Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 34
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Neuroimage ; 285: 120479, 2024 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38040399

RESUMEN

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in behaving monkeys has a strong potential to bridge the gap between human neuroimaging and primate neurophysiology. In monkey fMRI, to restrain head movements, researchers usually surgically implant a plastic head-post on the skull. Although time-proven to be effective, this technique could create burdens for animals, including a risk of infection and discomfort. Furthermore, the presence of extraneous objects on the skull, such as bone screws and dental cement, adversely affects signals near the cortical surface. These side effects are undesirable in terms of both the practical aspect of efficient data collection and the spirit of "refinement" from the 3R's. Here, we demonstrate that a completely non-invasive fMRI scan in awake monkeys is possible by using a plastic head mask made to fit the skull of individual animals. In all of the three monkeys tested, longitudinal, quantitative assessment of head movements showed that the plastic mask has effectively suppressed head movements, and we were able to obtain reliable retinotopic BOLD signals in a standard retinotopic mapping task. The present, easy-to-make plastic mask has a strong potential to simplify fMRI experiments in awake monkeys, while giving data that is as good as or even better quality than that obtained with the conventional head-post method.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Animales , Humanos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Haplorrinos , Cabeza/fisiología , Movimientos de la Cabeza
2.
PLoS Biol ; 17(3): e2006405, 2019 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30925163

RESUMEN

Electrophysiological evidence suggested primarily the involvement of the middle temporal (MT) area in depth cue integration in macaques, as opposed to human imaging data pinpointing area V3B/kinetic occipital area (V3B/KO). To clarify this conundrum, we decoded monkey functional MRI (fMRI) responses evoked by stimuli signaling near or far depths defined by binocular disparity, relative motion, and their combination, and we compared results with those from an identical experiment previously performed in humans. Responses in macaque area MT are more discriminable when two cues concurrently signal depth, and information provided by one cue is diagnostic of depth indicated by the other. This suggests that monkey area MT computes fusion of disparity and motion depth signals, exactly as shown for human area V3B/KO. Hence, these data reconcile previously reported discrepancies between depth processing in human and monkey by showing the involvement of the dorsal stream in depth cue integration using the same technique, despite the engagement of different regions.


Asunto(s)
Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Neuronas/metabolismo , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Electrofisiología , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Compuestos Férricos/química , Haplorrinos , Humanos , Ratones Noqueados , Nanopartículas/química , Neuronas/citología , Máquina de Vectores de Soporte , Percepción Visual/fisiología
3.
Neuroimage ; 236: 118212, 2021 08 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34082117

RESUMEN

Olfaction could prove to be an early marker of neurodegenerative diseases, including Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases. To use olfaction for disease diagnosis, elucidating the standard olfactory functions in healthy humans is necessary. However, the olfactory function in the human brain is less frequently assessed because of methodological difficulties associated with olfactory-related cerebral areas. Using ultra-high fields (UHF), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with high spatial resolution and sensitivity may allow for the measurement of activation in the cerebral areas. This study aimed to apply 7-Tesla fMRI to assess olfactory function in the human brain by exposing individuals to four different odorants for 8 s. We found that olfactory stimulation mainly activated the piriform and orbitofrontal cortex in addition to the amygdala. Among these regions, univariate fMRI analysis indicated that subjective odor intensity significantly correlated with the averaged fMRI signals in the piriform cortex but not with subjective hedonic tone in any region. In contrast, multivariate fMRI analysis showed that subjective hedonic tone could be discriminated from the fMRI response patterns in the posterior orbitofrontal cortex. Thus, the piriform cortex is mainly associated with subjective odor intensity, whereas the posterior orbitofrontal cortex are involved in the discrimination of the subjective hedonic tone of the odorant. UHF-fMRI may be useful for assessing olfactory function in the human brain.


Asunto(s)
Amígdala del Cerebelo/fisiología , Percepción Olfatoria/fisiología , Corteza Piriforme/fisiología , Corteza Prefrontal/fisiología , Adulto , Amígdala del Cerebelo/diagnóstico por imagen , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Estimulación Física , Corteza Piriforme/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Prefrontal/diagnóstico por imagen , Adulto Joven
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 32(2): 338-352, 2020 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31633464

RESUMEN

Using behavioral and fMRI paradigms, we asked how the physical plausibility of complex 3-D objects, as defined by the object's congruence with 3-D Euclidean geometry, affects behavioral thresholds and neural responses to depth information. Stimuli were disparity-defined geometric objects rendered as random dot stereograms, presented in plausible and implausible variations. In the behavior experiment, observers were asked to complete (1) a noise-based depth task that involved judging the depth position of a target embedded in noise and (2) a fine depth judgment task that involved discriminating the nearer of two consecutively presented targets. Interestingly, results indicated greater behavioral sensitivities of depth judgments for implausible versus plausible objects across both tasks. In the fMRI experiment, we measured fMRI responses concurrently with behavioral depth responses. Although univariate responses for depth judgments were largely similar across cortex regardless of object plausibility, multivariate representations for plausible and implausible objects were notably distinguishable along depth-relevant intermediate regions V3 and V3A, in addition to object-relevant LOC. Our data indicate significant modulations of both behavioral judgments of and neural responses to depth by object context. We conjecture that disparity mechanisms interact dynamically with the object recognition problem in the visual system such that disparity computations are adjusted based on object familiarity.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Adulto , Corteza Cerebral/diagnóstico por imagen , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Corteza Visual/diagnóstico por imagen , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
5.
Neuroimage ; 174: 87-96, 2018 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29524623

RESUMEN

Using fMRI and multivariate analyses we sought to understand the neural representations of articulated body shape and local kinematics in biological motion. We show that in addition to a cortical network that includes areas identified previously for biological motion perception, including the posterior superior temporal sulcus, inferior frontal gyrus, and ventral body areas, the ventral lateral nucleus, a presumably motoric thalamic area is sensitive to both form and kinematic information in biological motion. Our findings suggest that biological motion perception is not achieved as an end-point of segregated cortical form and motion networks as often suggested, but instead involves earlier parts in the visual system including a subcortical network.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Tálamo/fisiología , Adulto , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Adulto Joven
6.
PLoS Biol ; 13(8): e1002231, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26274866

RESUMEN

Animals are constantly exposed to the time-varying visual world. Because visual perception is modulated by immediately prior visual experience, visual cortical neurons may register recent visual history into a specific form of offline activity and link it to later visual input. To examine how preceding visual inputs interact with upcoming information at the single neuron level, we designed a simple stimulation protocol in which a brief, orientated flashing stimulus was subsequently coupled to visual stimuli with identical or different features. Using in vivo whole-cell patch-clamp recording and functional two-photon calcium imaging from the primary visual cortex (V1) of awake mice, we discovered that a flash of sinusoidal grating per se induces an early, transient activation as well as a long-delayed reactivation in V1 neurons. This late response, which started hundreds of milliseconds after the flash and persisted for approximately 2 s, was also observed in human V1 electroencephalogram. When another drifting grating stimulus arrived during the late response, the V1 neurons exhibited a sublinear, but apparently increased response, especially to the same grating orientation. In behavioral tests of mice and humans, the flashing stimulation enhanced the detection power of the identically orientated visual stimulation only when the second stimulation was presented during the time window of the late response. Therefore, V1 late responses likely provide a neural basis for admixing temporally separated stimuli and extracting identical features in time-varying visual environments.


Asunto(s)
Neocórtex/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Animales , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Neuronas/fisiología , Técnicas de Placa-Clamp , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos
7.
J Neurosci ; 36(46): 11727-11738, 2016 11 16.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27852780

RESUMEN

The primary visual cortex exhibits a late, long response with a latency of >300 ms and an immediate early response that occurs ∼100 ms after a visual stimulus. The late response is thought to contribute to visual functions such as sensory perception, iconic memory, working memory, and forming connections between temporally separated stimuli. However, how the visual late response is generated and organized is not completely understood. In the mouse primary visual cortex in vivo, we isolated long-delayed responses by using a brief light-flash stimulus for which the stimulus late response occurred long after the stimulus offset and was not contaminated by the instantaneous response evoked by the stimulus. Using whole-cell patch-clamp recordings, we demonstrated that the late rebound response was shaped by a net-balanced increase in excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances, whereas transient imbalances were caused by intermittent inhibitory barrage. In contrast to the common assumption that the neocortical late response reflects a feedback signal from the downstream higher-order cortical areas, our pharmacological and optogenetic analyses demonstrated that the late responses likely have a thalamic origin. Therefore, the late component of a sensory-evoked cortical response should be interpreted with caution. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: The long-delayed responses of neocortical neurons are thought to arise from cortical feedback activity that is related to sensory perception and cognition. The mechanism of neocortical late responses was investigated using multiple electrophysiological techniques and the findings indicate that it actually arises from the thalamus. In addition, during the late response, excitation and inhibition are balanced, but inhibition is dominant in patterning action potentials.


Asunto(s)
Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Corteza Visual/efectos de la radiación , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Animales , Conectoma/métodos , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/efectos de la radiación , Inhibición Neural/efectos de la radiación , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/efectos de la radiación , Percepción Visual/efectos de la radiación
8.
J Neurosci ; 35(27): 9823-35, 2015 Jul 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26156985

RESUMEN

The brain's skill in estimating the 3-D orientation of viewed surfaces supports a range of behaviors, from placing an object on a nearby table, to planning the best route when hill walking. This ability relies on integrating depth signals across extensive regions of space that exceed the receptive fields of early sensory neurons. Although hierarchical selection and pooling is central to understanding of the ventral visual pathway, the successive operations in the dorsal stream are poorly understood. Here we use computational modeling of human fMRI signals to probe the computations that extract 3-D surface orientation from binocular disparity. To understand how representations evolve across the hierarchy, we developed an inference approach using a series of generative models to explain the empirical fMRI data in different cortical areas. Specifically, we simulated the responses of candidate visual processing algorithms and tested how well they explained fMRI responses. Thereby we demonstrate a hierarchical refinement of visual representations moving from the representation of edges and figure-ground segmentation (V1, V2) to spatially extensive disparity gradients in V3A. We show that responses in V3A are little affected by low-level image covariates, and have a partial tolerance to the overall depth position. Finally, we show that responses in V3A parallel perceptual judgments of slant. This reveals a relatively short computational hierarchy that captures key information about the 3-D structure of nearby surfaces, and more generally demonstrates an analysis approach that may be of merit in a diverse range of brain imaging domains.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Orientación , Análisis de Varianza , Encéfalo/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicofísica , Análisis de Regresión , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Estudiantes , Universidades , Disparidad Visual , Vías Visuales
9.
J Neurosci ; 35(7): 3056-72, 2015 Feb 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25698743

RESUMEN

The binocular disparity between the views of the world registered by the left and right eyes provides a powerful signal about the depth structure of the environment. Despite increasing knowledge of the cortical areas that process disparity from animal models, comparatively little is known about the local architecture of stereoscopic processing in the human brain. Here, we take advantage of the high spatial specificity and image contrast offered by 7 tesla fMRI to test for systematic organization of disparity representations in the human brain. Participants viewed random dot stereogram stimuli depicting different depth positions while we recorded fMRI responses from dorsomedial visual cortex. We repeated measurements across three separate imaging sessions. Using a series of computational modeling approaches, we report three main advances in understanding disparity organization in the human brain. First, we show that disparity preferences are clustered and that this organization persists across imaging sessions, particularly in area V3A. Second, we observe differences between the local distribution of voxel responses in early and dorsomedial visual areas, suggesting different cortical organization. Third, using modeling of voxel responses, we show that higher dorsal areas (V3A, V3B/KO) have properties that are characteristic of human depth judgments: a simple model that uses tuning parameters estimated from fMRI data captures known variations in human psychophysical performance. Together, these findings indicate that human dorsal visual cortex contains selective cortical structures for disparity that may support the neural computations that underlie depth perception.


Asunto(s)
Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Corteza Visual/irrigación sanguínea , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa , Probabilidad
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 115(6): 2779-90, 2016 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26912596

RESUMEN

The visual impression of an object's surface reflectance ("gloss") relies on a range of visual cues, both monocular and binocular. Whereas previous imaging work has identified processing within ventral visual areas as important for monocular cues, little is known about cortical areas involved in processing binocular cues. Here, we used human functional MRI (fMRI) to test for brain areas selectively involved in the processing of binocular cues. We manipulated stereoscopic information to create four conditions that differed in their disparity structure and in the impression of surface gloss that they evoked. We performed multivoxel pattern analysis to find areas whose fMRI responses allow classes of stimuli to be distinguished based on their depth structure vs. material appearance. We show that higher dorsal areas play a role in processing binocular gloss information, in addition to known ventral areas involved in material processing, with ventral area lateral occipital responding to both object shape and surface material properties. Moreover, we tested for similarities between the representation of gloss from binocular cues and monocular cues. Specifically, we tested for transfer in the decoding performance of an algorithm trained on glossy vs. matte objects defined by either binocular or by monocular cues. We found transfer effects from monocular to binocular cues in dorsal visual area V3B/kinetic occipital (KO), suggesting a shared representation of the two cues in this area. These results indicate the involvement of mid- to high-level visual circuitry in the estimation of surface material properties, with V3B/KO potentially playing a role in integrating monocular and binocular cues.


Asunto(s)
Visión Binocular/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Algoritmos , Mapeo Encefálico , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Visual/diagnóstico por imagen , Vías Visuales/diagnóstico por imagen , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
11.
J Neurosci ; 33(43): 16992-7007, 2013 Oct 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24155304

RESUMEN

Occlusion is a primary challenge facing the visual system in perceiving object shapes in intricate natural scenes. Although behavior, neurophysiological, and modeling studies have shown that occluded portions of objects may be completed at the early stage of visual processing, we have little knowledge on how and where in the human brain the completion is realized. Here, we provide functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) evidence that the occluded portion of an object is indeed represented topographically in human V1 and V2. Specifically, we find the topographic cortical responses corresponding to the invisible object rotation in V1 and V2. Furthermore, by investigating neural responses for the occluded target rotation within precisely defined cortical subregions, we could dissociate the topographic neural representation of the occluded portion from other types of neural processing such as object edge processing. We further demonstrate that the early topographic representation in V1 can be modulated by prior knowledge of a whole appearance of an object obtained before partial occlusion. These findings suggest that primary "visual" area V1 has the ability to process not only visible or virtually (illusorily) perceived objects but also "invisible" portions of objects without concurrent visual sensation such as luminance enhancement to these portions. The results also suggest that low-level image features and higher preceding cognitive context are integrated into a unified topographic representation of occluded portion in early areas.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Corteza Visual/anatomía & histología
12.
Mol Ther ; 21(3): 707-14, 2013 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23319060

RESUMEN

We here report the results of a Phase I/IIa open-label four dose-escalation clinical study assessing the safety, tolerability, and possible therapeutic efficacy of a single intramuscular administration of DVC1-0101, a new gene transfer vector based on a nontransmissible recombinant Sendai virus (rSeV) expressing the human fibroblast growth factor-2 (FGF-2) gene (rSeV/dF-hFGF2), in patients with peripheral arterial disease (PAD). Gene transfer was done in 12 limbs of 12 patients with rest pain, and three of them had ischemic ulcer(s). No cardiovascular or other serious adverse events (SAEs) caused by gene transfer were detected in the patients over a 6-month follow-up. No infectious viral particles, as assessed by hemagglutination activity, were detected in any patient during the study. No representative elevation of proinflammatory cytokines or plasma FGF-2 was seen. Significant and continuous improvements in Rutherford category, absolute claudication distance (ACD), and rest pain were observed (P < 0.05 to 0.01). To the best of our knowledge, this is the first clinical trial of the use of a gene transfer vector based on rSeV. The single intramuscular administration of DVC1-0101 to PAD patients was safe and well tolerated, and resulted in significant improvements of limb function. Larger pivotal studies are warranted as a next step.


Asunto(s)
Factor 2 de Crecimiento de Fibroblastos/genética , Terapia Genética/métodos , Enfermedad Arterial Periférica/terapia , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Citocinas/metabolismo , Femenino , Técnicas de Transferencia de Gen , Vectores Genéticos , Humanos , Inyecciones Intramusculares , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Enfermedad Arterial Periférica/genética , Virus Sendai/genética , Resultado del Tratamiento
13.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 108(34): 14234-9, 2011 Aug 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21821793

RESUMEN

After the first report of induced pluripotent stem cells (iPSCs), considerable efforts have been made to develop more efficient methods for generating iPSCs without foreign gene insertions. Here we show that Sendai virus vector, an RNA virus vector that carries no risk of integrating into the host genome, is a practical solution for the efficient generation of safer iPSCs. We improved the Sendai virus vectors by introducing temperature-sensitive mutations so that the vectors could be easily removed at nonpermissive temperatures. Using these vectors enabled the efficient production of viral/factor-free iPSCs from both human fibroblasts and CD34(+) cord blood cells. Temperature-shift treatment was more effective in eliminating remaining viral vector-related genes. The resulting iPSCs expressed human embryonic stem cell markers and exhibited pluripotency. We suggest that generation of transgene-free iPSCs from cord blood cells should be an important step in providing allogeneic iPSC-derived therapy in the future.


Asunto(s)
Vectores Genéticos/genética , Células Madre Pluripotentes Inducidas/metabolismo , Virus Sendai/genética , Temperatura , Transgenes/genética , Animales , Biomarcadores/metabolismo , Diferenciación Celular , Células Madre Embrionarias/citología , Células Madre Embrionarias/metabolismo , Epigénesis Genética , Sangre Fetal/citología , Fibroblastos/metabolismo , Estratos Germinativos/citología , Estratos Germinativos/metabolismo , Humanos , Células Madre Pluripotentes Inducidas/citología , Ratones
14.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(9): 1527-41, 2013 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23647559

RESUMEN

The visual system's flexibility in estimating depth is remarkable: We readily perceive 3-D structure under diverse conditions from the seemingly random dots of a "magic eye" stereogram to the aesthetically beautiful, but obviously flat, canvasses of the Old Masters. Yet, 3-D perception is often enhanced when different cues specify the same depth. This perceptual process is understood as Bayesian inference that improves sensory estimates. Despite considerable behavioral support for this theory, insights into the cortical circuits involved are limited. Moreover, extant work tested quantitatively similar cues, reducing some of the challenges associated with integrating computationally and qualitatively different signals. Here we address this challenge by measuring fMRI responses to depth structures defined by shading, binocular disparity, and their combination. We quantified information about depth configurations (convex "bumps" vs. concave "dimples") in different visual cortical areas using pattern classification analysis. We found that fMRI responses in dorsal visual area V3B/KO were more discriminable when disparity and shading concurrently signaled depth, in line with the predictions of cue integration. Importantly, by relating fMRI and psychophysical tests of integration, we observed a close association between depth judgments and activity in this area. Finally, using a cross-cue transfer test, we found that fMRI responses evoked by one cue afford classification of responses evoked by the other. This reveals a generalized depth representation in dorsal visual cortex that combines qualitatively different information in line with 3-D perception.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Percepción de Profundidad/fisiología , Adulto , Algoritmos , Encéfalo/irrigación sanguínea , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Oxígeno/sangre , Estimulación Luminosa , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Probabilidad , Psicofísica , Adulto Joven
15.
J Neurophysiol ; 110(1): 190-203, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23576705

RESUMEN

Reliable estimation of three-dimensional (3D) surface orientation is critical for recognizing and interacting with complex 3D objects in our environment. Human observers maximize the reliability of their estimates of surface slant by integrating multiple depth cues. Texture and binocular disparity are two such cues, but they are qualitatively very different. Existing evidence suggests that representations of surface tilt from each of these cues coincide at the single-neuron level in higher cortical areas. However, the cortical circuits responsible for 1) integration of such qualitatively distinct cues and 2) encoding the slant component of surface orientation have not been assessed. We tested for cortical responses related to slanted plane stimuli that were defined independently by texture, disparity, and combinations of these two cues. We analyzed the discriminability of functional MRI responses to two slant angles using multivariate pattern classification. Responses in visual area V3B/KO to stimuli containing congruent cues were more discriminable than those elicited by single cues, in line with predictions based on the fusion of slant estimates from component cues. This improvement was specific to congruent combinations of cues: incongruent cues yielded lower decoding accuracies, which suggests the robust use of individual cues in cases of large cue conflicts. These data suggest that area V3B/KO is intricately involved in the integration of qualitatively dissimilar depth cues.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Disparidad Visual/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Propiedades de Superficie , Adulto Joven
16.
J Vis ; 13(6): 20, 2013 May 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23729771

RESUMEN

In almost all of the recent vision experiments, stimuli are controlled via computers and presented on display devices such as cathode ray tubes (CRTs). Display characterization is a necessary procedure for such computer-aided vision experiments. The standard display characterization called "gamma correction" and the following linear color transformation procedure are established for CRT displays and widely used in the current vision science field. However, the standard two-step procedure is based on the internal model of CRT display devices, and there is no guarantee as to whether the method is applicable to the other types of display devices such as liquid crystal display and digital light processing. We therefore tested the applicability of the standard method to these kinds of new devices and found that the standard method was not valid for these new devices. To overcome this problem, we provide several novel approaches for vision experiments to characterize display devices, based on linear, nonlinear, and hybrid search algorithms. These approaches never assume any internal models of display devices and will therefore be applicable to any display type. The evaluations and comparisons of chromaticity estimation accuracies based on these new methods with those of the standard procedure proved that our proposed methods largely improved the calibration efficiencies for non-CRT devices. Our proposed methods, together with the standard one, have been implemented in a MATLAB-based integrated graphical user interface software named Mcalibrator2. This software can enhance the accuracy of vision experiments and enable more efficient display characterization procedures. The software is now available publicly for free.


Asunto(s)
Algoritmos , Color , Sistemas de Computación , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Interfaz Usuario-Computador , Psicofísica
17.
Brain Nerve ; 73(11): 1231-1236, 2021 Nov.
Artículo en Japonés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34759060

RESUMEN

Humans can immediately perceive a rich and stable 3D visual world even though the visual inputs on the retina are 2D. Which raises the question; which cortical regions are responsible for reconstructing stereoscopic perception? In this article, I would like to outline the general research methods of stereopsis and introduce our recent research projects that show that the middle-or higher-order visual areas, V3A and V3B/KO, located adjacent to the interparietal sulcus, play an important role in the reconstruction of the 3D visual world.


Asunto(s)
Corteza Visual , Mapeo Encefálico , Percepción de Profundidad , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa
18.
eNeuro ; 2021 Jun 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34140352

RESUMEN

Depth sensitivity has been shown to be modulated by object context (plausibility). It is possible that it is behavioural relevance rather than object plausibility per se which drives this effect. Here, we manipulated the biological relevance of objects (face or a non-face) and tested whether object relevance affects behavioural sensitivity and neural responses to depth-position. In a first experiment, we presented human observers with disparity-defined faces and non-faces, and observers were asked to judge the depth position of the target under signal-noise and clear (fine) task conditions. In the second experiment, we concurrently measured behavioural and fMRI responses to depth. We found that behavioural performance varied across stimulus conditions such that they were significantly worse for the upright face than the inverted face and the random shape in the SNR task, but worse for the random shape than the upright face in the feature task. Pattern analysis of fMRI responses revealed that activity of FFA was distinctly different during depth judgments of the upright face versus the other two stimuli, with its responses (and to a stronger extent, those of V3) appearing functionally-relevant to behavioural performance. We speculate that FFA is not only involved in object analysis, but exerts considerable influence on stereoscopic mechanisms as early as in V3 based on a broader appreciation of the stimulus' behavioural relevance.Significance StatementWe asked how disparity sensitivity is modulated by object (biological) relevance using behavioural and neuroimaging paradigms. We show that behavioural sensitivity to depth-position changes in biological (face) vs non-biological (random surface) contexts, and that these changes are task-dependent. Imaging results highlight a potentially key role of the fusiform region for governing the modulation of stereo encoding by object relevance. These findings highlight powerful interactions between object recognition mechanisms and stereoencoding, such that the utility of disparity information may be up/down weighed depending on the biological relevance of the object.

19.
Cortex ; 136: 124-139, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33545617

RESUMEN

We sought to understand the spatiotemporal characteristics of biological motion perception. We presented observers with biological motion walkers that differed in terms of form coherence or kinematics (i.e., the presence or absence of natural acceleration). Participants were asked to discriminate the facing direction of the stimuli while their magnetoencephalographic responses were concurrently imaged. We found that two univariate response components can be observed around ~200 msec and ~650 msec post-stimulus onset, each engaging lateral-occipital and parietal cortex prior to temporal and frontal cortex. Moreover, while univariate responses show biological motion form-specificity only after 300 msec, multivariate patterns specific to form can be well discriminated from those for local cues as early as 100 msec after stimulus onset. By finally examining the representational similarity of fMRI and MEG patterned responses, we show that early responses to biological motion are most likely sourced to occipital cortex while later responses likely originate from extrastriate body areas.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Movimiento , Corteza Visual , Mapeo Encefálico , Humanos , Magnetoencefalografía , Lóbulo Occipital , Estimulación Luminosa
20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19838014

RESUMEN

Induced pluripotent stem cells (iPSC) have been generated from somatic cells by introducing reprogramming factors. Integration of foreign genes into the host genome is a technical hurdle for the clinical application. Here, we show that Sendai virus (SeV), an RNA virus and carries no risk of altering host genome, is an efficient solution for generating safe iPSC. Sendai-viral human iPSC expressed pluripotency genes, showed demethylation characteristic of reprogrammed cells. SeV-derived transgenes were decreased during cell division. Moreover, viruses were able to be easily removed by antibody-mediated negative selection utilizing cell surface marker HN that is expressed on SeV-infected cells. Viral-free iPSC differentiated to mature cells of the three embryonic germ layers in vivo and in vitro including beating cardiomyocytes, neurons, bone and pancreatic cells. Our data demonstrated that highly-efficient, non-integrating SeV-based vector system provides a critical solution for reprogramming somatic cells and will accelerate the clinical application.


Asunto(s)
Reprogramación Celular/genética , Vectores Genéticos/genética , Genoma Humano/genética , Células Madre Pluripotentes/citología , Células Madre Pluripotentes/metabolismo , Virus Sendai/genética , Transgenes/genética , Biomarcadores/metabolismo , Línea Celular Tumoral , Proliferación Celular , Reprogramación Celular/fisiología , Metilación de ADN , Fibroblastos/metabolismo , Perfilación de la Expresión Génica , Vectores Genéticos/aislamiento & purificación , Humanos , Virus Sendai/aislamiento & purificación , Transducción Genética , Integración Viral
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA