian (that is the shared border with VO-2) to those close to the upper vertical meridian (that is the shared border with PHC-2). The polar angle representation in PHC-2 is a mirror reversal of the PHC-1 representation. PHC-1 and PHC-2 share a foveal representation and show a strong bias toward representations of peripheral eccentricities. Both the foveal and peripheral representations of PHC-1 and PHC-2 respond more strongly to scenes than to objects or faces, with greater scene preference in PHC-2 than PHC-1. Importantly, both areas heavily overlap with the functionally defined parahippocampal place area. Our results suggest that ventral visual cortex can be subdivided on the basis of topographic criteria into a greater number of discrete maps than previously thought.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Atención/fisiología , Percepción de Color/fisiología , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicofísica , Corteza Visual/anatomía & histología , Corteza Visual/irrigación sanguínea , Vías Visuales/irrigación sanguínea , Adulto Joven
17.
Elife ; 92020 06 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32519949

RESUMEN

Despite evidence that context promotes the visual recognition of objects, decades of research have led to the pervasive notion that the object processing pathway in primate cortex consists of multiple areas that each process the intrinsic features of a few particular categories (e.g. faces, bodies, hands, objects, and scenes). Here we report that such category-selective neurons do not in fact code individual categories in isolation but are also sensitive to object relationships that reflect statistical regularities of the experienced environment. We show by direct neuronal recording that face-selective neurons respond not just to an image of a face, but also to parts of an image where contextual cues-for example a body-indicate a face ought to be, even if what is there is not a face.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Animales , Corteza Cerebral/citología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Cara/fisiología , Macaca , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Estimulación Luminosa
18.
Annu Rev Vis Sci ; 5: 341-372, 2019 09 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31226011

RESUMEN

Our assignment was to review the development of the face-processing network, an assignment that carries the presupposition that a face-specific developmental program exists. We hope to cast some doubt on this assumption and instead argue that the development of face processing is guided by the same ubiquitous rules that guide the development of cortex in general.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/crecimiento & desarrollo , Corteza Visual/crecimiento & desarrollo , Animales , Humanos , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología
19.
Nat Commun ; 10(1): 1443, 2019 03 26.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30914653

RESUMEN

The original version of this Article contained an error in the author affiliations. Affiliation 3 incorrectly read 'Department of Biological and Brain Sciences, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21218, USA'.

20.
Neuropsychologia ; 128: 150-165, 2019 05.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29753019

RESUMEN

Patients with injury to early visual cortex or its inputs can display the Riddoch phenomenon: preserved awareness for moving but not stationary stimuli. We provide a detailed case report of a patient with the Riddoch phenomenon, MC. MC has extensive bilateral lesions to occipitotemporal cortex that include most early visual cortex and complete blindness in visual field perimetry testing with static targets. Nevertheless, she shows a remarkably robust preserved ability to perceive motion, enabling her to navigate through cluttered environments and perform actions like catching moving balls. Comparisons of MC's structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data to a probabilistic atlas based on controls reveals that MC's lesions encompass the posterior, lateral, and ventral early visual cortex bilaterally (V1, V2, V3A/B, LO1/2, TO1/2, hV4 and VO1 in both hemispheres) as well as more extensive damage to right parietal (inferior parietal lobule) and left ventral occipitotemporal cortex (VO1, PHC1/2). She shows some sparing of anterior occipital cortex, which may account for her ability to see moving targets beyond ~15 degrees eccentricity during perimetry. Most strikingly, functional and structural MRI revealed robust and reliable spared functionality of the middle temporal motion complex (MT+) bilaterally. Moreover, consistent with her preserved ability to discriminate motion direction in psychophysical testing, MC also shows direction-selective adaptation in MT+. A variety of tests did not enable us to discern whether input to MT+ was driven by her spared anterior occipital cortex or subcortical inputs. Nevertheless, MC shows rich motion perception despite profoundly impaired static and form vision, combined with clear preservation of activation in MT+, thus supporting the role of MT+ in the Riddoch phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Ceguera Cortical/diagnóstico por imagen , Ceguera Cortical/psicología , Percepción de Movimiento , Corteza Visual/patología , Mapeo Encefálico , Infarto Cerebral/patología , Infarto Cerebral/psicología , Sensibilidad de Contraste , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Persona de Mediana Edad , Neuroimagen , Psicofísica , Percepción Visual
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA