Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
1.
Child Dev ; 91(1): 43-62, 2020 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30187910

RESUMEN

To study effects of the gender-packaging of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) toys, mother-child dyads (31 daughters; 30 sons; M = 5.2 years) were randomly assigned to play with a mechanical toy packaged for girls (GoldieBlox) or boys (BobbyBlox). When familiarizing themselves with the toy to prepare for play, mothers given BobbyBlox built more with toy pieces than did mothers given GoldieBlox. During dyadic play, mothers with sons built more; mothers with daughters read the toy's narrative instructions more. Children's independent play likewise varied with game packaging. Girls learned the mechanical belt-drive principle better from playing with BobbyBlox; boys learned the principle better from playing with GoldieBlox. Implications for gender-schema theories, STEM interventions, and toy marketing are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Juego e Implementos de Juego , Preescolar , Ingeniería , Femenino , Humanos , Invenciones , Masculino , Matemática , Ciencia , Factores Sexuales
2.
Arch Sex Behav ; 45(7): 1827-38, 2016 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26951493

RESUMEN

The evaluation of gender nonconformity in children was examined in two studies. In Study 1, 48 young adults evaluated the positivity of culturally popular labels for gender nonconformity, including "tomboy," "sissy," and two new labels generated in a pilot study, "mama's boy" and "brat." The "mama's boy" was described as a boy who has positive feminine traits (gentle and well-mannered) as opposed to the "sissy" who was described as having negative feminine traits (crying and easily frightened). In Study 2, 161 young adults read descriptions of gender-typical and nonconforming children, evaluating them in several domains. The label "mama's boy" was considered negative in Study 1 but an unlabeled positive nonconforming boy was rated as likable and competent in Study 2. However, participants worried about nonconforming boys, saying they would encourage them to behave differently and describing such children with derogatory sexual orientation slurs. "Tomboy" was generally considered a positive label in Study 1. In Study 2, gender nonconforming girls were considered neither likable nor dislikeable, and neither competent nor incompetent, reflecting ambivalence about girls' nonconformity. It may be that we use gender nonconformity labels as indicators of sexual orientation, even in young children. Therefore, even when an individual displays objectively positive traits, the stigma associated with homosexuality taints judgments about their nonconforming behavior.


Asunto(s)
Identidad de Género , Conducta Sexual/psicología , Estigma Social , Adolescente , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Proyectos Piloto , Adulto Joven
3.
Child Dev ; 87(2): 414-28, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26548652

RESUMEN

Gender schema theory (GST) posits that children approach opportunities perceived as gender appropriate, avoiding those deemed gender inappropriate, in turn affecting gender-differentiated career trajectories. To test the hypothesis that children's gender salience filters (GSF-tendency to attend to gender) moderate these processes, 62 preschool girls (M = 4.5 years) were given GSF measures. Two weeks later, they played a computer game about occupations that manipulated the game-character's femininity (hyperfeminized Barbie vs. less feminized Playmobil Jane). Following game play, girls' interests in feminine activities showed an interaction of game condition and GSF: High-GSF girls showed intensified feminine activity interests only with Barbie; low-GSF girls showed no change with either character. Neither GSF nor game condition affected occupational interests. Implications for GST, individual differences, and occupational interventions are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Infantil/psicología , Conducta de Elección/fisiología , Feminidad , Estereotipo , Preescolar , Femenino , Humanos , Ocupaciones
4.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 29(Pt 2): 330-46, 2011 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21592153

RESUMEN

Participants were 150 school-age boys and girls, 58 high school students, and 145 university students drawn from communities in the Southeastern United States. In this cross-sectional study, family role attitudes and expectations were examined across development. Parental work traditionality (occupational prestige and traditionality, and employed hours) predicted daughters' social role attitudes and plans for future family roles, such that daughters' envisioned families resembled that of their parents. Sons' and daughters' own attitudes about adult family roles predicted their plans to work or stay home with their future children; however, mothers' work traditionality predicted daughters' future plans over and above daughters' own attitudes. The only exception to this was in the case of university daughters, where university women's attitudes about social roles fully mediated this relationship. It may be that, as young women approach adulthood and the formation of families, they adjust their vision of their future self to match more closely their own attitudes about the caregiving role.


Asunto(s)
Identidad de Género , Juicio , Responsabilidad Parental/psicología , Rol , Mujeres Trabajadoras/psicología , Adolescente , Factores de Edad , Anticipación Psicológica , Niño , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Relaciones Padres-Hijo , Valores Sociales , Socialización , Adulto Joven
5.
Adv Child Dev Behav ; 47: 77-115, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25344994

RESUMEN

Women and girls in the United States continue to be underrepresented in STEM, particularly in engineering and technology fields. This gap has been attracting recent attention from those motivated to ensure that girls and women have access to a full range of personally satisfying careers as well as from those concerned with developing a rich talent pool to meet national workforce needs. This chapter is focused on interventions that have been designed to address this STEM gender gap. We begin by documenting the STEM gender gap and then review change mechanisms emerging from theories of gender development that may be harnessed in intervention efforts. In addition, we pro vide a taxonomy of intervention goals which we then use to organize an illustrative review of sample interventions. After commenting on some of the findings and limitations of past work, we offer suggestions for enhancing the systematic evaluation of intervention programs that include careful selection of comparison groups, a broad array of STEM outcome measures, assessment of potentially unintended consequences, and meta-analyses.


Asunto(s)
Logro , Selección de Profesión , Identidad de Género , Intención , Motivación , Adulto , Niño , Curriculum , Ingeniería/educación , Femenino , Objetivos , Humanos , Masculino , Ciencia/educación , Sexismo , Socialización , Estereotipo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA