Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 55
Filtrar
Más filtros

Bases de datos
Tipo del documento
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Neurosci ; 41(14): 3234-3253, 2021 04 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33622775

RESUMEN

Popular models of decision-making propose that noisy sensory evidence accumulates until reaching a bound. Behavioral evidence as well as trial-averaged ramping of neuronal activity in sensorimotor regions of the brain support this idea. However, averaging activity across trials can mask other processes, such as rapid shifts in decision commitment, calling into question the hypothesis that evidence accumulation is encoded by delay period activity of individual neurons. We mined two sets of data from experiments in four monkeys in which we recorded from superior colliculus neurons during two different decision-making tasks and a delayed saccade task. We applied second-order statistical measures and spike train simulations to determine whether spiking statistics were similar or different in the different tasks and monkeys, despite similar trial-averaged activity across tasks and monkeys. During a motion direction discrimination task, single-trial delay period activity behaved statistically consistent with accumulation. During an orientation detection task, the activity behaved superficially like accumulation, but statistically consistent with stepping. Simulations confirmed both findings. Importantly, during a simple saccade task, with similar trial-averaged activity, neither process explained spiking activity, ruling out interpretations based on differences in attention, reward, or motor planning. These results highlight the need for exploring single-trial spiking dynamics to understand cognitive processing and raise the interesting hypothesis that the superior colliculus participates in different aspects of decision-making depending on task differences.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT How are decisions based on sensory information transformed into actions? We report that single-trial neuronal activity dynamics in the superior colliculus of monkeys show differences in decision-making tasks depending on task idiosyncrasies and requirements and despite similar trial-averaged ramping activity. These results highlight the importance of exploring single-trial spiking dynamics to understand cognitive processing and raise the interesting hypothesis that the superior colliculus participates in different aspects of decision-making depending on task requirements.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología
2.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 17(3): e1008779, 2021 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33780449

RESUMEN

Current dominant views hold that perceptual confidence reflects the probability that a decision is correct. Although these views have enjoyed some empirical support, recent behavioral results indicate that confidence and the probability of being correct can be dissociated. An alternative hypothesis suggests that confidence instead reflects the magnitude of evidence in favor of a decision while being relatively insensitive to the evidence opposing the decision. We considered how this alternative hypothesis might be biologically instantiated by developing a simple neural network model incorporating a known property of sensory neurons: tuned inhibition. The key idea of the model is that the level of inhibition that each accumulator unit receives from units with the opposite tuning preference, i.e. its inhibition 'tuning', dictates its contribution to perceptual decisions versus confidence judgments, such that units with higher tuned inhibition (computing relative evidence for different perceptual interpretations) determine perceptual discrimination decisions, and units with lower tuned inhibition (computing absolute evidence) determine confidence. We demonstrate that this biologically plausible model can account for several counterintuitive findings reported in the literature where confidence and decision accuracy dissociate. By comparing model fits, we further demonstrate that a full complement of behavioral data across several previously published experimental results-including accuracy, reaction time, mean confidence, and metacognitive sensitivity-is best accounted for when confidence is computed from units without, rather than units with, tuned inhibition. Finally, we discuss predictions of our results and model for future neurobiological studies. These findings suggest that the brain has developed and implements this alternative, heuristic theory of perceptual confidence computation by relying on the diversity of neural resources available.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Animales , Biología Computacional , Inhibición Psicológica , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Percepción/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología
3.
Neuroimage ; 229: 117700, 2021 04 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33418072

RESUMEN

Scientific excellence is a necessity for progress in biomedical research. As research becomes ever more international, establishing international collaborations will be key to advancing our scientific knowledge. Understanding the similarities in standards applied by different nations to animal research, and where the differences might lie, is crucial. Cultural differences and societal values will also contribute to these similarities and differences between countries and continents. Our overview is not comprehensive for all species, but rather focuses on non-human primate (NHP) research, involving New World marmosets and Old World macaques, conducted in countries where NHPs are involved in neuroimaging research. Here, an overview of the ethics and regulations is provided to help assess welfare standards amongst primate research institutions. A comparative examination of these standards was conducted to provide a basis for establishing a common set of standards for animal welfare. These criteria may serve to develop international guidelines, which can be managed by an International Animal Welfare and Use Committee (IAWUC). Internationally, scientists have a moral responsibility to ensure excellent care and welfare of their animals, which in turn, influences the quality of their research. When working with animal models, maintaining a high quality of care ("culture of care") and welfare is essential. The transparent promotion of this level of care and welfare, along with the results of the research and its impact, may reduce public concerns associated with animal experiments in neuroscience research.


Asunto(s)
Acceso a la Información/ética , Bienestar del Animal/ética , Investigación Biomédica/ética , Internacionalidad , Neurociencias/ética , Bienestar del Animal/legislación & jurisprudencia , Animales , Investigación Biomédica/legislación & jurisprudencia , Miembro de Comité , Humanos , Neurociencias/legislación & jurisprudencia , Primates
4.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(7): E1588-E1597, 2018 02 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29382765

RESUMEN

Recent studies suggest that neurons in sensorimotor circuits involved in perceptual decision-making also play a role in decision confidence. In these studies, confidence is often considered to be an optimal readout of the probability that a decision is correct. However, the information leading to decision accuracy and the report of confidence often covaried, leaving open the possibility that there are actually two dissociable signal types in the brain: signals that correlate with decision accuracy (optimal confidence) and signals that correlate with subjects' behavioral reports of confidence (subjective confidence). We recorded neuronal activity from a sensorimotor decision area, the superior colliculus (SC) of monkeys, while they performed two different tasks. In our first task, decision accuracy and confidence covaried, as in previous studies. In our second task, we implemented a motion discrimination task with stimuli that were matched for decision accuracy but produced different levels of confidence, as reflected by behavioral reports. We used a multivariate decoder to predict monkeys' choices from neuronal population activity. As in previous studies on perceptual decision-making mechanisms, we found that neuronal decoding performance increased as decision accuracy increased. However, when decision accuracy was matched, performance of the decoder was similar between high and low subjective confidence conditions. These results show that the SC likely signals optimal decision confidence similar to previously reported cortical mechanisms, but is unlikely to play a critical role in subjective confidence. The results also motivate future investigations to determine where in the brain signals related to subjective confidence reside.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Neuronas/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Animales , Toma de Decisiones , Macaca mulatta , Masculino
6.
J Neurophysiol ; 119(3): 849-861, 2018 03 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29167328

RESUMEN

Both cognitive and motor symptoms in people with Parkinson's disease (PD) arise from either too little or too much dopamine (DA). Akinesia stems from DA neuronal cell loss, and dyskinesia often stems from an overdose of DA medication. Cognitive behaviors typically associated with frontal cortical function, such as working memory and task switching, are also affected by too little or too much DA in PD. Whether motor and cognitive circuits overlap in PD is unknown. In this article, we show that whereas motor performance improves in people with PD when on dopaminergic medication compared with off medication, perceptual decision-making based on previously learned information (priors) remains impaired whether on or off medications. To rule out effects of long-term DA treatment and dopaminergic neuronal loss such as occur in PD, we also tested a group of people with dopa-unresponsive focal dystonia, a disease that involves the basal ganglia, like PD, but has motor symptoms that are insensitive to dopamine treatment and is not thought to involve frontal cortical DA circuits, unlike PD. We found that people with focal dystonia showed intact perceptual decision-making performance but impaired use of priors in perceptual decision-making, similar to people with PD. Together, the results show a dissociation between motor and cognitive performance in people with PD and reveal a novel cognitive impairment, independent of sensory and motor impairment, in people with focal dystonia. The combined results from people with PD and people with focal dystonia provide mechanistic insights into the role of basal ganglia non-dopaminergic circuits in perceptual decision-making based on priors.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones/fisiología , Dopamina/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Enfermedad de Parkinson/fisiopatología , Enfermedad de Parkinson/psicología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Trastornos Distónicos/fisiopatología , Trastornos Distónicos/psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 120(5): 2614-2629, 2018 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30183470

RESUMEN

Recent findings indicate that monkeys can report their confidence in perceptual decisions and that this information is encoded in neurons involved in making decisions, including the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and the supplementary eye field (SEF). A key issue to consider when studying confidence is that decision accuracy often correlates with confidence reports; when we are performing well, we generally feel more confident. Expanding on work performed in humans, we designed a novel task for monkeys that dissociates perceptual information leading to decisions from perceptual information leading to confidence reports. Using this task, we recently showed that decoded ensemble activity recorded from the superior colliculus (SC) reflected decisions rather than confidence reports. However, our previous population level analysis collapsed over multiple SC neuronal types and therefore left open the possibility that first, individual discharge rates might encode information related to decision confidence, and second, different neuronal cell types within the SC might signal decision confidence independently of decision accuracy. We found that when decision accuracy and decision confidence covaried, modulation occurred primarily in neurons with prelude activity (buildup neurons). However, isolating decision confidence from decision accuracy uncovered that only a few, primarily buildup neurons showed signals correlating uniquely with decision confidence and the effect sizes were very small. Based on this work and our previous work using decoding methods, we conclude that neuronal signals for decision confidence, independent of decision accuracy, are unlikely to exist at the level of single or populations of neurons in the SC. Our results together with other recent work call into question normative models of confidence based on the optimal readout of decision signals. NEW & NOTEWORTHY Models of decision confidence suggest that our sense of confidence is an optimal readout of perceptual decision signals. Here, we report that a subcortical area, the superior colliculus (SC), contains neurons with activity that signal decisions and confidence in a task in which decision accuracy and confidence covary, similar to area lateral intraparietal area in cortex. The signals from SC occur primarily in the neurons with prelude activity (buildup neurons). However, in a task that dissociates decision accuracy from decision confidence, we find that only a few individual neurons express unique signals of confidence. These results call into question normative models of confidence based on optimal readout of perceptual decision signals.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Neuronas/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Masculino , Percepción , Colículos Superiores/citología
8.
J Neurophysiol ; 116(1): 191-200, 2016 07 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27075543

RESUMEN

The mammalian superior colliculus (SC) is a midbrain structure that integrates multimodal sensory inputs and computes commands to initiate rapid eye movements. SC neurons burst with the sudden onset of a visual stimulus, followed by persistent activity that may underlie shifts of attention and decision making. Experiments in vitro suggest that circuit reverberations play a role in the burst activity in the SC, but the origin of persistent activity is unclear. In the present study we characterized an afterdepolarization (ADP) that follows action potentials in slices of rat SC. Population responses seen with voltage-sensitive dye imaging consisted of rapid spikes followed immediately by a second distinct depolarization of lower amplitude and longer duration. Patch-clamp recordings showed qualitatively similar behavior: in nearly all neurons throughout the SC, rapid spikes were followed by an ADP. Ionic and pharmacological manipulations along with experiments with current and voltage steps indicated that the ADP of SC neurons arises from Na(+) current that either persists or resurges following Na(+) channel inactivation at the end of an action potential. Comparisons of pharmacological properties and frequency dependence revealed a clear parallel between patch-clamp recordings and voltage imaging experiments, indicating a common underlying membrane mechanism for the ADP in both single neurons and populations. The ADP can initiate repetitive spiking at intervals consistent with the frequency of persistent activity in the SC. These results indicate that SC neurons have intrinsic membrane properties that can contribute to electrical activity that underlies shifts of attention and decision making.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de la Membrana/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Sodio/metabolismo , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Animales , Potenciales de la Membrana/efectos de los fármacos , Neuronas/efectos de los fármacos , Técnicas de Placa-Clamp , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Bloqueadores de los Canales de Sodio/farmacología , Canales de Sodio/metabolismo , Colículos Superiores/efectos de los fármacos , Técnicas de Cultivo de Tejidos , Imagen de Colorante Sensible al Voltaje
9.
J Neurosci ; 34(23): 7976-87, 2014 Jun 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24899719

RESUMEN

How does the brain decide where to look? Neuronal networks within the superior colliculus (SC) encode locations of intended eye movements. When faced with multiple targets, the relative activities of neuronal populations compete for the selection of a saccade. However, the computational principles underlying saccadic choices remain poorly understood. We used voltage imaging of slices of rat SC to record circuit dynamics of population responses to single- and dual-site electrical stimulation to begin to reveal some of the principles of how populations of neurons interact. Stimulation of two distant sites simultaneously within the SC produced two distinct peaks of activity, whereas stimulation of two nearby sites simultaneously exhibited a single, merged peak centered between the two sites. The distances required to produce merged peaks of activity corresponded to target separations that evoked averaging saccades in humans performing a corresponding dual target task. The merged activity was well accounted for by a linear weighed summation and a divisive normalization of the responses evoked by the single-site stimulations. Interestingly, the merging of activity occurred within the superficial SC, suggesting a novel pathway for saccadic eye movement choice.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales de Acción/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos , Colículos Superiores/citología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Animales , Estimulación Eléctrica , Técnicas In Vitro , Técnicas de Placa-Clamp , Estimulación Luminosa , Ratas
10.
J Neurosci ; 34(20): 6822-33, 2014 May 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24828636

RESUMEN

Neural circuits that translate sensory information into motor commands are organized in a feedforward manner converting sensory information into motor output. The superior colliculus (SC) follows this pattern as it plays a role in converting visual information from the retina and visual cortex into motor commands for rapid eye movements (saccades). Feedback from movement to sensory regions is hypothesized to play critical roles in attention, visual image stability, and saccadic suppression, but in contrast to feedforward pathways, motor feedback to sensory regions has received much less attention. The present study used voltage imaging and patch-clamp recording in slices of rat SC to test the hypothesis of an excitatory synaptic pathway from the motor layers of the SC back to the sensory superficial layers. Voltage imaging revealed an extensive depolarization of the superficial layers evoked by electrical stimulation of the motor layers. A pharmacologically isolated excitatory synaptic potential in the superficial layers depended on stimulus strength in the motor layers in a manner consistent with orthodromic excitation. Patch-clamp recording from neurons in the sensory layers revealed excitatory synaptic potentials in response to glutamate application in the motor layers. The location, size, and morphology of responsive neurons indicated they were likely to be narrow-field vertical cells. This excitatory projection from motor to sensory layers adds an important element to the circuitry of the SC and reveals a novel feedback pathway that could play a role in enhancing sensory responses to attended targets as well as visual image stabilization.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Postsinápticos Excitadores/fisiología , Retroalimentación Fisiológica/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Sinapsis/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Ratas , Ratas Sprague-Dawley
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 114(6): 3039-49, 2015 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26378203

RESUMEN

A long-standing question in systems neuroscience is how the activity of single neurons gives rise to our perceptions and actions. Critical insights into this question occurred in the last part of the 20th century when scientists began linking modulations of neuronal activity directly to perceptual behavior. A significant conceptual advance was the application of signal detection theory to both neuronal activity and behavior, providing a quantitative assessment of the relationship between brain and behavior. One metric that emerged from these efforts was choice probability (CP), which provides information about how well an ideal observer can predict the choice an animal makes from a neuron's discharge rate distribution. In this review, we describe where CP has been studied, locational trends in the values found, and why CP values are typically so low. We discuss its dependence on correlated activity among neurons of a population, assess whether it arises from feedforward or feedback mechanisms, and investigate what CP tells us about how many neurons are required for a decision and how they are pooled to do so.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Conducta de Elección , Modelos Neurológicos , Animales , Retroalimentación Fisiológica , Humanos , Probabilidad
12.
J Neurophysiol ; 114(1): 662-76, 2015 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25995346

RESUMEN

The mammalian superior colliculus (SC) is a laminar midbrain structure that translates visual signals into commands to shift the focus of attention and gaze. The SC plays an integral role in selecting targets and ultimately generating rapid eye movements to those targets. In all mammals studied to date, neurons in the SC are arranged topographically such that the location of visual stimuli and the endpoints of orienting movements form organized maps in superficial and deeper layers, respectively. The organization of these maps is thought to underlie attentional priority by assessing which regions of the visual field contain behaviorally relevant information. Using voltage imaging and patch-clamp recordings in parasagittal SC slices from the rat, we found the synaptic circuitry of the visuosensory map in the SC imposes a strong bias. Voltage imaging of responses to electrical stimulation revealed more spread in the caudal direction than the rostral direction. Pharmacological experiments demonstrated that this asymmetry arises from GABAA receptor activation rostral to the site of stimulation. Patch-clamp recordings confirmed this rostrally directed inhibitory circuit and showed that it is contained within the visuosensory layers of the SC. Stimulation of two sites showed that initial stimulation of a caudal site can take priority over subsequent stimulation of a rostral site. Taken together, our data indicate that the circuitry of the visuosensory SC is hard-wired to give higher priority to more peripheral targets, and this property is conferred by a uniquely structured, dedicated inhibitory circuit.


Asunto(s)
Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología , Animales , Bencilaminas/farmacología , Mapeo Encefálico , Estimulación Eléctrica , Fármacos actuantes sobre Aminoácidos Excitadores/farmacología , Antagonistas del GABA/farmacología , Ácido Glutámico/farmacología , Potenciales de la Membrana/efectos de los fármacos , Potenciales de la Membrana/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/efectos de los fármacos , Técnicas de Placa-Clamp , Ácidos Fosfínicos/farmacología , Piridazinas/farmacología , Ratas Sprague-Dawley , Receptores de GABA-A/metabolismo , Receptores de GABA-B/metabolismo , Colículos Superiores/efectos de los fármacos , Técnicas de Cultivo de Tejidos , Imagen de Colorante Sensible al Voltaje
13.
J Neurophysiol ; 111(4): 804-16, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24259551

RESUMEN

In the absence of sensory information, we rely on past experience or memories to guide our actions. Because previous experimental and clinical reports implicate basal ganglia nuclei in the generation of movement in the absence of sensory stimuli, we ask here whether one output nucleus of the basal ganglia, the substantia nigra pars reticulata (nigra), influences the specification of an eye movement in the absence of sensory information to guide the movement. We manipulated the level of activity of neurons in the nigra by introducing electrical stimulation to the nigra at different time intervals while monkeys made saccades to different locations in two conditions: one in which the target location remained visible and a second in which the target location appeared only briefly, requiring information stored in memory to specify the movement. Electrical manipulation of the nigra occurring during the delay period of the task, when information about the target was maintained in memory, altered the direction and the occurrence of subsequent saccades. Stimulation during other intervals of the memory task or during the delay period of the visually guided saccade task had less effect on eye movements. On stimulated trials, and only when the visual stimulus was absent, monkeys occasionally (∼20% of the time) failed to make saccades. When monkeys made saccades in the absence of a visual stimulus, stimulation of the nigra resulted in a rotation of the endpoints ipsilaterally (∼2°) and increased the reaction time of contralaterally directed saccades. When the visual stimulus was present, stimulation of the nigra resulted in no significant rotation and decreased the reaction time of contralaterally directed saccades slightly. Based on these measurements, stimulation during the delay period of the memory-guided saccade task influenced the metrics of saccades much more than did stimulation during the same period of the visually guided saccade task. Because these effects occurred with manipulation of nigral activity well before the initiation of saccades and in trials in which the visual stimulus was absent, we conclude that information from the basal ganglia influences the specification of an action as it is evolving primarily during performance of memory-guided saccades. When visual information is available to guide the specification of the saccade, as occurs during visually guided saccades, basal ganglia information is less influential.


Asunto(s)
Memoria , Movimientos Sacádicos , Sustancia Negra/fisiología , Animales , Macaca mulatta , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Sustancia Negra/citología , Factores de Tiempo
14.
15.
bioRxiv ; 2024 Aug 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39131309

RESUMEN

There is a critical need to generate age- and sex-specific survival curves to characterize chronological aging consistently across nonhuman primates (NHP) used in biomedical research. Accurate measures of chronological aging are essential for inferences into genetic, demographic, and physiological variables driving differences in NHP lifespan within and between species. Understanding NHP lifespans is relevant to public health because unraveling the demographic, molecular, and clinical bases of health across the life course in translationally relevant NHP species is fundamentally important to the study of human aging. Data from more than 110,000 captive individual NHP were contributed by 15 major research institutions to generate sex-specific Kaplan-Meier survival curves using uniform methods in 12 translational aging models: Callithrix jacchus (common marmoset), Chlorocebus aethiops sabaeus (vervet/African green), Macaca fascicularis (cynomolgus macaque), M. fuscata (Japanese macaque), M. mulatta (rhesus macaque), M. nemestrina (pigtail macaque), M. radiata (bonnet macaque), Pan troglodytes spp. (chimpanzee), Papio hamadryas spp. (baboon), Plecturocebus cupreus (coppery titi monkey), Saguinus oedipus (cotton-top tamarin), and Saimiri spp. (squirrel monkey). After employing strict inclusion criteria, primary analysis results are based on 12,269 NHP that survived to adulthood and died of natural/health-related causes. A secondary analysis was completed for 32,616 NHP that died of any cause. For the primary analyses, we report ages of 25th, 50th, 75th, and 85th percentiles of survival, maximum observed ages, rates of survivorship, and sex-based differences captured by quantile regression models and Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests. Our findings show a pattern of reduced male survival among catarrhines (African and Asian primates), especially macaques, but not platyrrhines (Central and South American primates). For many species, median lifespans were lower than previously reported. An important consideration is that these analyses may offer a better reflection of healthspan than lifespan. Captive NHP used in research are typically euthanized for humane welfare reasons before their natural end of life, often after diagnosis of their first major disease requiring long-term treatment with reduced quality of life (e.g., endometriosis, cancer, osteoarthritis). Supporting the idea that these data are capturing healthspan, for several species typical age at onset of chronic disease is similar to the median lifespan estimates. This data resource represents the most comprehensive characterization of sex-specific lifespan and age-at-death distributions for 12 biomedically relevant species, to date. The results clarify the relationships among NHP ages and will provide a valuable resource for the aging research community, improving human-NHP age equivalencies, informing investigators of the expected survival rates of NHP assigned to studies, providing a metric for comparisons in future studies, and contributing to our understanding of the factors that drive lifespan differences within and among species.

16.
J Neurophysiol ; 109(3): 734-41, 2013 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23155174

RESUMEN

Memory-guided saccades are slower than visually guided saccades. The usual explanation for this slowing is that the absence of a visual drive reduces the discharge of neurons in the superior colliculus. We tested a related hypothesis: that the slowing of memory-guided saccades was due also to the more frequent occurrence of gaze-evoked blinks with memory-guided saccades compared with visually guided saccades. We recorded gaze-evoked blinks in three monkeys while they performed visually guided and memory-guided saccades and compared the kinematics of the different saccade types with and without blinks. Gaze-evoked blinks were more common during memory-guided saccades than during visually guided saccades, and the well-established relationship between peak and average velocity for saccades was disrupted by blinking. The occurrence of gaze-evoked blinks was associated with a greater slowing of memory-guided saccades compared with visually guided saccades. Likewise, when blinks were absent, the peak velocity of visually guided saccades was only slightly higher than that of memory-guided saccades. Our results reveal interactions between circuits generating saccades and blink-evoked eye movements. The interaction leads to increased curvature of saccade trajectories and a corresponding decrease in saccade velocity. Consistent with this interpretation, the amount of saccade curvature and slowing increased with gaze-evoked blink amplitude. Thus, although the absence of vision decreases the velocity of memory-guided saccades relative to visually guided saccades somewhat, the cooccurrence of gaze-evoked blinks produces the majority of slowing for memory-guided saccades.


Asunto(s)
Parpadeo/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Movimientos Sacádicos/fisiología , Potenciales de Acción , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Macaca mulatta , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Colículos Superiores/citología , Colículos Superiores/fisiología
17.
Comp Med ; 73(3): 216-228, 2023 06 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37208151

RESUMEN

Our goal in this manuscript is to advance the assessment and treatment of monkey species in neuroscience research. We hope to begin a discussion and establish baseline data on how complications are identified and treated. We surveyed the neuroscience research community working with monkeys and compiled responses to questions about investigator demographics, assessment of animal wellbeing, treatment choices, and approaches to mitigate risks associated with CNS procedures and promote monkey health and wellbeing. The majority of the respondents had worked with nonhuman primates (NHP) for over 15 y. Identification of procedure-related complications and efficacy of treatment generally rely on common behavioral indices. Treatments for localized inflammatory responses are generally successful, whereas the treatment success for meningitis or meningoencephalitis, abscesses, and hemorrhagic stroke are less successful. Behavioral signs of pain are treated successfully with NSAIDs and opioids. Our future plans are to collate treatment protocols and develop best practices that can be shared across the neuroscience community to improve treatment success rates and animal welfare and therefore science. Human protocols can be used to develop best practices, assess outcomes, and promote further refinements in treatment practices for monkeys to enhance research outcomes.


Asunto(s)
Bienestar del Animal , Humanos , Animales , Haplorrinos
18.
Hum Gene Ther ; 34(5-6): 228-246, 2023 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36719771

RESUMEN

Viral vector technologies are commonly used in neuroscience research to understand and manipulate neural circuits, but successful applications of these technologies in non-human primate models have been inconsistent. An essential component to improve these technologies is an impartial and accurate assessment of the effectiveness of different viral constructs in the primate brain. We tested a diverse array of viral vectors delivered to the brain and extraocular muscles of macaques and compared three methods for histological assessment of viral-mediated fluorescent transgene expression: epifluorescence (Epi), immunofluorescence (IF), and immunohistochemistry (IHC). Importantly, IF and IHC identified a greater number of transduced neurons compared to Epi. Furthermore, IF and IHC reliably provided enhanced visualization of transgene in most cellular compartments (i.e., dendritic, axonal, and terminal fields), whereas the degree of labeling provided by Epi was inconsistent and predominantly restricted to somas and apical dendrites. Because Epi signals are unamplified (in contrast to IF and IHC), Epi may provide a more veridical assessment for the amount of accumulated transgene and, thus, the potential to chemogenetically or optogenetically manipulate neuronal activity. The comparatively weak Epi signals suggest that the current generations of viral constructs, regardless of delivered transgene, are not optimized for primates. This reinforces an emerging viewpoint that viral vectors tailored for the primate brain are necessary for basic research and human gene therapy.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Primates , Animales , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Primates/genética , Neuronas/metabolismo , Transgenes , Expresión Génica , Vectores Genéticos/genética
19.
Sci Adv ; 9(16): eadg3918, 2023 04 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37083539

RESUMEN

Imaging large-population, single-cell fluorescent dynamics in freely behaving animals larger than mice remains a key endeavor of neuroscience. We present a large-field-of-view open-source miniature microscope (MiniLFOV) designed for large-scale (3.6 mm × 2.7 mm), cellular resolution neural imaging in freely behaving rats. It has an electrically adjustable working distance of up to 3.5 mm ± 100 µm, incorporates an absolute head orientation sensor, and weighs only 13.9 g. The MiniLFOV is capable of both deep brain and cortical imaging and has been validated in freely behaving rats by simultaneously imaging >1000 GCaMP7s-expressing neurons in the hippocampal CA1 layer and in head-fixed mice by simultaneously imaging ~2000 neurons in the dorsal cortex through a cranial window. The MiniLFOV also supports optional wire-free operation using a novel, wire-free data acquisition expansion board. We expect that this new open-source implementation of the UCLA Miniscope platform will enable researchers to address novel hypotheses concerning brain function in freely behaving animals.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Microscopía , Ratones , Ratas , Animales , Microscopía/métodos , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Encéfalo/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Cráneo , Cabeza
20.
Curr Res Neurobiol ; 4: 100079, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37397811

RESUMEN

As science and technology evolve, there is an increasing need for promotion of international scientific exchange. Collaborations, while offering substantial opportunities for scientists and benefit to society, also present challenges for those working with animal models, such as non-human primates (NHPs). Diversity in regulation of animal research is sometimes mistaken for the absence of common international welfare standards. Here, the ethical and regulatory protocols for 13 countries that have guidelines in place for biomedical research involving NHPs were assessed with a focus on neuroscience. Review of the variability and similarity in trans-national NHP welfare regulations extended to countries in Asia, Europe and North America. A tabulated resource was established to advance solution-oriented discussions and scientific collaborations across borders. Our aim is to better inform the public and other stakeholders. Through cooperative efforts to identify and analyze information with reference to evidence-based discussion, the proposed key ingredients may help to shape and support a more informed, open framework. This framework and resource can be expanded further for biomedical research in other countries.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA